Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Vision Res ; 188: 115-125, 2021 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34315091

ABSTRACT

Peripheral worsening in shape discrimination (SD) can be compensated by size-scaling of peripheral stimuli. However, such scaling results in production of large stimuli that occupy a vast range of eccentricities. We used six proportionally decreasing spatial scales to address this pragmatic limitation and to explore how shape discrimination varies with radius in the nasal visual field. Five participants with normal vision discriminated circles and radial frequency (RF) patterns presented nasally to the fixation point at 5°, 10°, 15° and 20°. Stimuli were scaled with the nasal cortical magnification factor (nCMF) from a central stimulus in six spatial scales, which varied from 0.125 to 1, where 1 corresponded to 1.2° radius. Thresholds expressed in Weber fractions remained constant at eccentricities up to 20° regardless of the spatial scale. Weber fractions for the smaller spatial scales (0.125-0.5) were higher and more variable than for the larger spatial scales (0.75-1), yet still constant across periphery. The results provide evidence that peripheral shape discrimination is constrained by low-level properties, such as eccentricity, and can be predicted by the cortical magnification theory. However, above the peripheral modulation resolution limits, RF shape discrimination is based on the proportion between the modulation amplitude and the radius for larger scales (0.75-1), and demonstrates peripheral scale invariance for these stimuli. For eccentric shape discrimination tests, stimuli with low spatial frequency, high contrast, and radii corresponding to SS 0.75-0.875 should be used to ensure constant Weber fractions, small variability, and peripheral stimuli that are not excessively magnified.


Subject(s)
Radius , Visual Fields , Humans , Sensory Thresholds , Visual Perception
2.
J Vis ; 13(7): 10, 2013 Jun 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23766541

ABSTRACT

We investigate whether there are second-order form and motion mechanisms in human color vision. Second-order stimuli are contrast modulations of a noise carrier. The contrast envelopes are static Gabors of different spatial frequencies (0.125-1 cycles/°) or drifting Gabors of different temporal frequencies (0.25 cycles/°, 0.5-4 Hz). Stimuli are isoluminant red-green or achromatic. Second-order form processing is measured using a simultaneous 2IFC (two-interval forced-choice) detection and orientation identification task, and direction identification is used for second-order motion processing. We find that for simple detection thresholds, chromatic performance is as good or better than achromatic performance, whereas for both motion and form tasks, chromatic performance is poorer than achromatic. Chromatic second-order form perception is very poor across all spatial and temporal frequencies measured and has a lowpass contrast modulation sensitivity function with a spatial cutoff of 1 cycle/° and temporal cutoff of 4 Hz. Chromatic second-order motion sensitivity is even poorer than for form and typically is limited to 1-2 Hz. To determine whether this residual motion processing might be based on feature tracking, we used the pedestal paradigm of Lu and Sperling (1995). We find that adding a static pedestal of the same spatial frequency as the drifting Gabor envelope, with its contrast set to 1-2 times its detection threshold, impairs motion direction performance for the chromatic stimuli but not the achromatic. This suggests that the motion of second-order chromatic stimuli is not processed by a second-order system but by a third-order, feature-tracking system, although a genuine second-order motion system exists for achromatic stimuli.


Subject(s)
Color Vision/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysics
3.
J Vis ; 10(14): 20, 2010 Dec 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21163953

ABSTRACT

The investigation of the mechanism of global motion in color vision has been limited because the processing of the first-order chromatic RDK elements, based on low-level linear motion detectors, is impaired. Here we return to this problem by using second-order elements in a global motion stimulus. Second-order RDK elements were circular contrast-modulated (CM) envelopes of a low-pass filtered noise carrier. The stimuli were achromatic or isolated L/M- or S-cone opponent mechanisms. We measured simultaneously detection and motion direction identification thresholds at 100% motion coherence and at different RDK speeds with a 2-AFC paradigm. We found that direction identification thresholds were higher than detection thresholds for both chromatic and achromatic stimuli. The gap between these thresholds was greater for the chromatic than the achromatic stimuli and motion direction thresholds for the chromatic RDK were very high or impossible to obtain. We also measured global motion performance (RDK speed of 4 deg/s) by varying the coherence of limited lifetime RDK stimuli. Global motion thresholds could only be obtained for achromatic stimuli and not for chromatic ones. Within the limits of the present stimulus conditions, we found no global motion processing of second-order chromatic stimuli.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Color Vision/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Artifacts , Humans , Lighting , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
4.
Vision Res ; 44(10): 1039-45, 2004 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15031098

ABSTRACT

While most positional acuity tasks exhibit an age-related decline in performance, the effect of ageing upon vernier acuity continues to be the subject of some debate. In the present study we employed a stimulus design that enabled the simultaneous determination of bisection and vernier acuities in 36 subjects, aged between 22 and 84 years. This approach provided a means for directly testing the hypothesis that ageing affects bisection acuity but not vernier acuity by ensuring that differences in stimulus configuration and in the subject's task were kept to an absolute minimum. Optimum thresholds increased as a function of age for both bisection and vernier tasks. Inter-subject threshold variability also increased with age. Issues surrounding the comparison of absolute vernier thresholds across different studies are discussed and two important methodological factors are identified: the precise statistical method used to estimate thresholds, and the magnitude, in angular terms, of the smallest spatial offset of the elements of the vernier stimulus which can be displayed. Comparison with previously published data indicates that the discrepancy between this study and most previous investigations with respect to the effect of age upon vernier performance can be at least partly accounted for by differences in the minimum displayable vernier offset. Vernier thresholds do increase with age. The increased variability of vernier thresholds in older subjects would appear to limit the diagnostic value of the test as a means of enabling normal ageing to be distinguished from visual loss due to pathology of the eye or visual system.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Learning/physiology , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Vision Tests/methods
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...