Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 14 de 14
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 34(2): 475-88, 2008 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18377183

ABSTRACT

The use of visual occlusion as a cue to altitude maintenance in low-altitude flight (LAF) was investigated. The extent to which the ground surface is occluded by 3-D objects varies with altitude and depends on the height, radius, and density of the objects. Participants attempted to maintain a constant altitude during simulated flight over an undulating terrain with trees of various heights, radii, and densities. As would be predicted if participants used occlusion, root-mean-square error was related to the product of tree height and tree density (Experiment 1) and to the product of tree radius and tree density (Experiment 2). This relationship was also found for simulated terrains with a more realistic mixture of tree heights (Experiment 4). The authors present a modification to an occlusion model (T. Leung & J. Malik, 1997) that can be used to approximate occlusion in the context of LAF, and they evaluate the modified model using the present LAF data. On a practical level, simulating 3-D objects is computationally expensive. The present results suggest that performance may be maintained with fewer objects if their size is increased.


Subject(s)
Aircraft , Altitude , Orientation , Psychomotor Performance , Visual Perception , Adult , Association Learning , Computer Simulation , Humans , Models, Psychological , Sensory Deprivation , User-Computer Interface
2.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 18(2): 294-301, 2001 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11205974

ABSTRACT

In order to test a model of peripheral vision, various contrast sensitivity functions (CSF's) and fundamental eccentricity constants (FEC's) [see J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 8, 1762 (1991)] were applied to real-world, wide-field (6.4 degrees-32 degrees eccentricity) images. The FEC is used to model the change in contrast sensitivity as a function of retinal eccentricity. The processed test images were tested perceptually by determining the threshold FEC for which the observers could discriminate the test images from the original image. It was expected that higher CSF sensitivity would be associated with higher FEC's; and in fact, for images processed with low-pass (variable-window stimuli) CSF's, the threshold FEC's were larger for the higher-sensitivity (pattern-detection) CSF than for the lower-sensitivity (orientation detection) CSF. When two higher-sensitivity CSF's were compared, the bandpass (constant-window stimuli) CSF resulted in essentially the same FEC threshold as did the low-pass (variable-window stimuli) CSF. The fact that the FEC compensated for complex differences in the form of the CSF suggested that the discrimination task was mediated by a limited range of spatial frequencies over which the two CSF's were similar. Image contrast was then varied in order to extend the range of spatial frequencies tested. The FEC's estimated with the lower-contrast test images were unchanged for test images obtained with the high-sensitivity, bandpass CSF but increased for test images obtained with the high-sensitivity, low-pass CSF. These results suggest that peripheral contrast sensitivity as used in the present discrimination task is based on a high-sensitivity, bandpass CSF. The peripheral-vision model validated by the present analysis has practical applications in the evaluation of wide-field simulator images as well as area-of-interest or other foveating systems.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Models, Biological , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Humans , Middle Aged , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
4.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 16(6): 520-7, 1996 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8944200

ABSTRACT

An integrated system is described which we have found useful for efficiently collecting and analyzing both static and dynamic accommodation data using the Canon Autoref R-1 refractometer. The system consists of hardware modifications and software designed to both facilitate the measurement of accommodation and to process the resulting data. Several features of the Canon R-1, which may not be evident to some users, are also described. A program written in Microsoft C/C++ running under DOS, and designed for use on a PC, is available from the authors upon request.


Subject(s)
Accommodation, Ocular , Optometry/instrumentation , Refraction, Ocular , Electronics, Medical , Humans , Monitoring, Physiologic/instrumentation , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Software , User-Computer Interface
5.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 12(10): 2367-75, 1995 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7500218

ABSTRACT

A technique is described for producing variable-resolution images whose spatial detail decreases as a function of distance from their centers. These images can be matched in some sense to the normal spatial inhomogeneities of the human visual system, as well as to various abnormalities in spatial discrimination. A set of images was generated with a series of linear distortion functions whose low-pass characteristics differed at both the center and the periphery of the image as well as across the image. A forced-choice procedure was used to determine which test images were indistinguishable from unprocessed versions of themselves. Certain of the threshold distortion functions are compared with eccentricity scaling functions that have been used by others to characterize various aspects of peripheral vision. Finally, the concept of locally band-limited spaces is discussed, and an efficient sampling technique based on the concept is described. This technique can be used to generate an image that, under certain conditions, is visually equivalent to an otherwise identical image containing significantly more information.


Subject(s)
Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Humans , Vision Disparity/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology
6.
Vision Res ; 35(4): 495-506, 1995 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7900290

ABSTRACT

Similarity ratings were obtained to determine the minimum number of Gabor components that would produce a comparison texture that appeared preattentively similar to a 64-component standard texture. All textures were chosen to be both specifiable by a relatively small number of localized spectral components and sufficiently complex to approximate natural textures. The number of component orientations in the set of comparison textures was found to be a particularly important determinant of texture discrimination in that its effect on rated similarity was largely independent of the total number of components making up the texture. Textures were also presented at 0.75 degree and 20 degrees eccentricity, with the latter magnified by a factor of either 2 or 4. The overall similarity rating did not change with either magnification, whereas the critical number of orientations, defined as the number of orientations above which rated similarity was constant, did change for the higher magnification. The latter finding is consistent with the proposition that higher-order discriminations are mediated by higher cortical areas that integrate information across the visual field. Finally, the phase-bandwidth of a set of coherent textures was also varied in order to determine whether more explicit differences in the spatial structure of stimuli might affect rated similarity. In contrast to the results for component orientation, the ratings, obtained at 0.75 degree and 20 degrees, were different even when the phase-bandwidth stimuli were magnified by a factor of 4.


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Visual Fields , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Mathematics , Rotation
7.
Am J Optom Physiol Opt ; 65(11): 880-9, 1988 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3252735

ABSTRACT

The directional sensitivity (DS) of the red (pi 5), green (pi 4), and blue (pi 1) color mechanisms was measured for both the horizontal and vertical pupil meridians, thus localizing the peak of each pi-mechanism DS function within the two-dimensional pupillary aperture. In the horizontal meridian, there were greater differences among observers in DS function peak locations than there were for any one observer. Along the vertical meridian, peak location differences within observers were similar to those found in the horizontal meridian, whereas among-observer differences were much smaller. Significant differences between horizontal and vertical DS function halfwidths were also found for at least one pi-mechanism of each observer. It is concluded that meridional variations exist in either the morphology or orientational distribution of foveal photoreceptors, and that more than one alignment mechanism is responsible for photoreceptor orientation in the human fovea.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Pupil/physiology , Accommodation, Ocular , Adult , Color Perception Tests , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Acuity , Visual Fields , Visual Perception
8.
Vision Res ; 28(10): 1145-55, 1988.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3257017

ABSTRACT

Saccaadic response preferences and latencies were measured using dual targets presented at 5, 10 and 15 deg along the horizontal meridian in either one or both visual hemifields. In the unihemifield condition, subjects exhibited a strong preference (about 95%) in favor of the target presented closer to the original fixation point. Further, the presence of the second target did not increase the response latency to the preferred target. In the bihemifield condition using spatially symmetric targets, directional preference varied across subjects from 57 to 100% (mean = 83%). There was also a significant increase in response latency as compared with responses to either single targets or dual targets presented in one hemifield. This latency increase was observed for responses in both the preferred and nonpreferred directions and was taken as evidence of mutual interaction between the two hemispheres. The response preferences could be eliminated by delaying the onset of the preferred target by about 100 msec in the unihemifield condition and by about 50 msec in the bihemifield condition. Further, the interactions between the effects of the targets on response time in the bihemifield condition were found to be asymmetrical in that a target presented in the preferred hemifield has a greater effect than a target in the nonpreferred hemifield on responses to targets presented in the counterhemifield. The results suggest an asymmetry in hemispheric interaction which has not previously been demonstrated in the visualoculomotor system.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements , Saccades , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Time Factors , Visual Fields
9.
Vision Res ; 27(8): 1291-8, 1987.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3424677

ABSTRACT

Intraocular scatter of monochromatic light was assessed in three subjects (ages 21, 38, and 43) using the equivalent veil technique. For an annular stimulus of 3 deg inner and 8 deg outer diameter, the scattered illuminance at the center averaged 1.2% of the surround and was independent of wavelength from 420 to 650 nm. These results are inconsistent with predictions of wavelength dependence inherent in recent theories of ocular scatter. We conclude that intraocular scatter is produced by particles or cellular structures substantially larger than the wavelength of light.


Subject(s)
Ocular Physiological Phenomena , Adult , Female , Humans , Light , Male , Psychophysics , Retina/physiology , Scattering, Radiation , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Spectrophotometry
11.
J Comp Neurol ; 207(1): 99-103, 1982 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7096642

ABSTRACT

It is estimated by means of electron microscopy that the optic nerve of the turtle Pseudemys scripta elegans contains 394,900 fibers of which approximately 80% are myelinated. The total fiber count agrees well with counts obtained from electron microscopic studies on other turtle species. There are, however, differences among these species in the percentage of myelinated fibers in the optic nerve. The axon diameter distribution of the myelinated fibers (excluding myelin) has a mode at 0.87 micrometer while that of the unmyelinated fibers has a mode at 0.42 micrometer. Both distributions are unimodal and are of a similar form in all areas of the nerve sampled. The total fiber count reported here agrees well with previous reports of ganglion cell counts in Pseudemys and the characteristics of the fiber distributions are comparable to those reported for nonreptilian vertebrates.


Subject(s)
Nerve Fibers/ultrastructure , Optic Nerve/anatomy & histology , Turtles/anatomy & histology , Animals , Axons/ultrastructure , Microscopy, Electron , Nerve Fibers, Myelinated/ultrastructure , Neurons/ultrastructure
12.
Doc Ophthalmol ; 53(1): 3-15, 1982 May 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7105959

ABSTRACT

Stiles--Crawford functions (SCF's) were measured for both eyes of an albino observer with a marked pendular nystagmus using an eye position monitoring system. Both eyes demonstrated directional sensitivity as revealed by their SCF's. The directional sensitivity of both eyes was less than that of either the foveal or parafoveal region of normal observers. In addition, there was a significant difference between the SCF's for the albino observer's two eyes. Our results suggest that the psychophysical SCF is predominantly the result of the orientational distribution of groups of receptors on the retina.


Subject(s)
Albinism/physiopathology , Retina/physiopathology , Adult , Electroretinography , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Eye Movements , Humans , Male , Nystagmus, Pathologic/physiopathology , Statistics as Topic
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...