Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 85
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 163: 105744, 2024 May 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38825259

ABSTRACT

Every species' brain, body and behavior is shaped by the contingencies of their evolutionary history; these exert pressures that change their developmental trajectories. There is, however, another set of contingencies that shape us and other animals: those that occur during a lifetime. In this perspective piece, we show how these two histories are intertwined by focusing on the individual. We suggest that organisms--their brains and behaviors--are not solely the developmental products of genes and neural circuitry but individual centers of action unfolding in time. To unpack this idea, we first emphasize the importance of variation and the central role of the individual in biology. We then go over "errors in time" that we often make when comparing development across species. Next, we reveal how an individual's development is a process rather than a product by presenting a set of case studies. These show developmental trajectories as emerging in the contexts of the "the actual now" and "the presence of the past". Our consideration reveals that individuals are slippery-they are never static; they are a set of on-going, creative activities. In light of this, it seems that taking individual development seriously is essential if we aspire to make meaningful comparisons of neural circuits and behavior within and across species.

2.
bioRxiv ; 2023 Dec 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38106107

ABSTRACT

Active sensing is a behavioral strategy for exploring the environment. In this study, we show that contact vocal behaviors can be an active sensing mechanism that uses sampling to gain information about the social environment, in particular, the vocal behavior of others. With a focus on the realtime vocal interactions of marmoset monkeys, we contrast active sampling to a vocal accommodation framework in which vocalizations are adjusted simply to maximize responses. We conducted simulations of a vocal accommodation and an active sampling policy and compared them with real vocal exchange data. Our findings support active sampling as the best model for marmoset monkey vocal exchanges. In some cases, the active sampling model was even able to predict the distribution of vocal durations for individuals. These results suggest a new function for primate vocal interactions in which they are used by animals to seek information from social environments.

3.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(39): e2201194119, 2022 09 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36122243

ABSTRACT

The brain continuously coordinates skeletomuscular movements with internal physiological states like arousal, but how is this coordination achieved? One possibility is that the brain simply reacts to changes in external and/or internal signals. Another possibility is that it is actively coordinating both external and internal activities. We used functional ultrasound imaging to capture a large medial section of the brain, including multiple cortical and subcortical areas, in marmoset monkeys while monitoring their spontaneous movements and cardiac activity. By analyzing the causal ordering of these different time series, we found that information flowing from the brain to movements and heart-rate fluctuations were significantly greater than in the opposite direction. The brain areas involved in this external versus internal coordination were spatially distinct, but also extensively interconnected. Temporally, the brain alternated between network states for this regulation. These findings suggest that the brain's dynamics actively and efficiently coordinate motor behavior with internal physiology.


Subject(s)
Brain , Callithrix , Movement , Animals , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Callithrix/physiology , Heart Rate , Movement/physiology
4.
Elife ; 112022 07 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35880740

ABSTRACT

Human and non-human primates produce rhythmical sounds as soon as they are born. These early vocalizations are important for soliciting the attention of caregivers. How they develop remains a mystery. The orofacial movements necessary for producing these vocalizations have distinct spatiotemporal signatures. Therefore, their development could potentially be tracked over the course of prenatal life. We densely and longitudinally sampled fetal head and orofacial movements in marmoset monkeys using ultrasound imaging. We show that orofacial movements necessary for producing rhythmical vocalizations differentiate from a larger movement pattern that includes the entire head. We also show that signature features of marmoset infant contact calls emerge prenatally as a distinct pattern of orofacial movements. Our results establish that aspects of the sensorimotor development necessary for vocalizing occur prenatally, even before the production of sound.


Much like human babies, newborn monkeys cry and coo to get their caregiver's attention. They all produce these sounds in the same way. They push air from the lungs to vibrate the vocal cords, and adjust the movement of their jaws, lips, tongue and other muscles to create different kinds of sounds. Ultrasounds show that human fetuses begin making crying-like mouth movements during the last trimester of pregnancy. Yet the prenatal development of this crucial skill remains unclear, as most studies of early primate vocalization take place after birth. To explore this question, Narayanan et al. focused on a small species of monkeys known as marmosets. Regular ultrasounds were performed on four pregnant marmosets, starting on the first day the fetuses' faces became visible and ending the day before delivery. The developing marmosets acquired the ability to independently move their mouth from their head over time, a skill crucial for feeding and vocalizing. By the end of pregnancy, a subset of fetal mouth movements were nearly identical to those produced when baby marmosets call for their caregivers after birth. Human ultrasound studies are needed to confirm whether vocal development follows a similar trajectory in our species.This is likely given the developmental similarities between both species. If so, work in marmosets could be helpful to understand how conditions such as cerebral palsy interfere with this process, and to potentially develop early interventions.


Subject(s)
Callithrix , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Humans , Infant, Newborn , Sound
6.
PLoS Comput Biol ; 18(6): e1010173, 2022 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35696441

ABSTRACT

Evolution and development are typically characterized as the outcomes of gradual changes, but sometimes (states of equilibrium can be punctuated by sudden change. Here, we studied the early vocal development of three different mammals: common marmoset monkeys, Egyptian fruit bats, and humans. Consistent with the notion of punctuated equilibria, we found that all three species undergo at least one sudden transition in the acoustics of their developing vocalizations. To understand the mechanism, we modeled different developmental landscapes. We found that the transition was best described as a shift in the balance of two vocalization landscapes. We show that the natural dynamics of these two landscapes are consistent with the dynamics of energy expenditure and information transmission. By using them as constraints for each species, we predicted the differences in transition timing from immature to mature vocalizations. Using marmoset monkeys, we were able to manipulate both infant energy expenditure (vocalizing in an environment with lighter air) and information transmission (closed-loop contingent parental vocal playback). These experiments support the importance of energy and information in leading to punctuated equilibrium states of vocal development.


Subject(s)
Chiroptera , Voice , Acoustics , Animals , Callithrix , Humans , Vocalization, Animal
7.
J Neurophysiol ; 127(6): 1519-1531, 2022 06 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35475704

ABSTRACT

Adult behaviors, such as vocal production, often exhibit temporal regularity. In contrast, their immature forms are more irregular. We ask whether the coupling of motor behaviors with arousal changes gives rise to temporal regularity: Do they drive the transition from variable to regular motor output over the course of development? We used marmoset monkey vocal production to explore this putative influence of arousal on the nonlinear changes in their developing vocal output patterns. Based on a detailed analysis of vocal and arousal dynamics in marmosets, we put forth a general model incorporating arousal and auditory feedback loops for spontaneous vocal production. Using this model, we show that a stable oscillation can emerge as the baseline arousal increases, predicting the transition from stochastic to periodic oscillations observed during marmoset vocal development. We further provide a solution for how this model can explain vocal development as the joint consequence of energetic growth and social feedback. Together, we put forth a plausible mechanism for the development of arousal-mediated adaptive behavior.NEW & NOTEWORTHY The development of motor behaviors, and the influence of energetic and social factors on it, has long been of interest, yet we lack an integrated picture of how these different systems may interact. Through the lens of vocal development in infant marmosets, this study offers a solution for social behavior development by linking motor production with arousal states. Increases in arousal can drive the system out of stochastic states toward oscillatory dynamics ready for communication.


Subject(s)
Callithrix , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Arousal , Feedback, Sensory , Humans , Social Behavior
8.
Dev Psychobiol ; 63(5): 1583-1588, 2021 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33826142

ABSTRACT

The development of the earliest vocalizations of human infants is influenced by social feedback from caregivers. As these vocalizations change, they increasingly elicit such feedback. This pattern of development is in stark contrast to that of our close phylogenetic relatives, Old World monkeys and apes, who produce mature-sounding vocalizations at birth. We put forth a scenario to account for this difference: Humans have a cooperative breeding strategy, which pressures infants to compete for the attention from caregivers. Humans use this strategy because large brained human infants are energetically costly and born altricial. An altricial brain accommodates vocal learning. To test this hypothetical scenario, we present findings from New World marmoset monkeys indicating that, through convergent evolution, this species adopted a largely identical developmental system-one that includes vocal learning and cooperative breeding.


Subject(s)
Brain , Callithrix , Animals , Humans , Phylogeny , Vocalization, Animal
9.
Curr Biol ; 30(24): 5026-5032.e3, 2020 12 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33065007

ABSTRACT

The domestication syndrome refers to a set of traits that are the by-products of artificial selection for increased tolerance toward humans [1-3]. One hypothesis is that some species, like humans and bonobos, "self-domesticated" and have been under selection for that same suite of domesticated phenotypes [4-8]. However, the evidence for this has been largely circumstantial. Here, we provide evidence that, in marmoset monkeys, the size of a domestication phenotype-a white facial fur patch-is linked to their degree of affiliative vocal responding. During development, the amount of parental vocal feedback experienced influences the rate of growth of this facial white patch, and this suggests a mechanistic link between the two phenotypes, possibly via neural crest cells. Our study provides evidence for links between vocal behavior and the development of morphological phenotypes associated with domestication in a nonhuman primate.


Subject(s)
Callithrix/physiology , Domestication , Phenotype , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Animals , Face/physiology , Female , Male , Pigmentation/physiology
10.
Nat Commun ; 11(1): 1096, 2020 Feb 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32094328

ABSTRACT

An amendment to this paper has been published and can be accessed via a link at the top of the paper.

11.
Trends Neurosci ; 43(2): 115-126, 2020 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31955902

ABSTRACT

Vocal production is hierarchical in the time domain. These hierarchies build upon biomechanical and neural dynamics across various timescales. We review studies in marmoset monkeys, songbirds, and other vertebrates. To organize these data in an accessible and across-species framework, we interpret the different timescales of vocal production as belonging to different levels of an autonomous systems hierarchy. The first level accounts for vocal acoustics produced on short timescales; subsequent levels account for longer timescales of vocal output. The hierarchy of autonomous systems that we put forth accounts for vocal patterning, sequence generation, dyadic interactions, and context dependence by sequentially incorporating central pattern generators, intrinsic drives, and sensory signals from the environment. We then show the framework's utility by providing an integrative explanation of infant vocal production learning in which social feedback modulates infant vocal acoustics through the tuning of a drive signal.


Subject(s)
Acoustics , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Callithrix
12.
Nat Commun ; 10(1): 4592, 2019 10 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31597928

ABSTRACT

Across vertebrates, progressive changes in vocal behavior during postnatal development are typically attributed solely to developing neural circuits. How the changing body influences vocal development remains unknown. Here we show that state changes in the contact vocalizations of infant marmoset monkeys, which transition from noisy, low frequency cries to tonal, higher pitched vocalizations in adults, are caused partially by laryngeal development. Combining analyses of natural vocalizations, motorized excised larynx experiments, tensile material tests and high-speed imaging, we show that vocal state transition occurs via a sound source switch from vocal folds to apical vocal membranes, producing louder vocalizations with higher efficiency. We show with an empirically based model of descending motor control how neural circuits could interact with changing laryngeal dynamics, leading to adaptive vocal development. Our results emphasize the importance of embodied approaches to vocal development, where exploiting biomechanical consequences of changing material properties can simplify motor control, reducing the computational load on the developing brain.


Subject(s)
Callithrix/physiology , Larynx/physiology , Vocal Cords/physiology , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Algorithms , Animals , Animals, Newborn , Callithrix/growth & development , Female , Larynx/growth & development , Male , Models, Biological , Noise , Sound , Vocal Cords/growth & development
13.
Neuron ; 104(1): 25-36, 2019 10 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31600513

ABSTRACT

Neuroscience needs behavior. However, it is daunting to render the behavior of organisms intelligible without suppressing most, if not all, references to life. When animals are treated as passive stimulus-response, disembodied and identical machines, the life of behavior perishes. Here, we distill three biological principles (materiality, agency, and historicity), spell out their consequences for the study of animal behavior, and illustrate them with various examples from the literature. We propose to put behavior back into context, with the brain in a species-typical body and with the animal's body situated in the world; stamp Newtonian time with nested ontogenetic and phylogenetic processes that give rise to individuals with their own histories; and supplement linear cause-and-effect chains and information processing with circular loops of purpose and meaning. We believe that conceiving behavior in these ways is imperative for neuroscience.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal , Brain , Animals , Cognition , Life
14.
Elife ; 82019 07 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31310236

ABSTRACT

In adult animals, movement and vocalizations are coordinated, sometimes facilitating, and at other times inhibiting, each other. What is missing is how these different domains of motor control become coordinated over the course of development. We investigated how postural-locomotor behaviors may influence vocal development, and the role played by physiological arousal during their interactions. Using infant marmoset monkeys, we densely sampled vocal, postural and locomotor behaviors and estimated arousal fluctuations from electrocardiographic measures of heart rate. We found that vocalizations matured sooner than postural and locomotor skills, and that vocal-locomotor coordination improved with age and during elevated arousal levels. These results suggest that postural-locomotor maturity is not required for vocal development to occur, and that infants gradually improve coordination between vocalizations and body movement through a process that may be facilitated by arousal level changes.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Movement , Psychomotor Performance , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Callithrix , Electrocardiography , Female , Heart Rate , Male
15.
Curr Opin Behav Sci ; 21: 27-32, 2018 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29868626

ABSTRACT

Human vocal development is typically conceived as a sequence of two processes-an early maturation phase where vocal sounds change as a function of body growth ("constraints") followed by a period during which social experience can influence vocal sound production ("flexibility"). However, studies of other behaviors (e.g., locomotion) reveal that growth and experience are interactive throughout development. As it turns out, vocal development is not exceptional; it is also the on-going result of the interplay between an infant's growing biological system of production (the body and the nervous system) and experience with caregivers. Here, we review work on developing marmoset monkeys - a species that exhibits strikingly similar vocal developmental processes to those of prelinguistic human infants - that demonstrates how constraints and flexibility are parallel and interactive processes.

16.
Curr Biol ; 28(8): 1306-1310.e2, 2018 04 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29628372

ABSTRACT

Strong relationships exist between social connections and information transmission [1-9], where individuals' network position plays a key role in whether or not they acquire novel information [2, 3, 5, 6]. The relationships between social connections and information acquisition may be bidirectional if learning novel information, in addition to being influenced by it, influences network position. Individuals who acquire information quickly and use it frequently may receive more affiliative behaviors [10, 11] and may thus have a central network position. However, the potential influence of learning on network centrality has not been theoretically or empirically addressed. To bridge this epistemic gap, we investigated whether ring-tailed lemurs' (Lemur catta) centrality in affiliation networks changed after they learned how to solve a novel foraging task. Lemurs who had frequently initiated interactions and approached conspecifics before the learning experiment were more likely to observe and learn the task solution. Comparing social networks before and after the learning experiment revealed that the frequently observed lemurs received more affiliative behaviors than they did before-they became more central after the experiment. This change persisted even after the task was removed and was not caused by the observed lemurs initiating more affiliative behaviors. Consequently, quantifying received and initiated interactions separately provides unique insights into the relationships between learning and centrality. While the factors that influence network position are not fully understood, our results suggest that individual differences in learning and becoming successful can play a major role in social centrality, especially when learning from others is advantageous.


Subject(s)
Lemur/psychology , Social Behavior , Social Networking , Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Female , Knowledge , Learning/physiology , Male
17.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(15): 3978-3983, 2018 04 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29581269

ABSTRACT

A key question for understanding speech evolution is whether or not the vocalizations of our closest living relatives-nonhuman primates-represent the precursors to speech. Some believe that primate vocalizations are not volitional but are instead inextricably linked to internal states like arousal and thus bear little resemblance to human speech. Others disagree and believe that since many primates can use their vocalizations strategically, this demonstrates a degree of voluntary vocal control. In the current study, we present a behavioral paradigm that reliably elicits different types of affiliative vocalizations from marmoset monkeys while measuring their heart rate fluctuations using noninvasive electromyography. By modulating both the physical distance between marmosets and the sensory information available to them, we find that arousal levels are linked, but not inextricably, to vocal production. Different arousal levels are, generally, associated with changes in vocal acoustics and the drive to produce different call types. However, in contexts where marmosets are interacting, the production of these different call types is also affected by extrinsic factors such as the timing of a conspecific's vocalization. These findings suggest that variability in vocal output as a function of context might reflect trade-offs between the drive to perpetuate vocal contact and conserving energy.


Subject(s)
Callithrix/physiology , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Arousal , Female , Heart Rate , Male , Speech , Voice
18.
PLoS Biol ; 16(2): e2003933, 2018 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29462148

ABSTRACT

The vocal behavior of infants changes dramatically during early life. Whether or not such a change results from the growth of the body during development-as opposed to solely neural changes-has rarely been investigated. In this study of vocal development in marmoset monkeys, we tested the putative causal relationship between bodily growth and vocal development. During the first two months of life, the spontaneous vocalizations of marmosets undergo (1) a gradual disappearance of context-inappropriate call types and (2) an elongation in the duration of context-appropriate contact calls. We hypothesized that both changes are the natural consequences of lung growth and do not require any changes at the neural level. To test this idea, we first present a central pattern generator model of marmoset vocal production to demonstrate that lung growth can affect the temporal and oscillatory dynamics of neural circuits via sensory feedback from the lungs. Lung growth qualitatively shifted vocal behavior in the direction observed in real marmoset monkey vocal development. We then empirically tested this hypothesis by placing the marmoset infants in a helium-oxygen (heliox) environment in which air is much lighter. This simulated a reversal in development by decreasing the effort required to respire, thus increasing the respiration rate (as though the lungs were smaller). The heliox manipulation increased the proportions of inappropriate call types and decreased the duration of contact calls, consistent with a brief reversal of vocal development. These results suggest that bodily growth alone can play a major role in shaping the development of vocal behavior.


Subject(s)
Callithrix/physiology , Vocalization, Animal , Aging/physiology , Animals , Atmosphere , Helium , Lung/growth & development , Lung/physiology , Models, Biological , Oxygen , Respiration , Respiratory Rate
19.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 115(6): 1143-1144, 2018 02 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29363597
20.
Sci Adv ; 3(7): e1701859, 2017 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28695214

ABSTRACT

Macaques do have a speech-ready vocal tract, but lack a speech-ready brain to control it.


Subject(s)
Speech , Vocal Cords , Animals , Brain , Haplorhini
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...