Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 66
Filter
1.
Clin Microbiol Infect ; 22(1): 71-77, 2016 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26454062

ABSTRACT

Cyst infection is a common complication of autosomal dominant polycystic kidney disease (ADPKD). Diagnosis is challenging with standard imaging techniques. We aimed to evaluate the diagnostic performance of [(18)F]fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography-computed tomography (18-FDG PET-CT) for the diagnosis of cyst infections among ADPKD patients, in comparison with computed tomography (CT) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). All APKD patients who underwent 18-FDG PET-CT for suspected cyst infection between 2006 and 2013 in a French teaching hospital were included. Diagnosis of cyst infection was retained a posteriori on an index of clinical suspicion. 18-FDG PET-CT findings were was considered to be positive in cases of cyst wall hypermetabolism. CT or MRI findings were were considered to be positive in cases of cyst wall thickening (and enhancement if contrast medium was injected) and infiltration of the adjacent fat. A control group of ADPKD patients with 18-FDG PET-CT performed for other reasons was included. Thirty-two 18-FDG PET-CT scans were performed in 24 ADPKD patients with suspected cyst infection. A diagnosis of cyst infection was retained in 18 of 32 cases: 14 with positive 18-FDG PET-CT findings, and four false negatives. There were no false positives and no hypermetabolism of cyst walls in nine ADPKD control patients. 18-FDG PET-CT had a sensitivity of 77%, a specificity of 100%, and a negative predictive value of 77%. 18-FDG PET-CT allowed a differential diagnosis in three patients. In contrast, CT had a sensitivity of 7% and a negative predictive value of 35% (p <0.001 vs. 18-FDG PET-CT). Only eight MRI scans were performed. The diagnostic performance of 18-FDG PET-CT is superior to that of CT in cyst infections, for comparable radiation doses and with no injection of nephrotoxic contrast medium, in ADPKD patients.


Subject(s)
Cysts/pathology , Infections/diagnosis , Infections/pathology , Polycystic Kidney, Autosomal Dominant/complications , Polycystic Kidney, Autosomal Dominant/pathology , Positron-Emission Tomography/methods , Adult , Aged , Cysts/diagnostic imaging , Female , Fluorodeoxyglucose F18/metabolism , France , Hospitals, Teaching , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Retrospective Studies , Sensitivity and Specificity , Staining and Labeling/methods , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
2.
Neurology ; 60(11): 1744-9, 2003 Jun 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12796524

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Dopaminergic therapy with levodopa improves motor function in PD patients, but the effects of levodopa on cognition in PD remain uncertain. OBJECTIVE: To use H(2)(15)O and PET to assess the effect of levodopa infusion on motor sequence learning in PD. METHODS: Seven right-handed PD patients were scanned "on" and "off" levodopa while performing a sequence learning task. The changes in learning performance and regional brain activation that occurred during this intervention were assessed. RESULTS: During PET imaging, levodopa infusion reduced learning performance as measured by subject report (p < 0.05). This behavioral change was accompanied by enhanced activation during treatment in the right premotor cortex and a decline in the ipsilateral occipital association area (p < 0.01). Levodopa-induced changes in learning-related activation responses in the occipital association cortex correlated with changes in learning indexes (p < 0.01). CONCLUSIONS: Levodopa treatment appears to have subtle detrimental effects on cognitive function in nondemented PD patients. These effects may be mediated through an impairment in brain activation in occipital association cortex.


Subject(s)
Antiparkinson Agents/pharmacology , Dopamine Agents/pharmacology , Levodopa/pharmacology , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Psychomotor Performance/drug effects , Aged , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/drug effects , Brain/physiopathology , Cognition/drug effects , Female , Humans , Learning/drug effects , Male , Middle Aged , Parkinson Disease/diagnostic imaging , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Tomography, Emission-Computed
3.
Neurology ; 60(4): 612-9, 2003 Feb 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12601101

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Although the pathophysiology remains unknown, most nondemented patients with PD have difficulty with frontal tasks, including trial-and-error sequence learning. If given time, they can perform cognitive tasks of moderate difficulty as well as controls. However, it is not known how brain function is altered during this time period to preserve higher cortical function in the face of PD pathology. METHOD: To evaluate this phenomenon, the authors matched sequence learning between PD and control subjects for the last 30 seconds of a PET scan. Learning during the initial 50 seconds of PET was unconstrained. RESULTS: Learning indices were equivalent between groups during the last 30 seconds of the scan, whereas rates of acquisition, correct movements, and forgetting differed in the first 30 seconds. In normal controls sequence learning was associated with activations in the right prefrontal, premotor, parietal, rostral supplementary motor area, and precuneus regions. To achieve equal performance, the PD group activated greater volume within these same regions, and also their left sided cortical homologs and the lateral cerebellum bilaterally. CONCLUSIONS: Mildly affected patients with PD demonstrated only modest impairment of learning during the first 30 seconds of the task and performed equivalently with controls thereafter. However, the mechanism by which they achieved equiperformance involved considerable changes in brain function. The PD group had to activate four times as much neural tissue as the controls, including recruiting brain from homologous cortical regions and bilateral lateral cerebellum.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Serial Learning , Aged , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Cognition Disorders/complications , Cognition Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Activity , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/complications , Parkinson Disease/diagnostic imaging , Reference Values , Time Factors , Tomography, Emission-Computed
4.
Neurology ; 59(2): 220-6, 2002 Jul 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12136061

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Clinical improvement with levodopa therapy for PD is associated with specific regional changes in cerebral glucose metabolism. However, it is unknown how these effects of treatment in the resting state relate to alterations in brain function that occur during movement. In this study, the authors used PET to assess the effects of levodopa on motor activation responses and determined how these changes related to on-line recordings of movement speed and accuracy. METHODS: Seven right-handed PD patients were scanned with H(2)15O/PET while performing a predictable paced sequence of reaching movements and while observing the same screen displays and tones. PET studies were performed during "on" and "off" states with an individually titrated constant rate levodopa infusion; movements were kinematically controlled across treatment conditions. RESULTS: Levodopa improved "off" state UPDRS motor ratings (34%; p < 0.006) and movement time (18%; p = 0.001). Spatial errors worsened during levodopa infusion (24%; p = 0.02). Concurrent regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) recordings revealed significant enhancement of motor activation responses in the posterior putamen bilaterally (p < 0.001), left ventral thalamus (p < 0.002), and pons (p < 0.005). Movement time improvement with treatment correlated with rCBF increases in the left globus pallidus and left ventral thalamus (p < 0.01). By contrast, the increase in spatial errors correlated with rCBF increases in the cerebellar vermis (p < 0.01). CONCLUSION: These results suggest that levodopa infusion may improve aspects of motor performance while worsening others. Different components of the motor cortico-striato-pallido-thalamo-cortical loop and related pathways may underlie motor improvement and adverse motoric effects of levodopa therapy for PD.


Subject(s)
Antiparkinson Agents/pharmacology , Cerebrovascular Circulation/drug effects , Dopamine Agents/pharmacology , Levodopa/pharmacology , Parkinson Disease/drug therapy , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance/drug effects , Aged , Antiparkinson Agents/administration & dosage , Brain/drug effects , Brain/metabolism , Dopamine Agents/administration & dosage , Female , Humans , Infusions, Intravenous , Levodopa/administration & dosage , Male , Middle Aged , Oxygen Radioisotopes , Parkinson Disease/diagnostic imaging , Parkinson Disease/metabolism , Radiopharmaceuticals , Tomography, Emission-Computed/methods
5.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 12(1): 42-60, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11198104

ABSTRACT

We examined the neural circuitry underlying the explicit learning of motor sequences in normal subjects and patients with early stage Parkinson's disease (PD) using 15O-water (H2 15O) positron emission tomography (PET) and network analysis. All subjects were scanned while learning motor sequences in a task emphasizing explicit learning, and during a kinematically controlled motor execution reference task. Because different brain networks are thought to subserve target acquisition and retrieval during motor sequence learning, we used separate behavioral indices to quantify these aspects of learning during the PET experiments. In the normal cohort, network analysis of the PET data revealed a significant covariance pattern associated with acquisition performance. This topography was characterized by activations in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFdl), rostral supplementary motor area (preSMA), anterior cingulate cortex, and in the left caudate/putamen. A second independent covariance pattern was associated with retrieval performance. This topography was characterized by bilateral activations in the premotor cortex (PMC), and in the right precuneus and posterior parietal cortex. The normal learning-related topographies failed to predict acquisition performance in PD patients and predicted retrieval performance less accurately in the controls. A separate network analysis was performed to identify discrete learning-related topographies in the PD cohort. In PD patients, acquisition performance was associated with a covariance pattern characterized by activations in the left PFdl, ventral prefrontal, and rostral premotor regions, but not in the striatum. Retrieval performance in PD patients was associated with a covariance pattern characterized by activations in the right PFdl, and bilaterally in the PMC, posterior parietal cortex, and precuneus. These results suggest that in early stage PD sequence learning networks are associated with additional cortical activation compensating for abnormalities in basal ganglia function.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Motor Cortex/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/diagnostic imaging , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Tomography, Emission-Computed , Adult , Aged , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Learning/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Activity/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Psychophysics
6.
Ann Neurol ; 49(2): 155-64, 2001 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11220735

ABSTRACT

We measured regional cerebral blood flow with H2 15O and positron emission tomography (PET) scanning at rest and during a motor task to study the mechanism of motor improvement induced by deep brain stimulation of the internal globus pallidus in Parkinson's disease. Six right-handed patients with Parkinson's disease were scanned while performing a predictable paced sequence of reaching movements and while observing the same screen displays and tones. PET studies were performed ON and OFF stimulation in a medication-free state. Internal globus pallidus deep brain stimulation improved off-state United Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale motor ratings (37%, p < 0.002) and reduced timing errors (movement onset time, 55%, p < 0.01) as well as spatial errors (10%, p < 0.02). Concurrent regional cerebral blood flow recordings revealed a significant enhancement of motor activation responses in the left sensorimotor cortex (Brodmann area [BA] 4), bilaterally in the supplementary motor area (BA 6), and in the right anterior cingulate cortex (BA 24/32). Significant correlations were evident between the improvement in motor performance and the regional cerebral blood flow changes mediated by stimulation. With internal globus pallidus deep brain stimulation, improved movement initiation correlated with regional cerebral blood flow increases in the left sensorimotor cortex and ventrolateral thalamus and in the contralateral cerebellum. By contrast, improved spatial accuracy correlated with regional cerebral blood flow increases in both cerebellar hemispheres and in the left sensorimotor cortex. These results suggest that internal globus pallidus deep brain stimulation may selectively improve different aspects of motor performance. Multiple, overlapping neural pathways may be modulated by this intervention.


Subject(s)
Electric Stimulation Therapy , Globus Pallidus/physiopathology , Parkinson Disease/therapy , Adult , Female , Globus Pallidus/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Parkinson Disease/diagnostic imaging , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Tomography, Emission-Computed
7.
J Neurophysiol ; 84(4): 1988-2000, 2000 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11024092

ABSTRACT

We previously showed that inactivating the anterior interpositus nucleus in cats disrupts prehension; paw paths, normally straight and accurate, become curved, hypometric, and more variable. In the present study, we determined the joint kinematic and dynamic origins of this impairment. Animals were restrained in a hammock and trained to reach and grasp a cube of meat from a narrow food well at varied heights; movements were monitored using the MacReflex analysis system. The anterior interpositus nucleus was inactivated by microinjection of the GABA agonist muscimol (0.25-0.5 microgram in 0.5 microliter saline). For each joint, we computed the torque due to gravity, inertial resistance (termed self torque), interjoint interactions (termed interaction torque), and the combined effects of active muscle contraction and passive soft tissue stretch (termed generalized muscle torque). Inactivation produced significant reductions in the amplitude, velocity, and acceleration of elbow flexion. However, these movements continued to scale normally with target height. Shoulder extension was reduced by inactivation but wrist angular displacement and velocity were not. Inactivation also produced changes in the temporal coordination between elbow, shoulder, and wrist kinematics. Dynamic analysis showed that elbow flexion both before and during inactivation was produced by the combined action of muscle and interaction torque, but that the timing depended on muscle torque. Elbow interaction and muscle torques were scaled to target height both before and during inactivation. Inactivation produced significant reductions in elbow flexor interaction and muscle torques. The duration of elbow flexor muscle torque was prolonged to compensate for the reduction in flexor interaction torque. Shoulder extension was produced by extensor interaction and muscle torques both before and during inactivation. Inactivation produced a reduction in shoulder extension, primarily by reduced interaction torque, but without compensation. Wrist plantarflexion, which occurred during elbow flexion, was driven by plantarflexor interaction and gravitational torques both before and during inactivation. Muscle torque acted in the opposite direction with a phase lead to restrain the plantarflexor interaction torque. During inactivation, there was a reduction in plantarflexor interaction torque and a loss of the phase lead of the muscle torque. Our findings implicate the C1/C3 anterior interpositus zone of the cerebellum in the anticipatory control of intersegmental dynamics during reaching, which zone is required for coordinating the motions of the shoulder and wrist with those of the elbow. In contrast, this cerebellar zone does not play a role in scaling the movement to match a target.


Subject(s)
Cerebellar Nuclei/physiology , Joints/physiology , Movement/physiology , Animals , Biomechanical Phenomena , Cats , Elbow/physiology , Electrophysiology , Reference Values , Shoulder/physiology , Torque , Wrist/physiology
8.
Brain Res ; 871(1): 127-45, 2000 Jul 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10882792

ABSTRACT

To examine the variations in regional cerebral blood flow during execution and learning of reaching movements, we employed a family of kinematically and dynamically controlled motor tasks in which cognitive, mnemonic and executive features of performance were differentiated and characterized quantitatively. During 15O-labeled water positron emission tomography (PET) scans, twelve right-handed subjects moved their dominant hand on a digitizing tablet from a central location to equidistant targets displayed with a cursor on a computer screen in synchrony with a tone. In the preceding week, all subjects practiced three motor tasks: 1) movements to a predictable sequence of targets; 2) learning of new visuomotor transformations in which screen cursor motion was rotated by 30 degrees -60 degrees; 3) learning new target sequences by trial and error, by using previously acquired routines in a task placing heavy load on spatial working memory. The control condition was observing screen and audio displays. Subtraction images were analyzed with Statistical Parametric Mapping to identify significant brain activation foci. Execution of predictable sequences was characterized by a modest decrease in movement time and spatial error. The underlying pattern of activation involved primary motor and sensory areas, cerebellum, basal ganglia. Adaptation to a rotated reference frame, a form of procedural learning, was associated with decrease in the imposed directional bias. This task was associated with activation in the right posterior parietal cortex. New sequences were learned explicitly. Significant activation was found in dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices. In this study, we have introduced a series of flexible motor tasks with similar kinematic characteristics and different spatial attributes. These tasks can be used to assess specific aspects of motor learning with imaging in health and disease.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Brain/physiology , Cerebrovascular Circulation , Motor Activity/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Brain/blood supply , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Cognition , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Learning , Male , Middle Aged , Organ Specificity , Oxygen Radioisotopes , Tomography, Emission-Computed
9.
J Neurophysiol ; 83(4): 1886-99, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10758100

ABSTRACT

This study examined the effects of selective inactivation of the cerebellar nuclei in the cat on the control of multijoint trajectories and trajectory adaptation to avoid obstacles. Animals were restrained in a hammock and trained to perform a prehension task in which they reached to grasp a small cube of meat from a narrow food well. To examine trajectory adaptation, reaching was obstructed by placing a horizontal bar in the limb's path. Inactivation was produced by microinjection of the GABA agonist muscimol (0.25-1.0 microg in 1 microL saline). Fastigial nucleus inactivation produced a severe impairment in balance and in head and trunk control but no effect on reaching and grasping. Dentate inactivation slowed movements significantly and produced a significant increase in tip path curvature but did not impair reaching and grasping. Selective inactivation of the anterior and posterior interpositus nuclei did not impair grasping but severely decreased the accuracy of reaching movements and produced different biases in wrist and paw paths. Anterior interpositus inactivation produced movement slowing (wrist speed) and under-reaching to the food well. Wrist and tip paths showed anterior biases and became more curved. Also animals could no longer make anticipatory adjustments in limb kinematics to avoid obstructions but sensory-evoked corrective responses were preserved. Posterior interpositus inactivation produced a significant increase in wrist speed and overreaching. Wrist and tip paths showed a posterior bias and became more curved, although in a different way than during anterior interpositus inactivation. Posterior interpositus inactivation did not impair trajectory adaptation to reach over the obstacle. During inactivation of either interpositus nucleus, all measures of kinematic temporal and spatial variability increased with somewhat greater effects being produced by anterior interpositus inactivation. We discuss our results in relation to the hypothesis that anterior and posterior interpositus have different roles in trajectory control, related possibly to feed-forward use of cutaneous and proprioceptive inputs, respectively. The loss of adaptive reprogramming during anterior interpositus inactivation further suggests a role in motor learning. Comparison with results from our earlier motor cortical study shows that the distinctive impairments produced by inactivation of these two nuclei are similar to those produced by selective inactivation of different zones in the forelimb area of rostral motor cortex. Our findings are consistent with the hypothesis that there are separate functional output channels from the anterior and posterior interpositus nuclei to rostral motor cortex for distinct aspects of trajectory control and, from anterior interpositus alone, for trajectory adaptation.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Cerebellar Nuclei/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Movement/physiology , Animals , Biomechanical Phenomena , Brain Mapping , Cats , Cerebellar Nuclei/drug effects , Electrophysiology , Feeding Behavior/drug effects , Feeding Behavior/physiology , Female , Forelimb/physiology , GABA Agonists/pharmacology , Microinjections , Movement/drug effects , Muscimol/pharmacology
10.
J Neurosci ; 20(23): 8916-24, 2000 Dec 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11102502

ABSTRACT

The planning of visually guided reaches is accomplished by independent specification of extent and direction. We investigated whether this separation of extent and direction planning for well practiced movements could be explained by differences in the adaptation to extent and directional errors during motor learning. We compared the time course and generalization of adaptation with two types of screen cursor transformation that altered the relationship between hand space and screen space. The first was a gain change that induced extent errors and required subjects to learn a new scaling factor. The second was a screen cursor rotation that induced directional errors and required subjects to learn new reference axes. Subjects learned a new scaling factor at the same rate when training with one or multiple target distances, whereas learning new reference axes took longer and was less complete when training with multiple compared with one target direction. After training to a single target, subjects were able to transfer learning of a new scaling factor to previously unvisited distances and directions. In contrast, generalization of rotation adaptation was incomplete; there was transfer across distances and arm configurations but not across directions. Learning a rotated reference frame only occurred after multiple target directions were sampled during training. These results suggest the separate processing of extent and directional errors by the brain and support the idea that reaching movements are planned as a hand-centered vector whose extent and direction are established via learning a scaling factor and reference axes.


Subject(s)
Hand/physiology , Learning/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Arm/physiology , Data Display , Elbow Joint/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/instrumentation , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Rotation , Shoulder/physiology , Space Perception/physiology
11.
Nat Neurosci ; 2(11): 1026-31, 1999 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10526344

ABSTRACT

Psychophysical studies of reaching movements suggest that hand kinematics are learned from errors in extent and direction in an extrinsic coordinate system, whereas dynamics are learned from proprioceptive errors in an intrinsic coordinate system. We examined consolidation and interference to determine if these two forms of learning were independent. Learning and consolidation of two novel transformations, a rotated spatial reference frame and altered intersegmental dynamics, did not interfere with each other and consolidated in parallel. Thus separate kinematic and dynamic models were constructed simultaneously based on errors computed in different coordinate frames, and possibly, in different sensory modalities, using separate working-memory systems. These results suggest that computational approaches to motor learning should include two separate performance errors rather than one.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Hand/innervation , Learning/physiology , Models, Neurological , Movement/physiology , Adult , Feedback , Female , Humans , Kinesthesis , Male , Rotation
12.
J Neurophysiol ; 81(3): 1045-56, 1999 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10085332

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study is to examine the mechanisms underlying control of intersegmental dynamics during reaching movements. Two experiments were conducted to determine the relative contributions of anticipatory and somatosensory feedback mechanisms in controlling intersegmental dynamics and whether adaptation to novel intersegmental dynamics generalizes across a range of movement directions. The mechanisms used to control interaction torques were examined by altering the inertial load of the forearm. Movements were restricted to the shoulder and elbow and supported on a horizontal plane by a frictionless air-jet system. Subjects made rapid out-and-back movements over a target line presented on a computer screen. The screen cursor disappeared at movement onset, and hand paths were displayed after each movement. After subjects adapted to a novel inertial configuration, the position of an attached mass was changed on pseudorandom trials. During these "surprise" trials, movements were initiated with the torque patterns appropriate to the previously learned inertial condition. As a result, characteristic errors in initial movement direction were predicted by an open-looped forward simulation. After these errors occurred, feedback mediated changes in torque emerged that, surprisingly, further decreased the accuracy of movement reversals. Nevertheless at the end of movement, the hand consistently returned to the starting position. It is plausible that the final position was determined completely by feedback-mediated changes in torque. In a second experiment, adaptation to a novel inertial load during movements made in a single direction showed limited transfer across a range of directions. These findings support and extend those of previous reports, which indicated combined anticipatory and postural mechanisms to coordinate rapid reaching movements. The current results indicate a three-stage control system that sequentially links anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms to control intersegmental dynamics. Our results, showing limited generalization across directions, are consistent with previous reports examining adaptation to externally applied forces and extend those findings to indicate that the nervous system uses sensory information to recalibrate internal representations of the musculoskeletal apparatus itself.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological , Movement/physiology , Posture/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Computer Simulation , Feedback , Female , Humans , Kinetics , Male , Middle Aged , Reproducibility of Results , Somatosensory Cortex/physiology
13.
J Neurosci Methods ; 86(2): 145-59, 1999 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10065983

ABSTRACT

In this review, we describe how pharmacological inactivation can be used to elucidate the central control of skilled limb movement. Local anesthetics and tetrodotoxin block neuronal cell bodies and passing fibers while gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and muscimol only block cell bodies. Blockade induction time is short (several minutes) for all the agents. Blockade duration produced by local anesthetics and GABA is 15-60 min, while that of tetrodotoxin and muscimol is up to several days. We describe our drug injection system, with an integrated microelectrode and a viewing port for visually monitoring drug flow into the injection cannula. We used glucose metabolism to assess the extent of inactivation. Intracortical lidocaine or muscimol injection produces a central core of maximal hypometabolism (1 mm radius), which could be due to drug spread, surrounded by an extensive region (several millimeters) of reduced hypometabolism, possibly due to reduced synaptic activity of neurons receiving projections from the core region. Drug injection only depresses neuronal activity, which contrasts with cooling, where there can be neuronal hyperexcitability at the periphery of the inactivation site. Our experiments in behaving animals show how pharmacological inactivation is an effective analytical tool for dissecting the differential functional contributions of subcortical and cortical forelimb representations to limb movement control.


Subject(s)
Motor Cortex/drug effects , Motor Cortex/physiology , Movement/drug effects , Movement/physiology , Animals , Microinjections , Motor Cortex/anatomy & histology , Psychomotor Performance , Red Nucleus/anatomy & histology , Red Nucleus/drug effects , Red Nucleus/physiology
14.
Neurosci Lett ; 260(1): 45-8, 1999 Jan 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10027696

ABSTRACT

Movement accuracy in normal subjects depends on feedforward commands based on representation in memory of spatial and biomechanical features. Here we ask whether memory deficits in Alzheimer's disease (AD) interfere with movement planning and execution. Nine AD patients and nine age-matched controls moved a cursor to targets without seeing their limb. Starting and target positions were always visible on a screen, while, during movement, cursor position was either visible or blanked. Patients' paths showed discontinuous segments and prolonged movement time; movement inaccuracy, which increased without visual feedback, correlated significantly with scores of disease severity, working memory and attention.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Movement/physiology , Humans
15.
Exp Brain Res ; 115(2): 217-33, 1997 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9224851

ABSTRACT

We have previously demonstrated that, in preparing themselves to aim voluntary impulses of isometric elbow force to unpredictable targets, subjects selected default values for amplitude and direction according the range of targets that they expected. Once a specific target appeared, subjects specified amplitude and direction through parallel processes. Amplitude was specified continuously from an average or central default; direction was specified stochastically from one of the target directions. Using the same timed response paradigm, we now report three experiments to examine how the time available for processing target information influences trajectory characteristics in two-degree-of-freedom forces and multijoint movements. We first sought to determine whether the specification of force direction could also take the form of a discrete stochastic process in pulses of wrist muscle force, where direction can vary continuously. With four equiprobable targets (two force amplitudes in each of two directions separated by 22 degrees or 90 degrees), amplitude was specified from a central default value for both narrow and wide target separations as a continuous variable. Direction, however, remained specified as a discrete variable for wide target separations. For narrow target separations, the directional distribution of default responses suggested the presence of both discrete and central values. We next examined point-to-point movements in a multijoint planar hand movement task with targets at two distances and two directions but at five directional separations (from 30 degrees to 150 degrees separation). We found that extent was again specified continuously from a central default. Direction was specified discretely from alternative default directions when target separation was wide and continuously from a central default when separation was narrow. The specification of both extent and direction evolved over a 200-ms time period beginning about 100 ms after target presentation. As in elbow force pulses, extent was specified progressively in both correct and wrong direction responses through a progressive improvement in the scaling of acceleration and velocity peaks to the target. On the other hand, movement time and hand path straightness did not change significantly in the course of specification. Thus, the specification of movement time and linearity, global features of the trajectories, are given priority over the specific values of extent and direction. In a third experiment, we varied the distances between unidirectional target pairs and found that movement extent is specified discretely, like direction, when the disparity in distances is large. The implications of these findings for contextual effects on trajectory planning are discussed. The independence of extent and direction specification and the prior setting of response duration and straightness provide critical support for the hypothesis that point-to-point movements are planned vectorially.


Subject(s)
Hand/physiology , Isometric Contraction/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Muscle, Skeletal/innervation , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Stochastic Processes , Wrist/physiology
16.
Neuroreport ; 7(14): 2357-61, 1996 Oct 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8951852

ABSTRACT

TO further understand visuomotor transformations in reaching, we compared adaptation to display rotation and altered gain in planar movements. Healthy subjects moved a cursor on a screen by moving an indicator on a horizontal digitizing tablet with their unseen hand. Adaptation to rotation was less complete and was accompanied by markedly increased directional variability. Adaptation training on a single target generalized broadly for gain change, but poorly for rotation. We propose that the difficulty in adapting to rotation arises from the substantial demands on short-term working memory imposed by the need to determine the new reference direction. Adaptation to gain change makes more modest demands on short-term memory to recalibrate the visuomotor scaling factor.


Subject(s)
Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological , Adult , Arm/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Rotation
17.
J Neurophysiol ; 73(6): 2535-9, 1995 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7666158

ABSTRACT

1. The dependence of directional biases in reaching movements on the initial position of the hand was studied in normal human subjects moving their unseen hand on a horizontal digitizing tablet to visual targets displayed on a vertical computer screen. 2. When initial hand positions were to the right of midline, movements were systematically biased clockwise. Biases were counterclockwise for starting points to the left. Biases were unaffected by the screen location of the starting and target positions. 3. Vision of the hand in relation to the target before movement, as well as practice with vision of the cursor during the movement, temporarily eliminated these biases. The spatial organization of the biases suggests that, without vision of the limb, the nervous system underestimates the distance of the hand from an axis or plane that includes its most common operating location. 4. To test the hypothesis that such an underestimate might represent an adaptation to a local area of work space or range effect, subjects were trained to reach accurately from right or left positions. After training, movements initiated from other locations, including ones that were previously error free, showed new biases that again represented underestimates of the distance of the initial hand position from the new trained location. 5. We conclude that hand path planning is dependent on learned representations of the location of the hand in the work space.


Subject(s)
Hand/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Skills/physiology , Photic Stimulation
18.
J Neurophysiol ; 73(2): 820-35, 1995 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7760137

ABSTRACT

1. We recently showed that patients lacking proprioceptive input from their limbs have particular difficulty performing multijoint movements. In a pantomimed slicing gesture requiring sharp reversals in hand path direction, patients showed large hand path distortions at movement reversals because of failure to coordinate the timing of the separate reversals at the shoulder and elbow joints. We hypothesized that these reversal errors resulted from uncompensated effects of inertial interactions produced by changes in shoulder joint acceleration that were transferred to the elbow. We now test this hypothesis and examine the role of proprioceptive input by comparing the motor performance of five normal subjects with that of two patients with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. 2. Subjects were to trace each of six template lines presented randomly on a computer screen by straight overlapping out-and-back movements of the hand on a digitizing tablet. The lines originated from a common starting position but were in different directions and had different lengths. Directions and lengths were adjusted so that tracing movements would all require the same elbow excursion, whereas shoulder excursion would vary. The effects of varying interaction torques on elbow kinematics were then studied. The subject's dominant arm was supported in the horizontal plane by a low-inertia brace equipped with ball bearing joints and potentiometers under the elbow and shoulder. Hand position was monitored by a magnetic pen attached to the brace 1 cm above a digitizing tablet and could be displayed as a screen cursor. Vision of the subject's arm was blocked and the screen cursor was blanked at movement onset to prevent visual feedback during movement. Elbow joint torques were calculated from joint angle recordings and compared with electromyographic recordings of elbow joint musculature. 3. In control subjects, outward and inward paths were straight and overlapped the template lines regardless of their direction. As prescribed by the task, elbow kinematics remained the same across movement directions, whereas interaction torques varied substantially. The timing of the onsets of biceps activity and the offsets of triceps activity during elbow flexion varied systematically with direction-dependent changes in interaction torques. Controls exploited or dampened these interaction torques as needed to meet the kinematic demands of the task. 4. In contrast, the patients made characteristic errors at movement reversals that increased systematically across movement directions. These reversal errors resulted from improper timing of elbow and shoulder joint reversals.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)


Subject(s)
Extremities/physiopathology , Nervous System Diseases/physiopathology , Proprioception , Adult , Afferent Pathways/physiopathology , Denervation , Elbow Joint/physiopathology , Electromyography , Extremities/innervation , Female , Humans , Joints/physiopathology , Male , Middle Aged , Movement , Muscles/physiopathology , Reference Values
19.
Can J Physiol Pharmacol ; 73(2): 273-84, 1995 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7621366

ABSTRACT

This paper reviews a series of experiments comparing intact controls with functionally deafferented patients to determine the role of proprioception in controlling dynamic interactions between limb segments during movement. We examine the control of hand path in a planar movement-reversal task and in a familiar three-dimensional gesture with similar biomechanical characteristics. In the planar task subjects had to move their hand out and back along a series of straight-line segments in the horizontal plane without visual feedback. The lengths and directions of the target line segments were chosen to require different amounts of shoulder motion while requiring the same elbow excursion. In controls, hand paths were, as required, straight with sharp bends at the outermost point. In patients, however, distinctive errors appeared at movement reversals, consisting of widened hand paths resulting from desynchronization in the reversals of elbow and shoulder motions. These errors reflected an inability to program elbow muscle contractions in accord with interaction torques produced at the elbow by variations in acceleration of the shoulder. The reversal errors were substantially reduced after patients had practiced for a few trials while visually monitoring movements of their arm. The improvement was not limited to the direction where they had practiced with vision, but also extended to other directions in which the elbow torques were different. This suggests that practice with vision of the arm served to improve the general rules that subjects used to plan movement, rather than simply improving the performance of a specific response. Similar to their performance on the planar task, the patients made errors in interjoint coordination during unconstrained three-dimensional gestures with movement reversals. We conclude (i) that both the planning and the learning of movement required an internal model of the dynamic properties of the limb that takes account of interaction torques acting at different joints; (ii) that this internal model is normally established and updated using proprioceptive information; but (iii) that when proprioception is lacking, vision of the limb in motion partially substitutes for proprioception.


Subject(s)
Joints/physiology , Movement/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Elbow/physiology , Extremities/physiology , Hand/physiology , Humans , Middle Aged , Shoulder/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
20.
J Neurophysiol ; 73(1): 347-60, 1995 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7714577

ABSTRACT

1. This paper introduces a series of studies in which we analyze the impairments in a planar reaching task in human patients with severe proprioceptive deficits resulting from large-fiber sensory neuropathy. We studied three patients, all of whom showed absence of discriminative tactile sensation, position sense, and stretch reflexes in the upper extremities. Muscle strength was normal. We compared the reaching movements of the patients with those of normal control subjects. The purpose of this first paper was no characterize the spatial errors in these patients that result primarily from impairments in the planning and execution of movement rather than in feedback control. This was done by using a task in which visual feedback of errors during movement was prevented. 2. Subjects were instructed to move their hand from given starting positions of different targets on a horizontal digitizing tablet. Hand position and targets were displayed on a computer screen. Subjects could not see their hand, and the screen display of hand position was blanked at the signal to move. Thus visual feedback during movement could not be used to achieve accuracy. Movement paths were displayed as knowledge of results after each trial. 3. Compared with controls, the patients made large spatial errors in both movement direction and extent. Directional errors were evident from movement onset, suggesting that they resulted from improper planning. In addition, patients' hand paths showed large curves and secondary movements after initial stops. 4. The overall control strategy used by patients appeared the same as that used by controls. Hand trajectories were approximately bell shaped, and movement extent was controlled by scaling a trajectory waveform in amplitude and time. However, both control subjects and patients showed systematic errors in movement extent that depended on the direction of hand movement. In control subjects, these systematic dependencies of extent on direction were small, but in patients they produced large and prominent errors. Analysis of the hand trajectories revealed that errors were associated with differences in velocity and acceleration for movements in different directions. In an earlier study, we showed that in subjects with normal sensation that the dependence of acceleration and velocity on direction results from a failure to take the inertial properties of the limb into account in programming the initial trajectory. In control subjects, these differences in initial acceleration are partially compensated by direction-dependent variations in movement time.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)


Subject(s)
Neurons, Afferent/pathology , Spatial Behavior , Adult , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Motor Activity , Movement , Proprioception
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...