Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Scand J Med Sci Sports ; 23(4): 468-77, 2013 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22093085

ABSTRACT

The effect of rowing ergometer design upon power delivery and coordination patterns of the rowing stroke was analyzed for 14 elite rowers. Rowers were tested in three ergometer conditions: the fixed stretcher Concept2c ergometer, the Concept2c ergometer mounted on sliding rails, and the sliding stretcher RowPerfect ergometer. Ergometers were instrumented to measure the external force generated at the handle and the foot stretcher and a nine-segment inverse dynamics model used to calculate joint and overall power delivery. Peak power generation and absorption at the knee joint was significantly greater, and total power delivered to the ergometer delayed on the fixed stretcher ergometer when compared to the sliding stretcher ergometers. No differences were found in the mechanical energy delivered to the handle of the three ergometers; however, greater joint mechanical energy production of the lower limb reduced mechanical efficiency when rowing the Concept2c fixed ergometer. The fixed foot stretcher on the Concept2c fixed ergometer acts to increase the inertial forces that the rower must overcome at the catch, increasing the moment and power output at the knee, and affecting the coordination pattern during the recovery phase.


Subject(s)
Equipment Design , Ergometry/instrumentation , Hip Joint/physiology , Knee Joint/physiology , Sports/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena/physiology , Energy Metabolism , Humans , Male , Torso/physiology , Young Adult
2.
Conscious Cogn ; 10(3): 425-35, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11697874

ABSTRACT

Modality specificity in priming is taken as evidence for independent perceptual systems. However, Easton, Greene, and Srinivas (1997) showed that visual and haptic cross-modal priming is comparable in magnitude to within-modal priming. Where appropriate, perceptual systems might share like information. To test this, we assessed priming and recognition for visual and auditory events, within- and across- modalities. On the visual test, auditory study resulted in no priming. On the auditory priming test, visual study resulted in priming that was only marginally less than within-modal priming. The priming results show that visual study facilitates identification on both visual and auditory tests, but auditory study only facilitates performance on the auditory test. For both recognition tests, within-modal recognition exceeded cross-modal recognition. The results have two novel implications for the understanding of perceptual priming: First, we introduce visual and auditory priming for spatio-temporal events as a new priming paradigm chosen for its ecological validity and potential for information exchange. Second, we propose that the asymmetry of the cross-modal priming observed here may reflect the capacity of these perceptual modalities to provide cross-modal constraints on ambiguity. We argue that visual perception might inform and constrain auditory processing, while auditory perception corresponds to too many potential visual events to usefully inform and constrain visual perception.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Memory/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Brain/physiology , Humans , Space Perception/physiology , Time Perception/physiology
3.
Mem Cognit ; 29(6): 893-902, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11716062

ABSTRACT

Learning complex relationships among items and representing them flexibly have been shown to be highly similar in function and structure to conscious forms of learning. However, it is unclear whether conscious learning is essential for the exhibition of flexibility in learning. Successful performance on the transitive inference task requires representational flexibility. Participants learned four overlapping premise pairs (A > B, B > C, C > D, D > E) that could be encoded separately or as a sequential hierarchy (A > B > C > D > E). Some participants (informed) were told prior to training that the task required an inference made from premise pairs. Other participants (uninformed) were told simply that they were to learn a series of pairs by trial and error. Testing consisted of unreinforced trials that included the non-adjacent pair, B versus D, to assess capacity for transitive inference. Not surprisingly, those in the informed condition outperformed those in the uninformed condition. After completion of training and testing, uninformed participants were given a postexperimental questionnaire to assess awareness of the task structure. In contrast with expectations, successful performance on the transitive inference task for uninformed participants does not depend on or correlate with postexperimental awareness. The present results suggest that relational learning tasks do not necessarily require conscious processes.


Subject(s)
Awareness , Concept Formation , Learning , Adult , Cognition , Female , Humans , Judgment , Knowledge of Results, Psychological , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual
4.
Learn Mem ; 7(1): 48-57, 2000 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10706602

ABSTRACT

Behavioral and neurobiological evidence shows that primacy and recency are subserved by memory systems for intermediate- and short-term memory, respectively. A widely accepted explanation of recency is that in short-term memory, new learning overwrites old learning. Primacy is not as well understood, but many hypotheses contend that initial items are better encoded into long-term memory because they have had more opportunity to be rehearsed. A simple, biologically motivated neural network model supports an alternative hypothesis of the distinct processing requirements for primacy and recency given single-trial learning without rehearsal. Simulations of the model exhibit either primacy or recency, but not both simultaneously. The incompatibility of primacy and recency clarifies possible reasons for two neurologically distinct systems. Inhibition, and its control of activity, determines those list items that are acquired and retained. Activity levels that are too low do not provide sufficient connections for learning to occur, while higher activity diminishes capacity. High recurrent inhibition, and progressively diminishing activity, allows acquisition and retention of early items, while later items are never acquired. Conversely, low recurrent inhibition, and the resulting high activity, allows continuous acquisition such that acquisition of later items eventually interferes with the retention of early items.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Models, Neurological , Neural Networks, Computer , Animals , Behavior/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Humans , Learning/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Neural Inhibition/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Retention, Psychology/physiology , Synapses/physiology
5.
Exp Brain Res ; 118(4): 541-50, 1998 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9504849

ABSTRACT

This study assessed whether stationary auditory information could affect body and head sway (as does visual and haptic information) in sighted and congenitally blind people. Two speakers, one placed adjacent to each ear, significantly stabilized center-of-foot-pressure sway in a tandem Romberg stance, while neither a single speaker in front of subjects nor a head-mounted sonar device reduced center-of-pressure sway. Center-of-pressure sway was reduced to the same level in the two-speaker condition for sighted and blind subjects. Both groups also evidenced reduced head sway in the two-speaker condition, although blind subjects' head sway was significantly larger than that of sighted subjects. The advantage of the two-speaker condition was probably attributable to the nature of distance compared with directional auditory information. The results rule out a deficit model of spatial hearing in blind people and are consistent with one version of a compensation model. Analysis of maximum cross-correlations between center-of-pressure and head sway, and associated time lags suggest that blind and sighted people may use different sensorimotor strategies to achieve stability.


Subject(s)
Blindness/psychology , Cues , Orientation/physiology , Posture/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Blindness/congenital , Female , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
6.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 23(1): 153-63, 1997 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9028025

ABSTRACT

Previous assessments of verbal cross-modal priming have typically been conducted with the visual and auditory modalities. Within-modal priming is always found to be substantially larger than cross-modal priming, a finding that could reflect modality modularity, or alternatively, differences between the coding of visual and auditory verbal information (i.e., geometric vs. phonological). The present experiments assessed implicit and explicit memory within and between vision and haptics, where verbal information could be coded in geometric terms. Because haptic perception of words is sequential or letter-by-letter, experiments were also conducted to isolate the effects of simultaneous versus sequential processing from the manipulation of modality. Together, the results reveal no effects of modality change on implicit or explicit tests. The authors discuss representational similarities between vision and haptics as well as image mediation as possible explanations for the results.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Neurological , Photic Stimulation
7.
J Comput Assist Tomogr ; 9(2): 339-41, 1985.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3973159

ABSTRACT

In a review of 200 patients, the position of the colon in relation to the posterior aspect of the kidneys on CT was studied. A small part of the colon was found in the left retrorenal position in 1.0% of the patients. An illustrative case report of an unusual colonic variation in which the colon coursed directly behind the kidneys is described. The anatomy of the retroperitoneal fascia and the significance of the retrorenal colon are discussed.


Subject(s)
Colon/diagnostic imaging , Kidney/diagnostic imaging , Tomography, X-Ray Computed , Female , Humans , Middle Aged
8.
Radiology ; 147(3): 875-6, 1983 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6844627

ABSTRACT

The usefulness of full-size radiographs obtained with the overhead tube following fluoroscopy was evaluated based on an analysis of 306 biphasic upper gastrointestinal studies that included follow-up postfluoroscopy views. In 32 studies with abnormal findings, the postfluoroscopy images were normal. In one case, the postfluoroscopy images revealed an abnormality not seen on spot views. The confidence levels rose in only 19 cases (6.2%) when postfluoroscopy images were used. In the appropriate setting, routine postfluoroscopy views after an upper gastrointestinal series are of little value. Nonetheless, in a teaching hospital, these images should be obtained when fluoroscopy is performed by junior residents.


Subject(s)
Digestive System/diagnostic imaging , Fluoroscopy/methods , Barium Sulfate , Diagnostic Errors , Gastrointestinal Diseases/diagnostic imaging , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...