Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
Add more filters











Language
Publication year range
1.
Psicológica (Valencia, Ed. impr.) ; 33(3): 711-733, 2012.
Article in English | IBECS | ID: ibc-102533

ABSTRACT

In order to be treated quantitatively, subjective gains and losses (utilities/disutilities) must be psychologically measured. If legitimate comparisons are sought between them, measurement must be at least interval level, with a common unit. If comparisons of absolute magnitudes across gains and losses are further sought, as in standard definitions of loss aversion, a common known zero must be added to the common unit requirement. These measurement issues are typically glossed over in complex models of decision under risk. This paper illustrates how Functional Measurement (FM) affords ways of addressing them, given some conditions. It establishes a relative ratio model for the integration of gains and losses in a mixed gamble situation with independent outcome probabilities. It subsequently documents how this model yields functional estimates of gains and losses on a common unit scale with a known zero. The psychological significance of the found integration model is discussed, and some of its implications for measurement further explored across two studies(AU)


Subject(s)
Humans , Male , Female , Models, Psychological , Probability Theory , Probability , Neuropsychology/methods , Neuropsychology/statistics & numerical data , Neuropsychology/trends , Students/psychology , Cognitive Science/methods , Analysis of Variance
2.
Percept Mot Skills ; 103(1): 5-13, 2006 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17037638

ABSTRACT

Time and the accuracy of a lexical decision performed on a letter string (either a word or nonword), presented foveally after a parafoveal preview displayed at 5 degrees of eccentricity and 100 msec. duration were measured. Students of Padova University, 10 women and 7 men, ages 19 to 23 years were subjects. The hypothesis investigated was whether the facilitatory effect, a reduction in lexical decision time due to the parafoveal preview, was tied to global visual information acquired in the visual periphery during the preview presentation. In Exp. 1, eight subjects performed the task either with no preview (No Preview) or with a preview presented at 5 degrees eccentricity in two conditions, preview of the same foveal string (Preview-Letters) and preview of symbols ("x xx...") of the same length as the foveal string (Preview Symbols). In Exp. 2, 9 subjects performed the task with two preview conditions, No Preview and preview of the foveal string in uppercase letters at 5 degrees of eccentricity (Preview Uppercase). Analyses suggested the reduction in lexical decision time due to the Preview with respect to the No Preview condition is tied to global information extracted during parafoveal presentation. The reduction in lexical decision time depends on word texture, i.e., letters' identities and also word boundary, in addition to word length.


Subject(s)
Decision Making , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Reaction Time , Visual Perception/physiology , Vocabulary , Adult , Awareness/physiology , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Male
3.
Vision Res ; 46(20): 3526-36, 2006 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16806390

ABSTRACT

This is the first study to demonstrate the selectivity of learning for contrast polarity. The finding is the main result of an investigation into the existence of central and peripheral vision mechanisms selective for contrast polarity within the texture-segregation process, using the perceptual learning paradigm in a detection task. Energy models (Malik & Perona, 1990) exclude segregation of textures composed of elements of odd-symmetric luminance profile by contrast polarity differences. Here the target was a Gabor patch (0.8 deg) of 1 cyc/deg in sine phase (odd-symmetry) embedded in a background of mirror-image elements. Our results showed that, in fovea, segregation on the basis of contrast polarity was above threshold from the first session. After learning, the target popped-out in both central and peripheral vision for durations over 10 ms. Our major result is that learning is selective for contrast polarity; it is also selective for orientation and position, all characteristics distinctive of early processing. Since the learning effects were obtained with texture composed of odd-symmetric mirror-image elements, they indicate that the output from odd-symmetric filters was not excluded or inhibited in texture segmentation, but instead played an active role. Our data support models of texture segmentation, in which detection of texture gradient is achieved on the basis of early cortical process, before the non-linear transformation of their output.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Field Dependence-Independence , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Learning , Photic Stimulation/methods , Practice, Psychological , Psychophysics
4.
Vision Res ; 46(6-7): 1091-8, 2006 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16289199

ABSTRACT

We found that a moving target line, more-vertical than 45 deg-oriented background lines, pops-out (d'=1.2) although it moves at the same speed of background elements and although it is invisible in static presentation (d'=.7). We suggest that the moving more-vertical target is more salient because the motion system responds to the orthogonal-velocity-component (V(perpendicular)=Delta d/Delta t sin theta) that is larger for the more-vertical target than for distracters. However, motion does not produce high d' when the target is more horizontal than background (d'=.6). This result is not expected if saliency resulted from the sum of saliency of orientation and motion independently coded but is instead predicted by visual search asymmetry. A line length effect on the moving target saliency also suggests that V(perpendicular) is extracted on the whole line and this operation is facilitated by line length in the same way for more-vertical and more-horizontal targets. Altogether, these results demonstrate that speed-based segmentation operating on V(perpendicular) not only affects speed and direction of motion discrimination, as previously demonstrated, but accounts for high saliency of image features that would otherwise prove undetectable of the basis of orientation-contrast.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological , Field Dependence-Independence , Humans , Orientation , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics
5.
Vision Res ; 45(18): 2384-96, 2005 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15979464

ABSTRACT

To investigate whether processing underlying texture segmentation is limited when texture is not attended, we measured orientation discrimination accuracy and visual evoked potentials (VEPs) while a texture bar was cyclically alternated with a uniform texture, either attended or not. Orientation discrimination was maximum when the bar was explicitly attended, above threshold when implicitly attended, and fell to just chance when unattended, suggesting that orientation discrimination based on grouping of elements along texture boundary requires explicit attention. We analyzed tsVEPs (variations in VEP amplitude obtained by algebraic subtraction of uniform-texture from segmented-texture VEPs) elicited by the texture boundary orientation discrimination task. When texture was unattended, tsVEPs still reflected local texture segregation. We found larger amplitudes of early tsVEP components (N75, P100, N150, N200) when texture boundary was parallel to texture elements, indicating a saliency effect, perhaps at V1 level. This effect was modulated by attention, disappearing when the texture was not attended, a result indicating that attention facilitates grouping by collinearity in the direction of the texture boundary.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Humans , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Surface Properties
6.
Perception ; 34(4): 391-407, 2005.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15943049

ABSTRACT

Three neighbouring opaque surfaces may appear split into two layers, one transparent and one opaque beneath, if an outline contour is drawn that encompasses two of them. The phenomenon was originally observed by Kanizsa [1955 Rivista di Psicologia 69 3-19; 1979 Organization in Vision: Essays on Gestalt Psychology (New York: Praeger)], for the case where an outline contour is drawn to encompass one of the two parts of a bicoloured figure and a portion of a background of lightest (or darkest) luminance. Preliminary observations revealed that the outline contour yields different effects: in addition to the stratification into layers described by Kanizsa, a second split, opposite in depth order, may occur when the outline contour is close in luminance to one of the three surfaces. An initial experiment was designed to investigate what conditions give rise to the two phenomenal transparencies: this led to the conclusion that an outline contour superimposed on an opaque surface causes this surface to emerge as a transparent layer when the luminances of the contour and the surface differ, in absolute value, by no more than 13.2 cd m(-2). We have named this phenomenon 'transparency of the intercepted surface', to distinguish it from the phenomenal transparency arising when the contour and surface are very different in luminance. When such a difference exists, the contour acts as a factor of surface definition and grouping: the portion of the homogeneous surface it bounds emerges as a fourth surface and groups with a nearby surface if there is one close in luminance. The transparency phenomena ('transparency of the contoured surface') perceived in this context conform to the constraints of Metelli's model, as demonstrated by a second experiment, designed to gather 'opacity' ratings of stimuli. The observer judgments conformed to the values predicted by Metelli's formula for perceived degree of transparency, alpha. The role of the outline contour in conveying figural and intensity information is discussed.


Subject(s)
Visual Perception , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Contrast Sensitivity , Female , Form Perception , Humans , Light , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychological Tests
7.
Neurosci Lett ; 371(1): 18-23, 2004 Nov 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15500959

ABSTRACT

We investigated the mechanisms that allow, via perceptual learning, selective modulation of a visual line-texture figure saliency in accordance with task relevance. Learning-dependent saliency increase was inferred by increased accuracy in orientation discrimination with task repetition. As a result of learning, accuracy increase was more pronounced when local and global orientation of the texture figure conflicted, and reached ceiling in both conflict and conflict-free conditions. This psychophysical effect was associated with a decrease in amplitude of negative VEP components in the configurations where global and local orientation conflicted, and to a weak increase of VEP's earliest negative component in the conflict-free condition. The VEP result is a direct demonstration that learning, in addition to increasing response of relevant channels, also reduces the weight of channels whose receptive field size and orientation tuning conflict with the task.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychophysics , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Electroencephalography , Humans , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Visual Fields/physiology
8.
Vis Neurosci ; 20(5): 501-10, 2003.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14977329

ABSTRACT

SM, a 21-year-old female, presents an extensive central scotoma (30 deg) with dense absolute scotoma (visual acuity = 10/100) in the macular area (10 deg) due to Stargardt's disease. We provide behavioral evidence of cortical plastic reorganization since the patient could perform several visual tasks with her poor-vision eyes better than controls, although high spatial frequency sensitivity and visual acuity are severely impaired. Between 2.5-deg and 12-deg eccentricities, SM presented (1) normal acuity for crowded letters, provided stimulus size is above acuity thresholds for single letters; (2) a two-fold sensitivity increase (d-prime) with respect to controls in a simple search task; and (3) largely above-threshold performance in a lexical decision task carried out randomly by controls. SM's hyper-vision may reflect a long-term sensory gain specific for unimpaired low spatial-frequency mechanisms, which may result from modifications in response properties due to practice-dependent changes in excitatory/inhibitory intracortical connections.


Subject(s)
Blindness/physiopathology , Scotoma/physiopathology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Visual Acuity , Acoustic Maculae/pathology , Acoustic Maculae/physiopathology , Adult , Blindness/etiology , Case-Control Studies , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Female , Humans , Psychometrics , Scotoma/complications , Scotoma/pathology , Sensory Thresholds , Space Perception/physiology , Vision Tests
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL