Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 26(7): 542-543, 2022 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35501247

ABSTRACT

Causal perturbations provide the strongest tests of the relationships between brain mechanism and brain function. In cognitive neuroscience, persuasive causal perturbations are difficult to achieve. In a recent paper, Ni et al. cleverly use the neuropsychiatric drug methylphenidate (Ritalin) to causally test the brain mechanisms that support goal-directed attention.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity , Methylphenidate , Attention , Brain , Humans , Methylphenidate/pharmacology
2.
Vision Res ; 194: 107959, 2022 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35182894

ABSTRACT

Attempting to match unfamiliar, highly similar faces at moderate differences in orientation in depth is surprisingly difficult. No neurocomputational account of these costs that addressed the representation of faces by which a face-similarity metric can be derived has been offered. A metric specifying the similarity of the to-be-distinguished faces is required as the rotation costs will be a function of the difficulty in distinguishing the faces. Consequently, rotation costs have typically been described in terms of angle of disparity, rather than the dissimilarity of the faces produced by the rotation. We assessed the effects of orientation disparity in a match-to-sample paradigm of a simultaneous presentation of a triangular display of three faces. Two lower test faces, a matching face and a foil, were always at the same orientation and differed by 0° to 20° from the sample on top. The similarity of the images was scaled by a model based on simple cell tuning, modeled as Gabor wavelets, that correlates almost perfectly with psychophysical similarity. Two measures of face similarity, with approximately additive effects on reaction times, accounted for matching performance: a) the decrease in similarity between the images of the matching and sample faces produced by increases in their orientation disparity, and b) the similarity between the matching face and the selection of a particular foil. The 20° orientation disparity was sufficient to yield a sizeable 301 msec increase in reaction time. An implication of the results is that the activity in V1 produced by viewing a face is fed forward to areas responsible for the individuation of that face.


Subject(s)
Face , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Reaction Time , Rotation
3.
Vision Res ; 157: 55-60, 2019 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29555301

ABSTRACT

Familiar objects, specified by name, can be identified with high accuracy when embedded in a rapidly presented sequence of images at rates exceeding 10 images/s. Not only can target objects be detected at such brief presentation rates, they can also be detected under high uncertainty, where their classification is defined negatively, e.g., "Not a Tool." The identification of a familiar speaker's voice declines precipitously when uncertainty is increased from one to a mere handful of possible speakers. Is the limitation imposed by uncertainty, i.e., the number of possible individuals, a general characteristic of processes for person individuation such that the identifiability of a familiar face would undergo a similar decline with uncertainty? Specifically, could the presence of an unnamed celebrity, thus any celebrity, be detected when presented in a rapid sequence of unfamiliar faces? If so, could the celebrity be identified? Despite the markedly greater physical similarity of faces compared to objects that are, say, not tools, the presence of a celebrity could be detected with moderately high accuracy (∼75%) at rates exceeding 7 faces/s. False alarms were exceedingly rare as almost all the errors were misses. Detection accuracy by moderate congenital prosopagnosics was lower than controls, but still well above chance. Given the detection of the presence of a celebrity, all subjects were almost always able to identify that celebrity, providing no role for a covert familiarity signal outside of awareness.


Subject(s)
Facial Recognition/physiology , Prosopagnosia/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Recognition, Psychology , Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology , Young Adult
4.
Neuropsychologia ; 116(Pt B): 205-214, 2018 07 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29408397

ABSTRACT

We compare and contrast five differences between person identification by voice and face. 1. There is little or no cost when a familiar face is to be recognized from an unrestricted set of possible faces, even at Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) rates, but the accuracy of familiar voice recognition declines precipitously when the set of possible speakers is increased from one to a mere handful. 2. Whereas deficits in face recognition are typically perceptual in origin, those with normal perception of voices can manifest severe deficits in their identification. 3. Congenital prosopagnosics (CPros) and congenital phonagnosics (CPhon) are generally unable to imagine familiar faces and voices, respectively. Only in CPros, however, is this deficit a manifestation of a general inability to form visual images of any kind. CPhons report no deficit in imaging non-voice sounds. 4. The prevalence of CPhons of 3.2% is somewhat higher than the reported prevalence of approximately 2.0% for CPros in the population. There is evidence that CPhon represents a distinct condition statistically and not just normal variation. 5. Face and voice recognition proficiency are uncorrelated rather than reflecting limitations of a general capacity for person individuation.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Neuroscience , Identification, Psychological , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Face , Humans , Imagination , Prosopagnosia , Voice
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...