Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 32
Filter
1.
Vis Neurosci ; 25(3): 469-74, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18598420

ABSTRACT

We examined achromatic contrast discrimination in asymptomatic carriers of 11778 Leber's hereditary optic neuropathy (LHON 18 controls) and 18 age-match were also tested. To evaluate magnocellular (MC) and Parvocellular (PC) contrast discrimination, we used a version of Pokorny and Smith's (1997) pulsed/steady-pedestal paradigms (PPP/SPP) thought to be detected via PC and MC pathways, respectively. A luminance pedestal (four 1 degree x 1 degree squares) was presented on a 12 cd/m2 surround. The luminance of one of the squares (trial square, TS) was randomly incremented for either 17 or 133 ms. Observers had to detect the TS, in a forced-choice task, at each duration, for three pedestal levels: 7, 12, 19 cd/m2. In the SPP, the pedestal was fixed, and the TS was modulated. For the PPP, all four pedestal squares pulsed for 17 or 133 ms, and the TS was simultaneously incremented or decremented. We found that contrast discrimination thresholds of LHON carriers were significantly higher than controls' in the condition with the highest luminance of both paradigms, implying impaired contrast processing with no evidence of differential sensitivity losses between the two systems. Carriers' thresholds manifested significantly longer temporal integration than controls in the SPP, consistent with slowed MC responses. The SPP and PPP paradigms can identify contrast and temporal processing deficits in asymptomatic LHON carriers, and thus provide an additional tool for early detection and characterization of the disease.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity , Genetic Carrier Screening , Optic Atrophy, Hereditary, Leber/genetics , Adolescent , Adult , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reference Values , Vision Tests , Visual Acuity , Visual Pathways
2.
Vision Res ; 46(27): 4449-63, 2006 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17052741

ABSTRACT

In order to account for the multi-phasic dynamics of photopigment regeneration in human rods, we developed a new model of the retinoid cycle. We first examined the relative roles of the classical and channeling mechanisms of metarhodopsin decay in establishing these dynamics. We showed that neither of these mechanisms alone, nor a linear combination of the two, can adequately account for the dynamics of rhodopsin regeneration at all bleach levels. Our new model adds novel inhibitory interactions in the cycle of regeneration of rhodopsin that are consistent with the 3D structure of rhodopsin. Our analyses show that the dynamics of human rod photopigment regeneration can be accounted for by end-product regulation of the channeling mechanism where all-trans retinal (tral) inhibits the binding of 11-cis retinal to the opsin.tral complex.


Subject(s)
Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/metabolism , Rhodopsin/metabolism , Vitamin A/physiology , Animals , Binding Sites , Humans , Models, Biological , Protein Structure, Quaternary , Vision, Ocular/physiology
3.
Vis Neurosci ; 22(4): 417-36, 2005.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16212700

ABSTRACT

Recently, we introduced a phototransduction model that was able to account for the reproducibility of vertebrate rod single-photon responses (SPRs) (Hamer et al., 2003). The model was able to reproduce SPR statistics by means of stochastic activation and inactivation of rhodopsin (R*), transducin (G alpha ), and phosphodiesterase (PDE). The features needed to capture the SPR statistics were (1) multiple steps of R* inactivation by means of multiple phosphorylations (followed by arrestin capping) and (2) phosphorylation dependence of the affinity between R* and the three molecules competing to bind with R* (G alpha, arrestin, and rhodopsin kinase). The model was also able to account for several other rod response features in the dim-flash regime, including SPRs obtained from rods in which various elements of the cascade have been genetically disabled or disrupted. However, the model was not tested under high light-level conditions. We sought to evaluate the extent to which the multiple phosphorylation model could simultaneously account for single-photon response behavior, as well as responses to high light levels causing complete response saturation and/or significant light adaptation (LA). To date no single model, with one set of parameters, has been able to do this. Dim-flash responses and statistics were simulated using a hybrid stochastic/deterministic model and Monte-Carlo methods as in Hamer et al. (2003). A dark-adapted flash series, and stimulus paradigms from the literature eliciting various degrees of light adaptation (LA), were simulated using a full differential equation version of the model that included the addition of Ca2+-feedback onto rhodopsin kinase via recoverin. With this model, using a single set of parameters, we attempted to account for (1) SPR waveforms and statistics (as in Hamer et al., 2003); (2) a full dark-adapted flash-response series, from dim flash to saturating, bright flash levels, from a toad rod; (3) steady-state LA responses, including LA circulating current (as in Koutalos et al., 1995) and LA flash sensitivity measured in rods from four species; (4) step responses from newt rods ( Forti et al., 1989) over a large dynamic range; (5) dynamic LA responses, such as the step-flash paradigm of Fain et al. (1989), and the two-flash paradigm of Murnick and Lamb (1996); and (6) the salient response features from four knockout rod preparations. The model was able to meet this stringent test, accounting for almost all the salient qualitative, and many quantitative features, of the responses across this broad array of stimulus conditions, including SPR reproducibility. The model promises to be useful in testing hypotheses regarding both normal and abnormal photoreceptor function, and is a good starting point for development of a full-range model of cone phototransduction. Informative limitations of the model are also discussed.


Subject(s)
Models, Neurological , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Animals , Calcium Signaling/physiology , Computer Simulation , Dark Adaptation/physiology , Feedback , G-Protein-Coupled Receptor Kinase 1/metabolism , Monte Carlo Method , Peptides, Cyclic/metabolism , Phosphoric Diester Hydrolases/metabolism , Phosphorylation , Photic Stimulation , Reproducibility of Results , Rhodopsin/metabolism , Vertebrates
4.
J Gen Physiol ; 122(4): 419-44, 2003 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12975449

ABSTRACT

Single-photon responses (SPRs) in vertebrate rods are considerably less variable than expected if isomerized rhodopsin (R*) inactivated in a single, memoryless step, and no other variability-reducing mechanisms were available. We present a new stochastic model, the core of which is the successive ratcheting down of R* activity, and a concomitant increase in the probability of quenching of R* by arrestin (Arr), with each phosphorylation of R* (Gibson, S.K., J.H. Parkes, and P.A. Liebman. 2000. Biochemistry. 39:5738-5749.). We evaluated the model by means of Monte-Carlo simulations of dim-flash responses, and compared the response statistics derived from them with those obtained from empirical dim-flash data (Whitlock, G.G., and T.D. Lamb. 1999. Neuron. 23:337-351.). The model accounts for four quantitative measures of SPR reproducibility. It also reproduces qualitative features of rod responses obtained with altered nucleotide levels, and thus contradicts the conclusion that such responses imply that phosphorylation cannot dominate R* inactivation (Rieke, F., and D.A. Baylor. 1998a. Biophys. J. 75:1836-1857; Field, G.D., and F. Rieke. 2002. Neuron. 35:733-747.). Moreover, the model is able to reproduce the salient qualitative features of SPRs obtained from mouse rods that had been genetically modified with specific pathways of R* inactivation or Ca2+ feedback disabled. We present a theoretical analysis showing that the variability of the area under the SPR estimates the variability of integrated R* activity, and can provide a valid gauge of the number of R* inactivation steps. We show that there is a heretofore unappreciated tradeoff between variability of SPR amplitude and SPR duration that depends critically on the kinetics of inactivation of R* relative to the net kinetics of the downstream reactions in the cascade. Because of this dependence, neither the variability of SPR amplitude nor duration provides a reliable estimate of the underlying variability of integrated R* activity, and cannot be used to estimate the minimum number of R* inactivation steps. We conclude that multiple phosphorylation-dependent decrements in R* activity (with Arr-quench) can confer the observed reproducibility of rod SPRs; there is no compelling need to invoke a long series of non-phosphorylation dependent state changes in R* (as in Rieke, F., and D.A. Baylor. 1998a. Biophys. J. 75:1836-1857; Field, G.D., and F. Rieke. 2002. Neuron. 35:733-747.). Our analyses, plus data and modeling of others (Rieke, F., and D.A. Baylor. 1998a. Biophys. J. 75:1836-1857; Field, G.D., and F. Rieke. 2002. Neuron. 35:733-747.), also argue strongly against either feedback (including Ca2+-feedback) or depletion of any molecular species downstream to R* as the dominant cause of SPR reproducibility.


Subject(s)
Eye Proteins , Photons , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/radiation effects , Rhodopsin/chemistry , Adenosine Triphosphate/metabolism , Animals , Biotransformation , Calcium Signaling/physiology , G-Protein-Coupled Receptor Kinase 1 , Guanosine Triphosphate/metabolism , Kinetics , Membrane Potentials/physiology , Mice , Mice, Knockout , Mice, Transgenic , Models, Biological , Patch-Clamp Techniques , Phosphorylation , Protein Kinases/genetics , Protein Kinases/physiology , Reproducibility of Results , Signal Transduction/physiology , Vertebrates
5.
Vis Neurosci ; 17(5): 679-99, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11153649

ABSTRACT

We evaluated the generality of two models of vertebrate phototransduction. The approach was to quantitatively optimize each model to the full waveform of high-quality, dark-adapted (DA), salamander rod flash responses. With the optimal parameters, each model was then used to account for signature, qualitative features of rod responses from three experimental paradigms (stimulus/response, "S/R suite"): (1) step responses; (2) the intensity dependence of the period of photocurrent saturation (Tsat vs. ln(I)); and (3) light-adapted (LA) incremental flash sensitivity as a function of background intensity. The first model was the recent successful model of Nikonov et al. (1998). The second model replaced the instantaneous Ca2+ buffering used in the Nikonov et al. model with a dynamic buffer. The results showed that, in the absence of the dynamic Ca2+ buffer, the Nikonov et al. model does not have sufficient flexibility to provide a good fit to the flash responses, and, using the same parameters, reproduce the salient features of the S/R suite--critical features at step onset and offset are absent; the Tsat function has too shallow a slope; and the model cannot generate the empirically observed I-range of Weber-Fechner LA behavior. Some features could be recovered by changing parameters, but only at the expense of the fit to the reference (Ref) data. When the dynamic buffer is added, the model is able to achieve an acceptable fit to the Ref data while reproducing several features of the S/R suite, including an empirically observed Tsat function, and an extended range of LA flash sensitivity adhering to Weber's law. The overall improved behavior of the model with a dynamic Ca2+ buffer indicates that it is an important mechanism to include in a working model of phototransduction, and that, despite the slow kinetics of amphibian rods, Ca2+ buffering should not be simulated as an instantaneous process. However, neither model was able to capture all the features with the same parameters yielding the optimal fit to the Ref data. In addition, neither model could maintain a good fit to the Ref data when five key biochemical parameters were held at their current known values. Moreover, even after optimization, a number of important parameters remained outside their empirical estimates. We conclude that other mechanisms will need to be added, including additional Ca2+-feedback mechanisms. The present research illustrates the importance of a hybrid qualitative/quantitative approach to model development, and the limitations of modeling restricted sets of data.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Ambystoma/physiology , Dark Adaptation/physiology , Models, Neurological , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Ambystoma/anatomy & histology , Animals , Calcium/metabolism , Calcium Signaling/physiology , Cyclic GMP/metabolism , Larva/cytology , Larva/metabolism , Membrane Potentials/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/cytology , Signal Transduction/physiology
6.
Mol Vis ; 6: 265-86, 2000 Dec 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11139649

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Recent biochemical and physiological data point to the existence of one or more Ca++-mediated feedback mechanisms modulating gain at stages early in the vertebrate phototransduction cascade, i.e., prior to activation of cGMP-phosphodiesterase (PDE). The present study is a computational analysis that combines quantitative optimization to key data with a qualitative evaluation of each candidate model's ability to capture "signature" features of representative rod responses obtained under a broad range of dark- (DA) and light-adapted (LA) conditions. The primary data motivating the analyses were the two-flash data of Murnick & Lamb. These data exhibited strikingly nonlinear behavior: the period of complete photocurrent saturation (Tsat) in response to a Test flash was reduced substantially when preceded by a less-intense saturating Pre-flash. Depending on the delay between Pre- and Test flashes, the change in Tsat (DTsat) could exceed the magnitude of the delay, and could be reduced by as much as approximately 50%, corresponding to a large reduction in gain by a factor of 10-15. The overall goal of the study was to evaluate what model structure(s) were commensurate with both the Murnick & Lamb data and the salient qualitative features of rod responses obtained under a broad range of DA and LA conditions. METHODS: Three candidate models were quantitatively optimized to the Murnick & Lamb saturated toad rod flash responses and, simultaneously, to a set of sub-saturated flash responses. Using the parameters from these optimizations, each candidate model was then used to simulate a suite of DA and LA responses. RESULTS: The analyses showed that: (1) Within the context of a model with Ca++ feedback onto rhodopsin (R*) lifetime (tR), the salient features of the Murnick & Lamb data can only be accounted for if the rate-limiting step is not the Ca++-sensitive step in the early cascade reactions, i.e., if PDE* lifetime, and not tR, is rate-limiting. (2) With tR rate-limiting, the model cannot account for DTsat exceeding the delay. (3) The Ca++-dependent reduction in tR required to effect the large gain is incommensurate with the empirical dynamics of dim-flash responses. (4) Regardless of which reaction is rate-limiting, a model using solely modulation of R* lifetime puts strong constraints on the domain of biochemical parameters commensurate with the large gain changes Murnick & Lamb observed. (5) The analyses show that, in principle, the Murnick & Lamb data can be accounted for when tR is both rate-limiting and Ca++-sensitive if, in addition to the feedback onto tR, there is an earlier, stronger Ca++ feedback that does not affect R* inactivation kinetics (e.g., gain at R* activation or transducin (T*) activation). (6) Ca++-modulation of R* activation or T* activation as the sole early gain mechanism can also account for the Murnick & Lamb data, but fails to predict the data of Matthews, and can thus be rejected along with any model of comparable form. CONCLUSIONS: The results imply that the Murnick & Lamb data per se are insufficient to rule out rate-limitation by (Ca++-sensitive) R* lifetime; evaluation of a broader set of responses is required. The analyses illustrate the importance of evaluating candidate models in relation to sets of data obtained under the broadest possible range of DA and LA conditions. The analyses are aided by the presence of reproducible signature, qualitative features in the data since these tend to constrain the domain of acceptable model structures and/or parameter sets. Some implications for vertebrate photoreceptor light-adaptation are discussed.


Subject(s)
3',5'-Cyclic-GMP Phosphodiesterases/metabolism , Calcium/physiology , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Animals , Bufonidae , Calcium Channels/metabolism , Computational Biology , Dark Adaptation , Electrophysiology , Enzyme Activation , Hydrolysis , Mathematics , Models, Biological , Photic Stimulation , Photoreceptor Cells, Vertebrate/physiology , Salamandridae
7.
Vision Res ; 35(23-24): 3279-96, 1995 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8560799

ABSTRACT

Monocular oscillatory-motion visual evoked potentials (VEPs) were measured in prospective and retrospective groups of infantile esotropia patients who had been aligned surgically at different ages. A nasalward-temporal response bias that is present prior to surgery was reduced below pre-surgery levels in the prospective group. Patients in the retrospective group who had been aligned before 2 yr of age showed lower levels of response asymmetry than those who were aligned after age 2. The data imply that binocular motion processing mechanisms in infantile esotropia patients are capable of some degree of recovery, and that this plasticity is restricted to a critical period of visual development.


Subject(s)
Esotropia/surgery , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Esotropia/physiopathology , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Infant , Prospective Studies , Retrospective Studies , Time Factors , Vision, Binocular/physiology
8.
Vis Neurosci ; 12(6): 1063-82, 1995.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8962827

ABSTRACT

We have developed a new model of phototransduction that accounts for the dynamics of primate and human cone flash responses in both their linear and saturating range. The model incorporates many of the known elements of the phototransduction cascade in vertebrate photoreceptors. The input stage is a new analytic expression for the activation and inactivation of cGMP-phosphodiesterase (PDE). Although the Lamb and Pugh (1992) model (of a delayed ramp for the rising phase of the PDE response in amphibian rods) provided a good fit for the first 2 log units of stimulus intensity without parameter adjustments, the remaining 4 log units of the data required nonlinear modifications of both delay and gain (slope). We show that this nonlinear behavior is a consequence of the delay approximation and develop a completely linear model to account for the rising phase of amphibian rod photocurrent responses over the full intensity range (approximately 6 log units). We use the same dynamic model to account for primate cone responses by decreasing the time constants of PDE activation and introducing an enhanced inactivation process. This PDE response activates a nonlinear calcium feedback stage that modulates guanylate cyclase synthesis of cyclic GMP. By adjustment of the throughput and feedback parameters, the full model successfully captures most of the features of the primate and human cone flash responses throughout their dynamic range. Our analysis suggests that initial processes in the transduction cascade may be qualitatively different from comparable processes in rods.


Subject(s)
Light , Models, Neurological , Primates/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , 3',5'-Cyclic-GMP Phosphodiesterases/metabolism , Animals , Computer Simulation , Enzyme Activation , Feedback , Humans , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/radiation effects
9.
Vision Res ; 34(18): 2387-402, 1994 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7975278

ABSTRACT

Using the sweep visual evoked potential (VEP), we have measured oscillatory displacement thresholds (OMTs) in 49 infants ranging in age from 7 to 54 weeks of age. The stimuli were high-contrast (80%), sine-wave gratings (1 c/deg) undergoing oscillatory displacements at 6 Hz. In addition to the motion thresholds, contrast thresholds for phase-reversing (6 Hz), 1 c/deg gratings were measured in the same session for 26 infants. In the main experiment, responses were recorded at the second harmonic (F2) of the stimulus frequency (12 Hz) under binocular viewing conditions. Our main finding is that, over the age range during which infants' peak contrast sensitivity (CS) first develops to within a factor of 2 of adult CS (9-12 weeks), infants' sensitivity to grating displacement is a factor of approximately 10 less than adults'. Moreover, infants' sensitivity to oscillatory motion undergoes relatively little development over the period between 2 and 15 months postnatal, gradually achieving a factor of 4.5 below adult values by 1 yr of age. Averaged over the entire age range tested, infants' OMTs were 167 sec arc, a factor of 6.4 times higher than the average OMT (26 sec arc) for 13 adults tested under identical conditions. In contrast, the infants' average CS for reversing gratings averaged only a factor of 2.5 less than the adults' average CS. In a second experiment, we took advantage of a developmental asymmetry in the monocular oscillatory motion VEP which allows for unambiguous identification of direction selective responses from very young infants. Monocular motion VEPs were measured in five infants (8-14 weeks) and their data analyzed at the fundamental frequency (F1). Responses at F1 were present in the monocular motion VEP from each infant and were 180 deg out of phase between the two eyes, identifying them as directional cortical responses with a nasalward/temporalward bias. These directional thresholds were equal to or lower than the symmetric (F2) thresholds. The presence of directional asymmetry in the motion VEP and the similarity of the monocular F1 and F2 OMTs support the notion that the OMTs measured in the main experiment were, in fact, derived from the responses of directionally selective cells in visual cortex. These data also imply that the OMTs are not derived from local contrast-reversal responses. Other models to explain infants' relative insensitivity to oscillatory motion are discussed.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Aging/physiology , Child Development/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Humans , Infant , Reaction Time , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Visual Cortex/growth & development
10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8195968

ABSTRACT

We have examined the effects of preoperative, full-time alternate occlusion on the development of visual motion processing mechanisms. Motion visual evoked potentials (MVEPs) were recorded longitudinally in 14 infantile esotropia patients during the course of standard preoperative occlusion therapy. The MVEP in these patients was initially asymmetric in a fashion consistent with a nasalward/temporalward response bias, with a motion asymmetry significantly higher than that of age-matched normals. The magnitude of the developmental motion asymmetry declined significantly after an average of 24 weeks of alternate occlusion. This result implies that the binocular motion-sensitive cells underlying the MVEP retain some degree of plasticity up to at least 1 year of age. Our data suggest further that the persistence of motion asymmetries in untreated infantile esotropia patients is maintained by an active process that can be disrupted by alternate occlusion. Alternate occlusion apparently eliminates a form of abnormal binocular interaction that supports the persistence of the motion asymmetry. We propose that one of the necessary pre-conditions for symmetricization of motion processing in infantile esotropia is the absence of abnormal competitive binocular interactions.


Subject(s)
Esotropia/physiopathology , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Motion Perception , Perceptual Disorders/prevention & control , Sensory Deprivation , Child, Preschool , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Motion Perception/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Vision, Binocular
11.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 10(9): 2084-7, 1993 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8410438

ABSTRACT

Is the flicker limit governed by the Ferry-Porter law or by some other law? We previously obtained strong evidence for the wide applicability of the Ferry-Porter law and for its variation with eccentricity [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 7, 743 (1990)]. Raninen and others [Vision Res. 28, 785 (1988); 31, 1875 1875 (1991)] have questioned our analysis of the data and our conclusions. We show that their criticism of the Ferry-Porter law is based on specious analyses and that their own data support the Ferry-Porter formulation. We also provide further evidence that the slope of the Ferry-Porter function increases markedly with eccentricity, implying that the inherent temporal properties of the retina grow more rapid with distance from the fovea.


Subject(s)
Flicker Fusion/physiology , Retina/physiology , Humans , Light , Mathematics
12.
J Opt Soc Am A ; 9(11): 1889-904, 1992 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1432340

ABSTRACT

To determine the linear, unadapted responses of the cone pathways, we have measured the critical fusion frequency (CFF) for green (555-nm) and red (642-nm) flicker as a function of retinal illuminance. Both functions obeyed the Ferry-Porter law (CFF proportional to log illuminance) to high accuracy over a > or = 5-log-unit range. In both foveola and periphery the CFF/illuminance functions were significantly steeper for green light than for red light. The peripheral 555-nm function had an average slope 1.26 times the average slope of the 642-nm function. An additive model of flicker detection could not account for the observed differences in slope. A threshold independence model, in which detection is based on the most sensitive mechanism, accurately fits the data. Whichever model is assumed, the presence of different slopes for the two wavelength flicker conditions strongly implies that the R- and G-cone pathways have different temporal properties. The occurrence of steeper CFF/illuminance slopes in response to green light implies that the linear (near-CFF) response of the G-cone pathways in inherently faster than that of the R-cone pathways at both retinal loci. These differences in R- and G-cone-mediated temporal properties complicate the fundamental concept of luminance and invalidate it for precise application over the full illuminance range.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Flicker Fusion/physiology , Humans , Light
13.
J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus ; 29(4): 232-9, 1992.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1512665

ABSTRACT

We have compared the performance of an off-axis (knife-edge) photorefractor with that of an on-axis (isotropic) system. Normal infants and children between the ages of 8 and 208 weeks were photographed with each camera both with and without cycloplegia. Refractive errors were estimated for each technique based on equations derived from ray-tracing. These refractions were compared to the results of retinoscopy under cycloplegia. Sensitivity and specificity of the two photorefraction systems were evaluated as a function of the magnitude of meridional hyperopia defined by retinoscopy. We also examined the effect of varying the photorefraction screening criterion. Thirteen percent of the infants in the screening sample presented with +3.50 diopters or more of meridional hyperopia. Using this level of ametropia as a referral criterion, the sensitivity and specificity of the off-axis system for infants without cycloplegia were 83% and 72%, respectively. For the on-axis system, sensitivity and specificity values were 85% and 53%. The use of cycloplegics did not significantly improve the performance of either system, but rather their use degraded the specificity of the on-axis system in the presence of moderate refractive errors. The results of the present study indicate that both on- and off-axis systems are effective in identifying highly ametropic infants, but that the off-axis system results in significantly fewer false positives. Moreover, the off-axis system has the advantages of an inherently greater dynamic range for a fixed camera design, and also more easily interpreted photographs.


Subject(s)
Mydriatics/pharmacology , Pupil/drug effects , Refractive Errors/diagnosis , Vision Tests/instrumentation , Child, Preschool , Humans , Infant , Photography , Predictive Value of Tests , Risk Factors , Sensitivity and Specificity
14.
Vision Res ; 32(2): 387-92, 1992 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1574853

ABSTRACT

Psychophysical area-intensity functions of individual 10-week-old human infants and adults were obtained in the dark adapted state, and in the presence of a steady background that elevated threshold 1 log unit above the dark adapted level. For dark adapted infants, the mean diameter for complete spatial summation (4.42 degrees; SD: 1.67 degrees) was significantly larger than that of adults (2.32 degrees; SD: 0.09 degrees). The background reduced the mean critical diameter to 2.67 degrees for infants (SD: 0.64 degrees) and to 1.16 degrees for adults (SD: 0.08 degrees). Spatial probability summation has similar effects on infant and adult thresholds, and, therefore, does not appear to account for the developmental decrease in critical diameters. Rather, decreases in receptive field size are suspected.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Retina/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Dark Adaptation/physiology , Differential Threshold , Psychophysics , Visual Fields
15.
IEEE Trans Biomed Eng ; 38(8): 808-13, 1991 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1937514

ABSTRACT

Our modern rectilinear visual environment contains visual stimuli for which evolution has not had time to optimally shape visual processing. One such stimulus, periodic stripes, is known to lead to visual depth ambiguity. In this paper we show that postural instability, as measured by the variance of fore and aft sway, is increased by viewing such stimuli. This instability may be the precursor of falls. Designers must evaluate the visual impressions conveyed by their systems in order to avoid postural instability due to visual ambiguity.


Subject(s)
Convergence, Ocular/physiology , Depth Perception/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Posture/physiology , Adult , Eye Movements/physiology , Humans , Optical Illusions/physiology , Reference Values
16.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 32(2): 436-9, 1991 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1993597

ABSTRACT

Visual evoked potentials (VEPs) were recorded monocularly in response to vertical gratings that underwent oscillatory apparent motion at a temporal frequency of 10 Hz. In normal infants 6 months or younger and in patients with a history of constant strabismus onset before 6 months of age, the oscillatory motion VEP contains a prominent first harmonic component that is temporally 180 degrees out of phase in the two eyes. This pattern is not seen in normal adults and is consistent with the presence of a nasalward/temporalward asymmetry of cortical responsiveness in infants and in patients with early onset strabismus.


Subject(s)
Esotropia/diagnosis , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Motion Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Child , Esotropia/physiopathology , Humans , Infant , Strabismus/diagnosis , Strabismus/physiopathology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology
17.
J Opt Soc Am A ; 7(4): 743-58, 1990 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2338596

ABSTRACT

The maximum flicker frequency was determined over a 5+6-log-unit range of retinal illuminance for a stimulus configuration designed to isolate the linear response from long-wavelength (R) cones. For a particular retinal location, the data conformed to the Ferry-Porter law and departed significantly from the predictions of the diffusion equation. The slope of the function was an invariant characteristic and was unaffected by stimulus intensity or area, modulation waveform, or modulation amplitude. However, the slope varied substantially with retinal locus, increasing by more than a factor of 2 between the foveola and 35 degrees eccentricity. This increase shows that the time constant of the linear, unadapted visual response decreases with increasing eccentricity. The difference between foveola and periphery remained at high spatial frequencies, implying that it was not attributable to lateral inhibitory effects.


Subject(s)
Flicker Fusion/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Algorithms , Animals , Humans , Mathematics , Models, Biological , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Reproducibility of Results , Retina/physiology , Statistics as Topic
18.
Vision Res ; 30(10): 1475-86, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2247957

ABSTRACT

Contrast sensitivity and grating acuity were measured using the sweep VEP method in a group of 48 infants from 2 to 40 weeks of age and in a group of 10 adults. Sinusoidal gratings were reversed in contrast at 12 alternations per sec at a space-average luminance of 220 cd/m2. During 10 sec trials, either the contrast or the spatial frequency was increased in a series of 19 steps. Thresholds were estimated by extrapolation of the VEP response functions to zero amplitude. The contrast threshold at low spatial frequencies developed rapidly from 7% contrast at 2-3 weeks to an asymptote of 0.5% at 9 weeks. For adults, maximum sensitivity at low spatial frequencies was 0.32-0.22%. The sweep VEP estimate of grating acuity showed a gradual increase in spatial frequency with age, starting at 5 c/deg during the first month and reaching 16.3 c/deg at 8 months. The mean adult acuity was 31.9 c/deg. There appeared to be two phases in the development of contrast sensitivity and acuity. Between 4 and 9 weeks overall contrast sensitivity increased by a factor of 4-5 at all spatial frequencies. Beyond 9 weeks, contrast sensitivity at low spatial frequencies remained constant, while sensitivity increased systematically at higher spatial frequencies.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Adult , Aging/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Acuity/physiology
19.
J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus ; 26(5): 254-60, 1989.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2795416

ABSTRACT

We modified a Polaroid SE camera for use as a photoretinoscope. A total of 187 infants between 2 and 18 months of age were photographed using this device. About half of these infants (97) participated in a double blind study in which the results of photorefraction were compared with those of standard cycloplegic retinoscopy. Eighty-three infants were photographed without cycloplegia. Thirty-four infants were photographed while cyclopleged. Photographs were evaluated for significant refractive errors and other ocular abnormalities. The effectiveness of the camera system to screen for significant refractive errors without the use of cycloplegia was assessed. Infants were identified to be at risk by photorefraction if, in any photograph, a hyperopic bright crescent calculated to be greater than or equal to +1.25 D was present in the pupil. Clinically significant refractive errors were defined by the results of cycloplegic retinoscopy: "at-risk" infants had either 3.5 D or more hyperopia in either eye, or astigmatism in either eye greater than or equal to 2.5 D, or anisometropia greater than or equal to 1.5 D. With these clinical criteria and the above photographic screening criterion, the camera's sensitivity and specificity were 83% and 69%, respectively. The present system compares favorably with earlier, more sophisticated units in alerting practitioners to potentially significant refractive errors in infants. Additionally, as a screening tool, this device offers the benefits of being inexpensive and easy to use, and of providing immediate feedback.


Subject(s)
Refraction, Ocular/instrumentation , Refractive Errors/diagnosis , Calibration , Double-Blind Method , Evaluation Studies as Topic , Humans , Infant , Photography/instrumentation , Pupil/drug effects , Refraction, Ocular/methods , Vision Screening
20.
Vision Res ; 29(5): 627-37, 1989.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2603399

ABSTRACT

Contrast response functions (CRFs) for the VEP were obtained with a Discrete Fourier Transform (DFT) technique employing swept contrast gratings. VEP CRFs in infants were found to have a form similar to those observed in adults, being linear functions of log contrast over a range of near-threshold contrasts. CRFs with low and high contrast lobes were present in infants, as they are in adults. Contrast thresholds were estimated by extrapolation of the CRF to zero microvolts. The effects of additive EEG noise and of the DFT data window on the shape of the measured CRF are considered. For large signals, the measured CRF is nearly independent of the additive noise, but at small signal values additive noise introduces a small bias towards larger amplitudes. The VEP signal-plus-noise distribution was modeled as a family of Rice distributions in order to evaluate the effects of bias on the estimates of threshold. The amount of bias depends inversely upon the slope of the CRF. The amount of bias introduced by a smoothing window also depends upon slope of the CRF as well as the sweep rate. The combined effects of additive noise and window bias were such that the total bias was nearly independent of CRF slope. Sweep VEP contrast thresholds were shown empirically to be unaffected by changes in the range of contrast swept.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Electroencephalography , Humans , Infant , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...