Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 20
Filter
1.
Neuropsychologia ; 147: 107561, 2020 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32712148

ABSTRACT

Since Tulving's influential work on the distinction between familiarity and recollection-based retrieval, numerous studies have found evidence for differential contribution of these retrieval mechanisms on emotional episodic memory. Particularly, retrieval advantage for emotional, compared to neutral, information has been related to recollection-, but not familiarity-mediated processes. Neuroimaging studies suggest that this recollection-based retrieval for emotional information is related to stronger engagement of regions in the medial temporal lobe (MTL), posterior parietal cortex (PPC), and prefrontal cortex (PFC). In the present study, we investigated neural correlates related to long-term memory of neutral information that has been associated with emotional and neutral contexts, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). During encoding, different neutral objects integrated with emotional or neutral scenes were presented. One week later, the encoded objects were intermixed with new ones and participants had to indicate whether the objects were previously seen or not, using the Remember/Know procedure (item memory). Furthermore, memory for the correct scene background category was also tested (contextual source memory). First, replicating previous findings, we observed a preference for recollection-dependent memory retrieval versus familiarity-dependent memory retrieval for those neutral objects encoded in emotional compared to neutral contexts. Second, consistent with these behavioral effects, objects encoded with emotional, compared to neutral, scenes produced larger memory-related activity in recollection-sensitive brain regions, including PPC and PFC regions. Third, correctly retrieved emotional compared to neutral contextual information was associated with increased activity in these brain areas. Together, these results suggest that memory for information encoded in emotional contexts is remarkably robust over time and mediated by recollection-based processes.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Memory, Episodic , Brain Mapping , Emotions , Humans , Mental Recall , Recognition, Psychology
2.
Behav Brain Res ; 380: 112417, 2020 02 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31812503

ABSTRACT

The term 'chill' refers to a short-term bodily event of high arousal, which marks an emotional peak experience when occurring in response to music. Chill responses arise in a clearly circumscribed time frame and can also be reliably elicited by unpleasant sounds. Previous research, however, mostly focused on individually selected stimuli and positive contexts, thus, limiting the scope of interpretation. Hence, we developed a standardized chill paradigm and used fMRI to test neural responses of 16 healthy volunteers to pleasant and unpleasant emotional sound material while collecting subjective reports of chill intensity and skin conductance response data. As predicted, we found chill-associated increases in autonomic arousal regardless of the valence of the sound material. Apart from activity in primary and higher auditory cortices, both pleasant and unpleasant chills were associated with anterior insula, thalamus and basal ganglia activity. In contrast, amygdala responses were observed only in association with chills elicited by unpleasant sounds. Thus, chills elicited by pleasant and unpleasant sounds share activity in a neural network that may be specifically involved in the arousal component of an emotional experience.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Basal Ganglia/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Galvanic Skin Response/physiology , Music , Thalamus/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Amygdala/diagnostic imaging , Arousal/physiology , Basal Ganglia/diagnostic imaging , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Nerve Net/physiology , Pleasure/physiology , Thalamus/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult
3.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol ; 29(2): 306-318, 2019 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30497840

ABSTRACT

Emotional-associative learning represents a translational model for the development, maintenance and treatment of anxiety disorders such as panic disorder (PD). The exact nature of the underlying fear learning and extinction deficits however, remains under debate. Using a three-day paradigm to separate the distinct learning and consolidation processes, we aimed to gain insights into the neurofunctional substrates of altered fear conditioning, extinction training and recall in PD. In contrast to studies employing one-session fear conditioning paradigms, a differential fear conditioning and delayed extinction task was conducted for the purpose of disentangling neural networks involved in fear acquisition, extinction training and recall of extinction memories. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), quality-controlled datasets from 10 patients with PD and 10 healthy controls were available from three consecutive days (day 1: acquisition; day 2: extinction training; day 3: extinction recall) with neutral faces serving as CSs and an aversive auditory stimulus (panic scream) as US. PD patients showed heightened fear circuitry (e.g. right amygdala and left insula) activation during early acquisition and prolonged activation in the right insula, left inferior frontal operculum and left inferior frontal gyrus during extinction recall compared to healthy controls. Stronger neural activation in structures conferring defensive reactivity during early acquisition and extinction recall may indicate the accelerated acquisition of conditioned responses, while extinction recall may be attenuated as a function of PD pathophysiology. Future studies should investigate the predictive value of experimental measures of extinction recall for clinical relapse.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Extinction, Psychological/physiology , Learning Disabilities , Mental Recall/physiology , Panic Disorder/complications , Adult , Brain Mapping , Conditioning, Classical , Fear , Female , Galvanic Skin Response , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Learning Disabilities/diagnostic imaging , Learning Disabilities/etiology , Learning Disabilities/rehabilitation , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Outpatients , Oxygen/blood , Young Adult
4.
Transl Psychiatry ; 8(1): 226, 2018 10 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30341278

ABSTRACT

Increased sympathetic noradrenergic signaling is crucially involved in fear and anxiety as defensive states. MicroRNAs regulate dynamic gene expression during synaptic plasticity and genetic variation of microRNAs modulating noradrenaline transporter gene (SLC6A2) expression may thus lead to altered central and peripheral processing of fear and anxiety. In silico prediction of microRNA regulation of SLC6A2 was confirmed by luciferase reporter assays and identified hsa-miR-579-3p as a regulating microRNA. The minor (T)-allele of rs2910931 (MAFcases = 0.431, MAFcontrols = 0.368) upstream of MIR579 was associated with panic disorder in patients (pallelic = 0.004, ncases = 506, ncontrols = 506) and with higher trait anxiety in healthy individuals (pASI = 0.029, pACQ = 0.047, n = 3112). Compared to the major (A)-allele, increased promoter activity was observed in luciferase reporter assays in vitro suggesting more effective MIR579 expression and SLC6A2 repression in vivo (p = 0.041). Healthy individuals carrying at least one (T)-allele showed a brain activation pattern suggesting increased defensive responding and sympathetic noradrenergic activation in midbrain and limbic areas during the extinction of conditioned fear. Panic disorder patients carrying two (T)-alleles showed elevated heart rates in an anxiety-provoking behavioral avoidance test (F(2, 270) = 5.47, p = 0.005). Fine-tuning of noradrenaline homeostasis by a MIR579 genetic variation modulated central and peripheral sympathetic noradrenergic activation during fear processing and anxiety. This study opens new perspectives on the role of microRNAs in the etiopathogenesis of anxiety disorders, particularly their cardiovascular symptoms and comorbidities.


Subject(s)
Brain/metabolism , Fear/physiology , Gene Expression Regulation , MicroRNAs/metabolism , Norepinephrine Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins/metabolism , Norepinephrine/physiology , Panic Disorder/metabolism , Sympathetic Nervous System/physiopathology , Adult , Alleles , Anxiety/genetics , Anxiety/metabolism , Brain/physiopathology , Brain Mapping , Conditioning, Classical , Extinction, Psychological , Female , Genetic Variation , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , MicroRNAs/genetics , Norepinephrine Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins/genetics , Panic Disorder/genetics , Panic Disorder/physiopathology , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide , Up-Regulation
5.
J Affect Disord ; 225: 715-722, 2018 01 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28922735

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Although research suggests that (a) childhood adversities and more recent stressful life events/conditions are risk factors for panic pathology and that (b) early life stress increases vulnerability to later psychopathology, it remains unclear whether childhood adversities amplify the association between more recent stressful life events/conditions and panic pathology. METHODS: Data were derived from a general population sample (Study of Health in Pomerania, SHIP). Lifetime panic pathology was assessed with the Munich Composite International Diagnostic Interview (M-CIDI). Childhood adversities (emotional, physical and sexual abuse; emotional and physical neglect) were assessed with the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ). More recent separation/loss events and long-lasting stressful conditions were assessed with the Stralsund Life Event List (SEL). Individuals with lifetime panic pathology (fearful spell, panic attack or panic disorder, N = 286) were compared to controls without any psychopathology (N = 286, matched for sex and age). RESULTS: Conditional logistic regressions revealed that childhood adversities as well as more recent separation/loss events and long-lasting stressful conditions were associated with panic pathology (OR 1.1-2.5). Moreover, more recent separation/loss events - but not long-lasting stressful conditions - interacted statistically with each of the examined childhood adversities except for sexual abuse in predicting panic pathology (OR 1.1-1.3). That is, separation/loss events were associated more strongly with panic pathology among individuals with higher childhood adversities. LIMITATIONS: Data were assessed retrospectively and might be subject to recall biases. CONCLUSIONS: Findings suggest that early childhood adversities amplify the risk of developing panic pathology after experiencing separation or loss events.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/psychology , Fear/psychology , Life Change Events , Panic Disorder/psychology , Adult , Arousal , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Risk Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires , Time Factors
6.
Transl Psychiatry ; 6: e773, 2016 Apr 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27045843

ABSTRACT

Epigenetic signatures such as methylation of the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene have been found to be altered in panic disorder (PD). Hypothesizing temporal plasticity of epigenetic processes as a mechanism of successful fear extinction, the present psychotherapy-epigenetic study for we believe the first time investigated MAOA methylation changes during the course of exposure-based cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) in PD. MAOA methylation was compared between N=28 female Caucasian PD patients (discovery sample) and N=28 age- and sex-matched healthy controls via direct sequencing of sodium bisulfite-treated DNA extracted from blood cells. MAOA methylation was furthermore analyzed at baseline (T0) and after a 6-week CBT (T1) in the discovery sample parallelized by a waiting time in healthy controls, as well as in an independent sample of female PD patients (N=20). Patients exhibited lower MAOA methylation than healthy controls (P<0.001), and baseline PD severity correlated negatively with MAOA methylation (P=0.01). In the discovery sample, MAOA methylation increased up to the level of healthy controls along with CBT response (number of panic attacks; T0-T1: +3.37±2.17%), while non-responders further decreased in methylation (-2.00±1.28%; P=0.001). In the replication sample, increases in MAOA methylation correlated with agoraphobic symptom reduction after CBT (P=0.02-0.03). The present results support previous evidence for MAOA hypomethylation as a PD risk marker and suggest reversibility of MAOA hypomethylation as a potential epigenetic correlate of response to CBT. The emerging notion of epigenetic signatures as a mechanism of action of psychotherapeutic interventions may promote epigenetic patterns as biomarkers of lasting extinction effects.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy , DNA Methylation , Epigenesis, Genetic , Monoamine Oxidase/genetics , Panic Disorder/genetics , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Panic Disorder/therapy , Sequence Analysis, DNA
7.
Psychol Med ; 45(8): 1675-85, 2015 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25482960

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous studies of the dimensional structure of panic attack symptoms have mostly identified a respiratory and a vestibular/mixed somatic dimension. Evidence for additional dimensions such as a cardiac dimension and the allocation of several of the panic attack symptom criteria is less consistent. Clarifying the dimensional structure of the panic attack symptoms should help to specify the relationship of potential risk factors like anxiety sensitivity and fear of suffocation to the experience of panic attacks and the development of panic disorder. METHOD: In an outpatient multicentre study 350 panic patients with agoraphobia rated the intensity of each of the ten DSM-IV bodily symptoms during a typical panic attack. The factor structure of these data was investigated with nonlinear confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The identified bodily symptom dimensions were related to panic cognitions, anxiety sensitivity and fear of suffocation by means of nonlinear structural equation modelling (SEM). RESULTS: CFA indicated a respiratory, a vestibular/mixed somatic and a cardiac dimension of the bodily symptom criteria. These three factors were differentially associated with specific panic cognitions, different anxiety sensitivity facets and suffocation fear. CONCLUSIONS: Taking into account the dimensional structure of panic attack symptoms may help to increase the specificity of the associations between the experience of panic attack symptoms and various panic related constructs.


Subject(s)
Anxiety Disorders/epidemiology , Fear/psychology , Panic Disorder/epidemiology , Phobic Disorders/epidemiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Agoraphobia , Airway Obstruction , Anxiety Disorders/psychology , Chest Pain , Chills , Cognition , Comorbidity , Dyspnea , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Germany/epidemiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Nausea , Panic Disorder/psychology , Phobic Disorders/psychology , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Risk Factors , Sensation Disorders/epidemiology , Sensation Disorders/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Sweating , Young Adult
8.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci ; 259 Suppl 2: S155-66, 2009 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19876674

ABSTRACT

Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is efficacious for panic disorder with agoraphobia (PD/A). Nevertheless, the active ingredients of treatment and the mechanisms through which CBT achieves its effects remain largely unknown. The mechanisms of action in CBT (MAC) study was established to investigate these questions in 369 patients diagnosed with PD/A. The MAC study utilized a multi-center, randomized controlled design, with two active treatment conditions in which the administration of exposure was varied, and a wait-list control group. The special feature of MAC is the way in which imbedded experimental, psychophysiological, and neurobiological paradigms were included to elucidate therapeutic and psychopathological processes. This paper describes the aims and goals of the MAC study and the methods utilized to achieve them. All aspects of the research design (e.g., assessments, treatment, experimental procedures) were implemented so as to facilitate the detection of active therapeutic components, and the mediators and moderators of therapeutic change. To this end, clinical, behavioral, physiological, experimental, and genetic data were collected and will be integrated.


Subject(s)
Agoraphobia/therapy , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy , Panic Disorder/therapy , Adult , Agoraphobia/psychology , Certification , Databases, Factual , Desensitization, Psychologic , Double-Blind Method , Fear/psychology , Female , Genetic Variation , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Panic Disorder/psychology , Patient Selection , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Socioeconomic Factors
9.
Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) ; 60(6): 750-7, 2004 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15163340

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: There is evidence from recent controlled clinical studies that replacement therapy of hypothyroidism with T4 in combination with a small amount of T3 may improve the well-being of the patients. As the issue is still the subject of controversial discussion, our study was assigned to confirm the superiority of a physiological combination of thyroid hormones (absorbed molar ratio 14 : 1) over T4 alone with regard to mood states and cognitive functioning. DESIGN AND PATIENTS: After a run-in period with the T4 study medication for 4 weeks, a controlled, randomized, double-blind, two-period (each 12 weeks), cross-over study without washout between the treatment periods was performed in 23 hypothyroid patients (three males, 20 females, age 23-69 years, 21 subjects after surgery/radioiodine, two with autoimmune thyroiditis) to compare the effects of the previous individual T4 dose (100-175 micro g) with a treatment in which 5% of the respective T4 dose was substituted by T3. MEASUREMENTS: Standard hormonal characteristics and standardized psychological tests to quantify mood and cognitive performance were measured after the run-in period and at the end of each treatment period. In 12 subjects, the concentration-time profiles of fT3 and fT4 were compared after the last administration of the respective study medication. TSH, fT3 and fT4 were measured with immunological assays. CLINICAL RESULTS: Replacement therapy with T4 and T4/T3 was not different in all steady-state hormonal, metabolic and cardiovascular characteristics except for TSH, which was more suppressed after T4/T3. The efficacy of replacement therapy with the T4/T3 combination was not different from the T4 monotherapy with regard to all psychological test scores describing mood and cognitive functioning of the patients. Mood was even significantly impaired by the T4/T3 combination in eight subjects, with TSH < 0.02 mU/l, compared to patients with normal TSH (Beck Depression Inventory: 8.25 +/- 5.01 vs. 4.07 +/- 5.60, P = 0.026). PHARMACOKINETIC RESULTS: The area under the concentration-time curve (AUC(0-8h)) of fT3 was significantly higher after T4/T3 compared to the T4 monotherapy (42.8 +/- 9.03 pmol x h/l vs. 36.3 +/- 8.50 pmol x h/l, P < 0.05) and was significantly correlated to serum TSH (r(s) = -0.609, P < 0.05). After T4/T3, patients with a history of Graves' disease or autoimmune thyroiditis had significantly higher serum trough levels of fT3 whereas the fT4 concentrations were significantly lower in patients with a nonautoimmune background. CONCLUSION: Replacement therapy of hypothyroidism with T4 plus T3 does not improve mood and cognitive performance compared to the standard T4 monotherapy. There is even a higher risk of signs of subclinical hyperthyroidism associated with impaired well-being of the patients, which is clearly caused by significant fluctuations in the steady-state fT3 serum concentrations.


Subject(s)
Hypothyroidism/drug therapy , Thyroxine/therapeutic use , Triiodothyronine/therapeutic use , Adult , Affect/drug effects , Aged , Biological Availability , Cognition/drug effects , Cross-Over Studies , Double-Blind Method , Drug Therapy, Combination , Female , Humans , Hypothyroidism/blood , Hypothyroidism/psychology , Male , Middle Aged , Statistics, Nonparametric , Thyrotropin/blood , Thyroxine/adverse effects , Thyroxine/blood , Treatment Failure , Triiodothyronine/adverse effects , Triiodothyronine/blood
10.
Psychol Rep ; 88(3 Pt 2): 1035-45, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11597050

ABSTRACT

The magnitude of the startle eyeblink response is diminished when the startle eliciting probe is shortly preceded by another stimulus. This so called prepulse inhibition is interpreted as an automatic sensorimotor gating mechanism. There is substantial support for prepulse inhibition deficits in subjects suffering from schizophrenia spectrum disorders and in psychosis-prone normals as well. Thus, prepulse inhibition deficits may reflect vulnerability on the hypothesized psychopathological continuum from "normal" to "schizophrenia." The present experiment investigated the amount of prepulse inhibition in a sample selected for "belief in extraordinary phenomena," an attitude related to measures of psychosis-proneness. Believers and skeptics were tested in an acoustic prepulse-inhibition paradigm. As expected presentation of prepulses clearly diminished magnitude of startle response, with greatest inhibition effects gained by lead intervals of 60 and 120 msec. Patterns of response were identical for believers and skeptics, i.e., attitude towards extraordinary phenomena did not seem to be related to functional information-processing deficits as has been observed in psychosis-prone normals.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Schizotypal Personality Disorder/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Reflex, Startle/physiology
11.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 156(2-3): 259-65, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11549227

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: Prepulse inhibition (PPI) of the startle reflex is a powerful tool for investigating sensorimotor gating in both animals and humans. Evidence of impaired PPI in patients with schizophrenia suggests that PPI performance might serve as a promising model to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms of this disorder. Animal data show that experimentally induced PPI deficits can be removed by the administration of antipsychotic agents. Recent clinical studies suggest that neuroleptic medication is capable of improving deficient PPI performance in schizophrenia patients as well. OBJECTIVES: The present paper reviews the published data on PPI performance in schizophrenia patients, focussing on medication effects. Using a modified meta-analytic approach, the consistency of PPI deficits in schizophrenia patients across studies is explored. In particular, methodological issues of defining PPI deficits and assessing PPI improvements are considered. METHOD: Literature search produced 12 original studies that investigated PPI performance in schizophrenia patients using comparable experimental conditions. Percentage change scores were calculated to compare the actual amount of PPI observed in schizophrenia patients and healthy controls across studies. RESULTS: Results revealed that the amount of PPI in medicated schizophrenia patients was fairly consistent across all studies. For medicated schizophrenia patients, the amount of PPI varied between 30% and 65% for the critical lead intervals. Moreover, medicated patients showed around 20% less PPI than healthy controls. Whether these group differences were statistically significant depended on the composition of the control group that showed large variability across studies. CONCLUSIONS: To delineate the effects of neuroleptic medication on PPI performance more precisely, future research should not further rely on between-group comparisons. Rather, future clinical research should take advantage of longitudinal designs to disentangle state-dependent medication effects from more stable, trait-linked factors that contribute to PPI deficits in schizophrenia.


Subject(s)
Antipsychotic Agents/therapeutic use , Reflex, Startle/drug effects , Schizophrenia/drug therapy , Schizophrenic Psychology , Humans
12.
Biol Psychiatry ; 47(1): 61-70, 2000 Jan 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10650450

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The magnitude of the startle eyeblink response is reduced if the startle eliciting stimulus is shortly preceded by another stimulus. There is evidence that schizophrenia patients exhibit impairments in this so-called prepulse inhibition. Our study investigated whether prepulse inhibition is affected by neuroleptic drug treatment as is suggested by animal research. METHODS: Prepulse inhibition was tested in five unmedicated and 20 medicated inpatients with schizophrenia, and 12 normal controls. RESULTS: The unmedicated schizophrenia patients showed a strong impairment of sensorimotor gating as indexed by the absence of prepulse inhibition. By contrast, the medicated patients showed a pronounced prepulse inhibition that did not differ from that of the normal controls. There was a substantial covariation between the rated severity of the positive syndrome and the amount of prepulse inhibition--i.e., the patients whose positive symptoms were rated as more severe showed less prepulse inhibition. CONCLUSIONS: These data suggest that the impaired sensorimotor gating of schizophrenia patients is not a stable vulnerability indicator, but may rather be related to the positive syndrome and may be improved by treatments with neuroleptic medication.


Subject(s)
Antipsychotic Agents/therapeutic use , Blinking/drug effects , Neural Inhibition/drug effects , Schizophrenia/drug therapy , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Attention/drug effects , Case-Control Studies , Electromyography , Female , Humans , Male , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Schizophrenic Psychology , Severity of Illness Index
13.
Psychophysiology ; 36(1): 66-75, 1999 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10098381

ABSTRACT

The temporal course of startle reflex modulation and autonomic response patterns to fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant pictures in subjects with high and low levels of animal fear was investigated. Thirty-eight high-fear and 48 low-fear volunteers viewed photos of snakes and spiders and pictures of neutral and pleasant content. The slides were presented for 6 s or for only 150 ms, depending on the group. Acoustic startle probes were presented at five different times after slide onset. Relative potentiation of the startle responses started 300 ms after onset of snake/spider pictures in fearful subjects. This fear-potentiated startle effect was maintained for the later probe times and was identical in the 150-ms condition. Fear-relevant pictures also prompted a sympathetically dominated autonomic response profile in fearful persons. These data support the idea that fear can be activated very rapidly, requiring only minimal stimulus input.


Subject(s)
Fear/physiology , Reflex, Startle/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Animals , Arousal/physiology , Blood Pressure/physiology , Cues , Electromyography , Female , Galvanic Skin Response/physiology , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Kinetics , Male , Photic Stimulation , Snakes , Spiders
14.
Psychophysiology ; 34(1): 97-107, 1997 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9009813

ABSTRACT

The present study was designed to examine the pattern of startle reflex modulation and autonomic responses for individuals high in animal or blood-injury fear when viewing pictures of their feared objects. Sixteen individuals in each fear group and 16 low-fear control individuals viewed 32 color slides depicting fear-relevant, unpleasant but fear-unrelated, neutral, and pleasant scenes. Free viewing times were assessed in a second phase of the procedure as an index of avoidance behavior. Exposure to pictures of feared objects resulted in a consistent startle reflex potentiation and behavioral avoidance in both fear groups. This activation of the basic aversive system was independent of the autonomic pattern of the fear responses, which differed for the high-fear groups. These results suggest that the probe startle response indexes the organism's basic motivational disposition and add new information to the assessment of fear.


Subject(s)
Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Blinking/physiology , Fear/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Heart Rate/physiology , Reflex, Startle/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans
15.
Psychophysiology ; 33(6): 698-710, 1996 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8961792

ABSTRACT

In two studies, we investigated the influence of aversive and nonaversive reinforcers on startle reactivity, visceral responses, and self-report during Pavlovian conditioning. Furthermore, we assessed how awareness of the stimulus contingencies affect conditioned discrimination in the different response systems. Conditioned potentiation of the startle response was only observed in the context of aversive learning. Moreover, blink potentiation occurred without awareness of the relationship between the conditioned and unconditioned stimulus. In contrast, skin conductance conditioning was independent of the aversiveness of the reinforcer and was only obtained for those individuals who could correctly verbalize the stimulus contingency in a postconditioning recognition test. Cardiac responses varied with the task demands of the situation and covaried with individual response stereotypes.


Subject(s)
Awareness/physiology , Learning/physiology , Reflex, Startle/physiology , Skin Physiological Phenomena , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
16.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 102(3): 453-65, 1993 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8408958

ABSTRACT

A multiple-response analysis of aversive learning was conducted in human subjects. For each subject, two pictorial stimuli were presented--one paired with electric shock. After training, the magnitude of the acoustic startle eyeblink reflex elicited in the context of the shocked picture increased dramatically and was significantly larger than for reflexes elicited during the nonshocked stimulus. Five different picture contents were tested in separate groups: Reflex potentiation was larger for pictures rated as pleasant than pictures rated as unpleasant. Conditioned responses were also evident for skin conductance, heart rate, and affective judgments. Different systems reflected different aspects of the acquired fear response: Conductance change covaried with arousal, and startle probe magnitude varied with affective valence (pleasure). The neurophysiological implications of the data are elucidated, and parallels drawn between animal and human subjects findings.


Subject(s)
Affect , Association Learning , Conditioning, Classical , Fear , Reflex, Startle , Adult , Arousal , Electroshock , Facial Expression , Female , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual
17.
Psychophysiology ; 30(3): 261-73, 1993 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8497555

ABSTRACT

Colored photographic pictures that varied widely across the affective dimensions of valence (pleasant-unpleasant) and arousal (excited-calm) were each viewed for a 6-s period while facial electromyographic (zygomatic and corrugator muscle activity) and visceral (heart rate and skin conductance) reactions were measured. Judgments relating to pleasure, arousal, interest, and emotional state were measured, as was choice viewing time. Significant covariation was obtained between (a) facial expression and affective valence judgments and (b) skin conductance magnitude and arousal ratings. Interest ratings and viewing time were also associated with arousal. Although differences due to the subject's gender and cognitive style were obtained, affective responses were largely independent of the personality factors investigated. Response specificity, particularly facial expressiveness, supported the view that specific affects have unique patterns of reactivity. The consistency of the dimensional relationships between evaluative judgments (i.e., pleasure and arousal) and physiological response, however, emphasizes that emotion is fundamentally organized by these motivational parameters.


Subject(s)
Behavior/physiology , Facial Expression , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Arousal/physiology , Electromyography , Emotions/physiology , Facial Muscles/physiology , Female , Galvanic Skin Response/physiology , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Personality/physiology , Reproducibility of Results , Sex Characteristics , Time Factors
18.
Integr Physiol Behav Sci ; 28(2): 171-6, 1993.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8318444

ABSTRACT

Animal data suggest that shock sensitization as well as aversive learning potentiates the acoustic startle reflex. The present experiment tested, whether this shock sensitization also occurs in human subjects and whether it precedes aversive conditioning. Sixty subjects viewed--prior to conditioning--a series of slides of different emotional contents including the to be conditioned stimuli (CSs). Afterwards, the experimenter attached the shock electrodes and initiated shock exposure. Then, subjects were randomly assigned to view a series of two slides, each for eight acquisition trials in which one slide was followed by a shock. Subsequently, extinction trials (12 for each slide) were administered. During preconditioning, acquisition, and extinction, startle probes occurred unpredictably during and between slide viewing. Preconditioning data replicated previous results by Lang and his associates, showing that the startle response magnitude is directly related to the affective valence induced by the slides. Shock exposure strongly facilitated the startle reflex magnitude. This shock sensitization was absent for the skin conductance response. Course of learning also varied for both response systems. The data suggest that startle reflex potentiation indexes the acquisition of an avoidance disposition, which is preceded by a general sensitization of the protective reflexes. Skin conductance learning follows arousal changes and is modulated by cognitive processes.


Subject(s)
Arousal , Avoidance Learning , Conditioning, Classical , Galvanic Skin Response , Reflex, Startle , Adult , Fear , Female , Humans , Mental Recall
19.
Integr Physiol Behav Sci ; 26(2): 119-26, 1991.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1878318

ABSTRACT

A differential conditioning study examined whether an acoustic startle probe, presented during extinction of an aversively conditioned visual stimulus, potentiated the reflex eyeblink response in humans and whether this potentiation varied with the change in affective valence of the conditioned stimulus. Sixty college students were randomly assigned to view a series of two slides, depicting either unpleasant/highly arousing, unpleasant/moderate arousing, neutral/calm, pleasant/moderate arousing or pleasant/highly arousing scenes and objects (duration: 8 sec). During preconditioning (8 trials) and extinction (24 trials) acoustic startle probes (white noise bursts [50 ms; 95 dBA] were administered during and between slide presentation). During acquisition (16 trials) CS+ was reinforced by an electric shock. Startle response magnitudes significantly increased from preconditioning to extinction and were substantially larger to CS+. Conditioned startle reflex augmentation linearly increased with the pleasantness of the slides. Furthermore, subjects showed a greater post-conditioning increase of judged aversiveness to slides that they had previously reported to be more pleasant, exactly paralleling the startle reflex results.


Subject(s)
Avoidance Learning , Blinking , Conditioning, Classical , Fear , Reflex, Startle , Adult , Affect , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time
20.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 98(4): 395-406, 1989 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2592673

ABSTRACT

Twenty-three subjects rated the belongingness of pairs of conditionable (photographic slides) and unconditioned (e.g., shock, tone, human scream) stimuli. Forty new subjects were then classically conditioned, using rating-defined high (angry face/scream) and low (landscape/scream) belongingness pairs. Finger-pulse responses to the high-belongingness pairs showed superior acquisition and resistance to extinction. Another 40 subjects were conditioned to compound stimuli: a slide (either landscape or angry face) that was the same over trials, and a yellow or blue background that was the discriminant cue for the unconditioned stimulus (scream). When the angry face (the high-belongingness slide) was the invariant part of the compound, relatively poorer differential pulse-volume and skin-conductance conditioning was observed. Thus, depending on the task, a priori belongingness rendered stimuli selectively conditionable, either enhancing or inhibiting visceral response associations.


Subject(s)
Association , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Extinction, Psychological , Female , Galvanic Skin Response , Humans , Male , Physical Stimulation , Pulse
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...