Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 194
Filter
1.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(6)2024 Jun 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38884282

ABSTRACT

Humanoid robots have been designed to look more and more like humans to meet social demands. How do people empathize humanoid robots who look the same as but are essentially different from humans? We addressed this issue by examining subjective feelings, electrophysiological activities, and functional magnetic resonance imaging signals during perception of pain and neutral expressions of faces that were recognized as patients or humanoid robots. We found that healthy adults reported deceased feelings of understanding and sharing of humanoid robots' compared to patients' pain. Moreover, humanoid robot (vs. patient) identities reduced long-latency electrophysiological responses and blood oxygenation level-dependent signals in the left temporoparietal junction in response to pain (vs. neutral) expressions. Furthermore, we showed evidence that humanoid robot identities inhibited a causal input from the right ventral lateral prefrontal cortex to the left temporoparietal junction, contrasting the opposite effect produced by patient identities. These results suggest a neural model of modulations of empathy by humanoid robot identity through interactions between the cognitive and affective empathy networks, which provides a neurocognitive basis for understanding human-robot interactions.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Brain , Empathy , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Robotics , Humans , Empathy/physiology , Male , Female , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Adult , Young Adult , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Multimodal Imaging/methods , Electroencephalography , Facial Expression , Pain/psychology , Pain/diagnostic imaging , Pain/physiopathology
2.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(9): e2313073121, 2024 Feb 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38381794

ABSTRACT

Theories of moral development propose that empathy is transmitted across individuals. However, the mechanisms through which empathy is socially transmitted remain unclear. Here, we combine computational learning models and functional MRI to investigate whether, and if so, how empathic and non-empathic responses observed in others affect the empathy of female observers. The results of three independent studies showed that watching empathic or non-empathic responses generates a learning signal that respectively increases or decreases empathy ratings of the observer. A fourth study revealed that the learning-related transmission of empathy is stronger when observing human rather than computer demonstrators. Finally, we show that the social transmission of empathy alters empathy-related responses in the anterior insula, i.e., the same region that correlated with empathy baseline ratings, as well as its functional connectivity with the temporoparietal junction. Together, our findings provide a computational and neural mechanism for the social transmission of empathy that accounts for changes in individual empathic responses in empathic and non-empathic social environments.


Subject(s)
Brain , Empathy , Humans , Female , Brain/physiology , Learning , Reinforcement, Psychology , Social Environment
3.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(2)2024 01 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38300214

ABSTRACT

Previous research on racial ingroup bias in empathy for pain focused on neural responses to a single person's suffering. It is unclear whether empathy for simultaneously perceived multiple individuals' pain (denoted as collective empathy in this study) is also sensitive to perceived racial identities of empathy targets. We addressed this issue by recording electroencephalography from Chinese adults who responded to racial identities of 2 × 2 arrays of Asian or White faces in which 4 faces, 1 face, or no face showed painful expressions. Participants reported greater feelings of others' pain and their own unpleasantness when viewing 4 compared to 1 (or no) painful faces. Behavioral responses to racial identities of faces revealed decreased speeds of information acquisition when responding to the face arrays with 4 (vs. 1 or no) painful expressions of Asian (but not White) faces. Moreover, Asian compared to White face arrays with 4 (vs. 1 or no) painful expressions elicited a larger positive neural response at 160-190 ms (P2) at the frontal/central electrodes and enhanced alpha synchronizations at 288-1,000 ms at the central electrodes. Our findings provide evidence for racial ingroup biases in collective empathy for pain and unravel its relevant neural underpinnings.


Subject(s)
Empathy , Racism , Adult , Humans , Electroencephalography , Pain , Emotions
4.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(2)2024 01 31.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38383721

ABSTRACT

Given the increasing presence of robots in everyday environments and the significant challenge posed by social interactions with robots, it is crucial to gain a deeper understanding into the social evaluations of robots. One potentially effective approach to comprehend the fundamental processes underlying controlled and automatic evaluations of robots is to probe brain response to different perception levels of robot-related stimuli. Here, we investigate controlled and automatic evaluations of robots based on brain responses during viewing of suprathreshold (duration: 200 ms) and subthreshold (duration: 17 ms) humanoid robot stimuli. Our behavioral analysis revealed that despite participants' self-reported positive attitudes, they held negative implicit attitudes toward humanoid robots. Neuroimaging analysis indicated that subthreshold presentation of humanoid robot stimuli elicited significant activation in the left amygdala, which was associated with negative implicit attitudes. Conversely, no significant left amygdala activation was observed during suprathreshold presentation. Following successful attenuation of negative attitudes, the left amygdala response to subthreshold presentation of humanoid robot stimuli decreased, and this decrease correlated positively with the reduction in negative attitudes. These findings provide evidence for separable patterns of amygdala activation between controlled and automatic processing of robots, suggesting that controlled evaluations may influence automatic evaluations of robots.


Subject(s)
Robotics , Humans , Robotics/methods , Brain/physiology , Neuroimaging , Amygdala/diagnostic imaging , Self Report
5.
Plant Phenomics ; 6: 0144, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38304301

ABSTRACT

Solar-induced chlorophyll fluorescence (SIF) has shown remarkable results in estimating vegetation carbon cycles, and combining it with the photochemical reflectance index (PRI) has great potential for estimating gross primary productivity (GPP). However, few studies have used SIF combined with PRI to estimate crop canopy GPP. Large temporal and spatial variability between SIF, PRI, and GPP has also been found in remote sensing observations, and the observed PRI and SIF are influenced by the ratio of different observed information (e.g., background, direct sunlit, and shaded leaves) and the physiological state of the vegetation. In this study, the PRI and SIF from a multi-angle spectrometer and the GPP from an eddy covariance system were used to assess the ability of the PRI to enhance the SIF-GPP estimation model. A semi-empirical kernel-driven Bidirectional Reflectance Distribution Function (BRDF) model was used to describe the hotspot PRI/SIF (PRIhs/SIFhs), and a modified two-leaf model was used to calculate the total canopy PRI/SIF (PRItot/SIFtot). We compared the accuracies of PRIhs/SIFhs and PRItot/SIFtot in estimating GPP. The results indicated that the PRItot+SIFtot-GPP model performed the best, with a correlation coefficient (R2) of the validation dataset of 0.88, a root mean square error (RMSE) of 3.74, and relative prediction deviation (RPD) of 2.71. The leaf area index (LAI) had a linear effect on the PRI/SIF estimation of GPP, but the temperature and vapor pressure differences had nonlinear effects. Compared with hotspot PRIhs/SIFhs, PRItot/SIFtot exhibited better consistency with GPP across different time series. Our research demonstrates that PRI is effective in enhancing SIF and PRI for estimating GPP on the rice canopy and also suggests that the two-leaf model would contribute to the vegetation index tracking the real-time crop productivity.

6.
Neurosci Bull ; 40(2): 157-170, 2024 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37635197

ABSTRACT

In this study, we investigated how empathic neural responses unfold over time in different empathy networks when viewing same-race and other-race individuals in dynamic painful conditions. We recorded magnetoencephalography signals from Chinese adults when viewing video clips showing a dynamic painful (or non-painful) stimulation to Asian and White models' faces to trigger painful (or neutral) expressions. We found that perceived dynamic pain in Asian models modulated neural activities in the visual cortex at 100 ms-200 ms, in the orbitofrontal and subgenual anterior cingulate cortices at 150 ms-200 ms, in the anterior cingulate cortex around 250 ms-350 ms, and in the temporoparietal junction and middle temporal gyrus around 600 ms after video onset. Perceived dynamic pain in White models modulated activities in the visual, anterior cingulate, and primary sensory cortices after 500 ms. Our findings unraveled earlier dynamic activities in multiple neural circuits in response to same-race (vs other-race) individuals in dynamic painful situations.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Racism , Adult , Humans , Pain , Empathy , Gyrus Cinguli/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Brain/physiology
7.
Macromol Rapid Commun ; 45(3): e2300510, 2024 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37849407

ABSTRACT

Polyimide (PI) film with hydrophilic greatly limits their application in the field of microelectronic device packaging. A novel hydrophobic PI film with sag structure and improved mechanical properties is prepared relying on the reaction between anhydride-terminated isocyanate-based polyimide (PIY) containing a seven-membered ring structure and the amino-terminated polyamide acid (PAA) via multi-hybrid strategy, this work named it as hybrid PI film and marked it as PI-PIY-X. PI-PIY-30 showed excellent hydrophobic properties, and the water contact angle could reach to 102°, which is 20% and 55% higher than simply PI film and PIY film, respectively. The water absorption is only 1.02%, with a decrease of 49% and 53% compared with PI and PIY. Due to that the degradation of seven-membered ring and generation of carbon dioxide led to the formation of sag structure, the size of sag structures is ≈16.84 and 534.55 nm for in-plane and out-plane direction, which are observed on surface of PI-PIY-30. Meanwhile, PI-PIY-30 possessed improved mechanical properties, and the tensile strength is 109.08 MPa, with 5% and more than 56% higher than that of pure PI and PIY film, showing greatly application prospects in the field of integrated circuit.


Subject(s)
Amino Acids , Anhydrides , Carbon Dioxide , Isocyanates , Water
8.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(20): 10546-10557, 2023 Oct 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37585733

ABSTRACT

Both enhanced discrimination of low-level features of auditory stimuli and mutations of SHANK3 (a gene that encodes a synaptic scaffolding protein) have been identified in autism spectrum disorder patients. However, experimental evidence regarding whether SHANK3 mutations lead to enhanced neural processing of low-level features of auditory stimuli is lacking. The present study investigated this possibility by examining effects of Shank3 mutations on early neural processing of pitch (tone frequency) in dogs. We recorded electrocorticograms from wild-type and Shank3 mutant dogs using an oddball paradigm in which deviant tones of different frequencies or probabilities were presented along with other tones in a repetitive stream (standards). We found that, relative to wild-type dogs, Shank3 mutant dogs exhibited larger amplitudes of early neural responses to deviant tones and greater sensitivity to variations of deviant frequencies within 100 ms after tone onsets. In addition, the enhanced early neural responses to deviant tones in Shank3 mutant dogs were observed independently of the probability of deviant tones. Our findings highlight an essential functional role of Shank3 in modulations of early neural detection of novel sounds and offer new insights into the genetic basis of the atypical auditory information processing in autism patients.

9.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(20): 10558-10574, 2023 10 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37615303

ABSTRACT

Spontaneous racial categorization of other-race individuals provides a cognitive basis of racial ingroup biases in empathy and prosocial behavior. In two experiments, we investigated whether fostering a creativity mindset reduces racial ingroup biases in empathy and undermines spontaneous racial categorization of other-race faces. Before and after a creative mindset priming procedure that required the construction of novel objects using discreteness, we recorded electroencephalography signals to Asian and White faces with painful or neutral expressions from Chinese adults to assess neural activities underlying racial ingroup biases in empathy and spontaneous racial categorization of faces. We found that a frontal-central positive activity within 200 ms after face onset (P2) showed greater amplitudes to painful (vs. neutral) expressions of Asian compared with White faces and exhibited repetition suppression in response to White faces. These effects, however, were significantly reduced by creative mindset priming. Moreover, the creative mindset priming enhanced the P2 amplitudes to others' pain to a larger degree in participants who created more novel objects. The priming effects were not observed in control participants who copied objects constructed by others. Our findings suggest that creative mindsets may reduce racial ingroup biases in empathic neural responses by undermining spontaneous racial categorization of faces.


Subject(s)
Empathy , Racism , Adult , Humans , Asian People , Electroencephalography , Empathy/physiology , Pain/psychology , Racism/psychology , Asian , White
10.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(13): 8759-8772, 2023 06 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37143178

ABSTRACT

Outgroup aggression characterizes intergroup conflicts in human societies. Previous research on relationships between cultural traits and outgroup aggression behavior showed inconsistent results, leaving open questions regarding whether cultural traits predict individual differences in outgroup aggression and related neural underpinnings. We conducted 2 studies to address this issue by collecting self-construal scores, EEG signals in response to Asian and White faces with painful or neutral expressions, and decisions to apply electric shocks to other-race individuals in a context of interracial conflict. We found that interdependent self-construals were well explained by 2 subcomponents, including esteem for group (EG) and relational interdependence (RI), which are related to focus on group collectives and harmonious relationships, respectively. Moreover, EG was positively associated with the decisions to punish racial outgroup targets, whereas RI was negatively related to the decisions. These opposite relationships were mediated by neural representations of perceived race at 120-160 ms after face onset. Our findings highlight the multifaceted nature of interdependent self-construal and the key role of neural representations of race in mediating the relationships of different subcomponents of cultural traits with racial outgroup punishment decisions in a context of interracial conflict.


Subject(s)
Aggression , Punishment , Humans , Pain
11.
Macromol Rapid Commun ; 44(13): e2300060, 2023 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37014631

ABSTRACT

Currently, heat accumulation has seriously affected the stabilities and life of electronic devices. Polyimide (PI) film with high thermal conductivity coefficient (λ) has long been held up as an ideal solution for heat dissipation. Based on the thermal conduction mechanisms and classical thermal conduction models, this review presents design ideas of PI films with microscopically ordered liquid crystalline structures which are of great significance for breaking the limit of λ enhancement and describes the construction principles of thermal conduction network in high-λ filler strengthened PI films. Furthermore, the effects of filler type, thermal conduction paths, and interfacial thermal resistances on thermally conductive behavior of PI film are systematically reviewed. Meanwhile, this paper summarizes the reported research and provides an outlook on the future development of thermally conductive PI films. Finally, it is expected that this review will give some guidance to future studies in thermally conductive PI film.


Subject(s)
Electronics , Liquid Crystals , Electric Conductivity , Hot Temperature , Thermal Conductivity
12.
Polymers (Basel) ; 15(6)2023 Mar 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36987162

ABSTRACT

For the advantages of low cost, excellent thermal insulation, and sound absorption properties, the rigid isocyanate-based polyimide foam (RPIF) presents great application prospects as a building insulation material. However, its inflammability and the accompanying toxic fumes create huge safety hazard. In this paper, reactive phosphate-containing polyol (PPCP) is synthesized and employed with expandable graphite (EG) to obtain RPIF with excellent use safety. EG can be considered as an ideal partner for PPCP to weaken the drawbacks in toxic fume release. Limiting oxygen index (LOI), cone calorimeter test (CCT), and toxic gas results show that the combination of PPCP and EG can synergistically enhance flame retardancy and the use safety of RPIF owing to the unique structure of a dense char layer possessing a flame barrier and toxic gas adsorption effects. When EG and PPCP are simultaneously applied to the RPIF system, the higher EG dosage will bring higher positive synergistic effects in the use safety of RPIF. The most preferred ratio of EG and PPCP is 2:1 (RPIF-10-5) in this study; RPIF-10-5 shows the highest LOI, low CCT results and specific optical density of smoke, and low HCN concentration. This design and the findings are of great significance to improving the application of RPIF.

13.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 18(1)2023 02 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36807483

ABSTRACT

The debate regarding whether women are more empathetic than men has broad scientific, social and clinical implications. However, previous independent questionnaires and brain imaging studies that tested different samples reported inconsistent results regarding sex/gender differences in empathic ability. We conducted three studies to investigate sex/gender differences in empathic ability using large-sample questionnaires and electroencephalography (EEG) measures. We showed that the estimation of empathic ability using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index questionnaire showed higher rating scores in women than in men in all studies. However, our EEG measures of empathy, indexed by both phase-locked and non-phased-locked neural responses to others' painful (vs neutral) facial expressions, support a null hypothesis of the sex/gender difference in empathic ability. In addition, we showed evidence that priming social expectations of women and men's ability to share and care about others' feelings eliminated the sex/gender difference in questionnaire measures of empathic ability. Our large-sample EEG results challenge the notion of women's superiority in empathy that is built based on subjective questionnaire measures that are sensitive to social desirability. Our findings indicate that whether the notion of women's superiority in empathic ability reflects a biological/social difference between women and men or a gender-role stereotype remains an open question.


Subject(s)
Emotions , Empathy , Male , Humans , Female , Sex Factors , Emotions/physiology , Electroencephalography , Surveys and Questionnaires
14.
Cereb Cortex ; 33(4): 1328-1346, 2023 02 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35368080

ABSTRACT

Life and death are 2 fundamental concepts regarding existence of organisms. People often signify these concepts using symbols to facilitate communications, but how the brain learns and represents these symbols remains unclear. In the present study, we quantified behavioral and brain responses during learning associations between words ("life" or "death") with shapes as concrete referents. Behavioral responses to word-shape pairs showed an affirmative response bias to life-shape pairs but a denial response bias to death-shape pairs. Multimodal brain imaging results revealed that the right frontal and dorsal cingulate cortices monitored these response biases, respectively. Moreover, relative to unlearned shapes, life-related shapes induced increased alpha (9-14 Hz) oscillations in the right parietal cortex and precuneus, whereas death-related shapes enhanced beta (15-30 Hz) oscillations in the left parietal cortex, superior temporal sulcus, and precuneus. Our findings unraveled distinct neurocognitive mechanisms underlying learning and representations of concrete referents of life and death concepts.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Parietal Lobe , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Learning , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Brain Mapping
15.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 49(7): 1113-1129, 2023 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35611400

ABSTRACT

An astonishing cultural phenomenon is where, far away from or close to a city center, people in different societies localize cemeteries that function as both sites of memory of lost ones and symbols of mortality. Yet a psychological account of such differences in behavioral responses to symbols of mortality is lacking. Across five studies (N = 1,590), we tested a psychological model that religious afterlife beliefs decrease behavioral avoidance of symbols of mortality (BASM) by developing and validating a word-position task for quantifying BASM. We showed evidence that religious believers, including Christians, Muslims, Hindus, and Buddhists, exhibited decreased BASM relative to nonbelievers. We also provide evidence for a causal relationship between religious afterlife beliefs and reduced BASM. Our findings provide new insight into the functional role of religious afterlife beliefs in modulating human avoidance behavior in response to symbols of mortality.


Subject(s)
Attitude to Death , Buddhism , Christianity , Hinduism , Islam , Religion and Psychology , Symbolism , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult , Avoidance Learning , Buddhism/psychology , Cemeteries/statistics & numerical data , China/ethnology , Christianity/psychology , Cities/statistics & numerical data , Culture , Europe/ethnology , Hinduism/psychology , Islam/psychology , Models, Psychological , Self Concept , East Asian People/psychology
16.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 124(3): 483-520, 2023 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35816568

ABSTRACT

Social categorization of faces occurs along multiple dimensions such as race and gender and influences interpersonal interactions widely. Does social categorization of faces along different dimensions take place in parallel or give priority to some faces along a specific dimension? Across seven studies, we proposed and empirically tested a social task demand hypothesis that social categorization of a particular group of faces occurs in priority along one dimension (race or gender) in response to specific social task demands. Study 1 showed behavioral evidence that other-race faces are more densely clustered into one category, whereas same-race faces are more distantly separated into two (male and female) categories. Studies 2-4 showed electrophysiological evidence for earlier spontaneous racial categorization of other-race faces but earlier spontaneous gender categorization of same-race faces during an individual face recognition task. Studies 5-7 manipulated social task demands and provided electrophysiological evidence that early processes underlying racial or gender categorization of other-race or same-race faces were inhibited or facilitated by task demands of evaluating gender roles or group membership of perceived faces. Our findings indicate that the priority of racial or gender categorization of other-race or same-race faces varies dynamically to meet social task demands of managing intergroup and cross-sex interactions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Facial Recognition , Humans , Male , Female , Face
17.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 26(12): 1026-1028, 2022 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36243672

ABSTRACT

Default beliefs are implicit assumptions about the future existence of oneself, others, and the environment. They differ from individual and cultural beliefs in that they are held universally, implicitly, and with high confidence. What is the content of default beliefs? How are they represented in the brain? And what is their role in social decision-making?


Subject(s)
Brain , Decision Making , Humans
18.
Soc Neurosci ; 17(5): 441-461, 2022 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36064327

ABSTRACT

Financial incentives are commonly used to motivate behaviors. However, there is also evidence that incentives can impede the behavior they are supposed to foster, for example, documented by a decrease in blood donations if a financial incentive is offered. Based on these findings, previous studies assumed that prosocial motivation is shaped by incentives. However, so far, there is no direct evidence showing an interaction between financial incentives and a specific prosocial motive. Combining drift-diffusion modeling and fMRI, we investigated the effect of financial incentives on empathy, i.e., one of the key motives driving prosocial decisions. In the empathy-alone condition, participants made prosocial decisions based on empathy. In the empathy-bonus condition, they were offered a financial bonus for prosocial decisions, in addition to empathy induction. On average, the bonus enhanced the information accumulation in empathy-based decisions. On the neural level, this enhancement was related to the anterior insula, the same region that also correlated with empathy ratings. Moreover, the effect of the financial incentive on anterior insula activation was stronger the lower a person scored on empathy. These findings show that financial incentives enhance prosocial motivation in the absence of empathy.


Subject(s)
Empathy , Motivation , Adult , Humans , Female , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Social Behavior
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...