Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 12 de 12
Filter
1.
Dyslexia ; 13(4): 257-75, 2007 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17948879

ABSTRACT

Data from the 10-year follow-up of the 1970 British Births Survey were examined for associations between motor performance and dyslexia. Five tests of motor performance were used: (a) balancing on one leg, (b) throwing a ball in the air, clapping and catching it, (c) walking backwards, (d) sorting matches and (e) graphaesthesia (recognizing shapes drawn on the palm of the hand). These tests were given to 12 950 children aged between 10 and 11 years old. The cohort was divided into nine groups based on three levels of literacy achievement and three levels of possible indicators of dyslexia. The group with the most severe underachievement and most possible indicators (children most likely to be severely dyslexic) comprised about 2% of the total. Of this group, 35.3% failed one motor test and 16.4% failed more than one (51.7% in total), compared with 26.8% and 7.7% of normal achievers. The children had greater problems with balance than those in the other two severely underachieving groups but the effects were small. It is suggested that the use of a balance test only as a screener for dyslexia could result in a proportion of dyslexics being missed and that remedial motor training programmes for children with dyslexia should be offered only to those with co-occurring motor difficulties.


Subject(s)
Dyslexia/diagnosis , Dyslexia/epidemiology , Motor Skills Disorders/epidemiology , Child , Cohort Studies , Comorbidity , Educational Measurement , Humans , United Kingdom/epidemiology
2.
Early Hum Dev ; 19(2): 147-52, 1989 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2737105

ABSTRACT

An investigation to determine whether there is any relationship between extremes of fetal heart rate during labour and subsequent heart rate at the age of 10 was carried out using data from the 1970 cohort of British Births. In 11,000 nationally representative children it was found that low fetal heart rate (below 120 beats/min) was associated with a heart rate at age 10 which was significantly lower than in those children whose fetal heart rate had remained between 120 and 160 beats/min (P less than 0.01). This relationship could not be explained by fetal asphyxiation, maternal antenatal hypotension or the method of pain relief during labour. There was no equivalent relationship with high fetal heart rate during labour. This could imply that some fetuses with low heart rates are not exhibiting fetal distress but have an inherent tendency to relatively slow heart rates.


Subject(s)
Child , Fetal Heart/physiology , Heart Rate , Pulse , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Predictive Value of Tests
3.
Hum Biol ; 61(2): 213-25, 1989 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2767671

ABSTRACT

Multiple regression techniques were used to determine the most efficient combination of height, weight and body mass index in the prediction of systolic and diastolic blood pressures for a national sample of 13,723 10-year-old children. In every analysis an adjustment was made for the depth of sphygomomanometer cuff used when taking the blood pressure. The variables which together best predicted the systolic blood pressures in boys were weight, height and (height)2, and in girls weight/(height)2 and height. Diastolic blood pressures were predicted best by weight only for boys and by weight/(height)2 and height for girls. Once these factors had been taken into account there was no difference in blood pressures in those children for whom there were signs of puberty.


Subject(s)
Blood Pressure , Body Height , Body Weight , Child , Female , Humans , Hypertension/diagnosis , Hypertension/etiology , Male , Obesity/complications , Puberty , United Kingdom
4.
Ann Hum Biol ; 16(2): 109-19, 1989.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2729888

ABSTRACT

The idea of representing obesity or degree of malnutrition using a weight-for-height power index has existed for many years and several authors believe that such an index should be uncorrelated with height. Data from the 1958 National Child Development Study and the 1970 Child Health and Education Study have therefore been used to determine the values of the constant k which lead to the weight-for-height power index weight/[height]k being uncorrelated with height for specific age groups. Different values of k were needed both for the various age groups, and for the two sexes. For boys and girls respectively, the values of k needed at age 7 years were 2.02 and 2.12, at age 10 the values were 2.53 and 2.58, at age 11, 2.53 and 2.50 and at age 16, 2.42 and 1.71. Different values were also needed for West Indians and Asians and pubertal and pre-pubertal children. The relationships between this power index and other measurements of weight-for-height (including weight/height; weight/[height]2--the Quetelet index; weight/[height]3--the Ponderal index; relative weight for height, and standardized weight for height), the examining doctor's assessment of obesity and weight and height themselves were investigated for 10-year-old children born in 1970 to determine which of them could be thought of as best at estimating obesity. We found that there was little to choose between the index which was uncorrelated with height (using derived values of the power), and the Quetelet index.


Subject(s)
Body Height , Body Weight , Adolescent , Age Factors , Child , Cohort Studies , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Obesity/epidemiology , Predictive Value of Tests , United Kingdom
5.
Diabet Med ; 5(8): 739-46, 1988 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2975565

ABSTRACT

We have developed a psychometric test to assess psychosocial adaptation to diabetes in young people aged from 10 to 17 years. This includes subscales which describe emotional difficulty with, and attitude to, diabetes. The content of the scales was drawn from unstructured discussions with 27 young people with diabetes. The scales were piloted on two further unselected groups of adolescents (n = 50, n = 99) to examine their reliability, validity, and acceptability. Highly significant correlations were found between the results of the scales and measures of psychosocial function (p less than 0.001), parents' perceptions. Performance on the scale also correlated with a number of behaviour variables and differentiated those who were reported to be restricted by their diabetes (p less than 0.005) and those who had had a previous psychiatric referral (p less than 0.05). The scales appear reliable within the constraints of the small population examined.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 1/psychology , Social Adjustment , Adolescent , Age Factors , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Personality Inventory , Sex Factors , Social Class , Time Factors
6.
Child Care Health Dev ; 14(4): 275-91, 1988.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3168169

ABSTRACT

Differences in educational performance and behaviour at age 10 years between 2900 hospitalized and 11,000 non-hospitalized children in the 1970 British Births Cohort were described previously. In the present analysis associations were found between the length of time children spent in hospital before the age of 5 years and performance on vocabulary tests at age 5 and age 10. Children whose first admission occurred between 2 and 5 years of age were particularly likely to show this association. There was also a relationship between the length of preschool hospitalization and reading and mathematics attainment at age 10. It was the number of times a child was admitted to hospital before the age of 5, rather than the length of stay, which was associated with antisocial and anxious behaviour at age 5. The association was evident when the first admission occurred between 2 and 5 years of age and not before age 2. There was no association with such behaviour at age 10 once social and family factors and readmission between ages 5 and 10 were taken into account. There was a strong association between admission between ages 5 and 10 and behaviour scores at age 10. The results are interpreted as providing evidence for a need to develop the hospital educational service to ameliorate these associations and to review schemes which reduce the anxiety-provoking aspects of hospitalization for young children.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Child, Hospitalized/psychology , Learning Disabilities/psychology , Length of Stay , Achievement , Child , Child, Preschool , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Mathematics , Patient Readmission , Reading , Risk Factors , Vocabulary
7.
Arch Dis Child ; 63(4): 356-9, 1988 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3365003

ABSTRACT

A survey of health districts in England and Wales was carried out at the end of 1984 to ascertain among other things the range of current programmes for preschool vision screening. The response rate was 81.3%. Altogether 94% of districts reported screening for both reduced visual acuity and squint; two districts screened for neither. A great variety of different types of tests were in use and screening was carried out at a variety of different ages. A high proportion of districts were screening children for reduced visual acuity in infancy, although screening tests applicable at this age have not been shown to be effective. Districts screened for squint between one and four times. Collection of routine monitoring information by districts was poor.


Subject(s)
Child Health Services/supply & distribution , Mass Screening , Vision Tests , Child, Preschool , Humans , Infant , Schools , Strabismus/diagnosis , Surveys and Questionnaires , United Kingdom , Vision Disorders/prevention & control , Visual Acuity
8.
J Epidemiol Community Health ; 42(1): 17-23, 1988 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3418281

ABSTRACT

The prevalence and causes of partial sight and blindness (best corrected distant visual acuity of 6/24 or less) have been studied in a nationally representative sample of 15,000 10-year-old children. The prevalence of blindness (acuity less than 6/60) was between 3.4 and 4.0/10,000. All these children had been registered as blind; less than half were in schools for the blind, the remainder were all in other special schools. The prevalence of partial sight (acuity less than or equal to 6/24 greater than or equal to 6/60) was between 5.4 and 8.7/10,000; less than half of these children were in schools for the visually handicapped or partially sighted; most were in ordinary schools; half were neither registered as partially sighted nor ascertained as in need of special education for visual handicap. The most common cause of partial sight or blindness in this cohort was congenital cataract; the second most common was congenital nystagmus. The study identified a number of children whose best acuity on examination was 6/24 or less who had either no ophthalmological diagnosis or who had been diagnosed as suffering from a refractive error. These children have been included in the study because at the time of the survey they had either not been prescribed spectacles or they had spectacles which they were not wearing; the functional visual level of these children was therefore equivalent to that of those defined as partially sighted.


Subject(s)
Blindness/epidemiology , Blindness/etiology , Cataract/complications , Cataract/congenital , Cataract/epidemiology , Child , Eye Neoplasms/complications , Eye Neoplasms/epidemiology , Female , Humans , Male , Nystagmus, Pathologic/complications , Nystagmus, Pathologic/congenital , Nystagmus, Pathologic/epidemiology , Optic Atrophy/complications , Optic Atrophy/epidemiology , United Kingdom , Visual Acuity
9.
Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) ; 294(6584): 1386-8, 1987 May 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3109668

ABSTRACT

A questionnaire survey of all health districts in England and Wales was carried out at the end of 1984 to document screening programmes for identifying hearing loss in childhood. The response rate was 81.3%. All districts performed distraction testing, all but nine aiming at doing so at 7-9 months of age. All districts tested children's hearing at school, generally before 7 years of age. The number of times that children were screened both before school and at school varied considerably, from one to six times before school and one to six times at school. Few districts collected information that would allow them to make judgments about the efficiency of effectiveness of their screening programmes.


Subject(s)
Hearing Loss/epidemiology , Mass Screening , Age Factors , Audiometry , Child , England , Hearing Tests , Humans , Infant , School Health Services , Wales
10.
Ann Dyslexia ; 36(1): 103-17, 1986 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24243454

ABSTRACT

This paper is an interim report on a large-scale survey. The background to the authors' research into dyslexia is briefly outlined. Next comes an explication of the contrast between "anomaly" and "normal variation." Some details are then provided of a survey of 12,905 children, age ten, who were given a variety of educational and cognitive tests relevant to a diagnosis of dyslexia. It is shown that the resultant distributions of scores are incompatible with the hypothesis of normal variation. The counter-hypothesis, viz. that dyslexia involves some kind of anomaly, has, therefore, to that extent resisted refutation.

11.
Dev Med Child Neurol ; 27(4): 504-13, 1985 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-4029521

ABSTRACT

Children with visual defects who took part in a 10-year survey were compared with their peers on measures of intelligence, reading, mathematics and sporting ability. Results are consistent with earlier findings of increased intelligence among children with myopia and slightly reduced intelligence among children with amblyopia. Those with other visual defects had normal intelligence scores. Once intelligence had been taken into account, only children with mild hypermetropia were under-achieving at reading. Those with severe myopia were reading better than expected. None of the children could be shown to be over- or under-achieving at maths, any variation being due to intelligence. The mothers of children with visual defects perceived them to be less able at sport. Comparison of the performances of children with minor visual defects who had and had not been prescribed spectacles did not suggest any disadvantage for those without spectacles, with the possible exception of children with mild hypermetropia. It is concluded that the majority of visual defects do not affect children's learning, and that current indications for prescribing spectacles need to be validated.


Subject(s)
Achievement , Education, Special , Vision Disorders/therapy , Amblyopia/therapy , Blindness/therapy , Child, Preschool , Eyeglasses , Female , Humans , Hyperopia/therapy , Intelligence , Male , Myopia/therapy , Reading , Vision Disorders/diagnosis , Visual Acuity
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...