Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
1.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38909896

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Binge Eating Disorder (BED) is thought of as a disorder of cognitive control but evidence regarding its neurocognitive mechanisms is inconclusive. Key limitations in prior research are a lack of consistent separation between effects of BED and obesity, and a disregard for self-report evidence suggesting that neurocognitive alterations may emerge primarily in loss- or harm-avoidance contexts. METHODS: Addressing these gaps, this longitudinal study investigated behavioral flexibility and its underlying neuro-computational processes in reward-seeking and loss-avoidance contexts. Obese participants with BED (BED), without BED (OB), and healthy normal-weight participants (NW) (Ntotal=96) performed a probabilistic reversal learning task during functional imaging, with different blocks focused on obtaining wins or avoiding losses. They were reinvited for a 6-months follow-up. RESULTS: Analyses informed by computational models of reinforcement learning showed that unlike BED, OB performed worse in the win than the loss condition. Computationally, this was explained by differential learning sensitivities in the win vs loss conditions between groups. In the brain, this was echoed in differential neural learning signals in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) per condition. The differences were subtle, but scaled with BED symptoms, such that more severe BED symptoms were associated with increasing bias towards improved learning from wins vs losses. Across conditions, OB switched more between choice options than NW. This was reflected in diminished representation of choice certainty in the vmPFC. CONCLUSIONS: Our study highlights the importance of distinguishing between obesity with and without BED to identify unique neuro-computational alterations underlying different styles of maladaptive eating behavior.

2.
Appetite ; 195: 107179, 2024 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38145879

ABSTRACT

Computational models and neurophysiological data propose that a 'gating mechanism' coordinates distractor-resistant maintenance and flexible updating of working memory contents: While maintenance of information is mainly implemented in the prefrontal cortex, updating of information is signaled by phasic increases in dopamine in the striatum. Previous literature demonstrates structural and functional alterations in these brain areas, as well as differential dopamine transmission among individuals with obesity, suggesting potential impairments in these processes. To test this hypothesis, we conducted an observational case-control fMRI study, dividing participants into groups with and without obesity based on their BMI. We probed maintenance and updating of working memory contents using a modified delayed match to sample task and investigated the effects of SNPs related to the dopaminergic system. While the task elicited the anticipated brain responses, our findings revealed no evidence for group differences in these two processes, neither at the neural level nor behaviorally. However, depending on Taq1A genotype, which affects dopamine receptor density in the striatum, participants with obesity performed worse on the task. In conclusion, this study does not support the existence of overall obesity-related differences in working memory gating. Instead, we propose that potentially subtle alterations may manifest specifically in individuals with a 'vulnerable' genotype.


Subject(s)
Dopamine , Memory, Short-Term , Humans , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Brain Mapping , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology
3.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 60: 101226, 2023 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36905874

ABSTRACT

Precisely charting the maturation of core neurocognitive functions such as reinforcement learning (RL) and flexible adaptation to changing action-outcome contingencies is key for developmental neuroscience and adjacent fields like developmental psychiatry. However, research in this area is both sparse and conflicted, especially regarding potentially asymmetric development of learning for different motives (obtain wins vs avoid losses) and learning from valenced feedback (positive vs negative). In the current study, we investigated the development of RL from adolescence to adulthood, using a probabilistic reversal learning task modified to experimentally separate motivational context and feedback valence, in a sample of 95 healthy participants between 12 and 45. We show that adolescence is characterized by enhanced novelty seeking and response shifting especially after negative feedback, which leads to poorer returns when reward contingencies are stable. Computationally, this is accounted for by reduced impact of positive feedback on behavior. We also show, using fMRI, that activity of the medial frontopolar cortex reflecting choice probability is attenuated in adolescence. We argue that this can be interpreted as reflecting diminished confidence in upcoming choices. Interestingly, we find no age-related differences between learning in win and loss contexts.


Subject(s)
Reinforcement, Psychology , Reward , Humans , Adolescent , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Probability
4.
J Neurosci ; 43(12): 2178-2189, 2023 03 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36823039

ABSTRACT

Cognition and brain structure undergo significant maturation from adolescence into adulthood. Model-based (MB) control is known to increase across development, which is mediated by cognitive abilities. Here, we asked two questions unaddressed in previous developmental studies. First, what are the brain structural correlates of age-related increases in MB control? Second, how are age-related increases in MB control from adolescence to adulthood influenced by motivational context? A human developmental sample (n = 103; age, 12-50, male/female, 55:48) completed structural MRI and an established task to capture MB control. The task was modified with respect to outcome valence by including (1) reward and punishment blocks to manipulate the motivational context and (2) an additional choice test to assess learning from positive versus negative feedback. After replicating that an age-dependent increase in MB control is mediated by cognitive abilities, we demonstrate first-time evidence that gray matter density (GMD) in the parietal cortex mediates the increase of MB control with age. Although motivational context did not relate to age-related changes in MB control, learning from positive feedback improved with age. Meanwhile, negative feedback learning showed no age effects. We present a first report that an age-related increase in positive feedback learning was mediated by reduced GMD in the parietal, medial, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Our findings indicate that brain maturation, putatively reflected in lower GMD, in distinct and partially overlapping brain regions could lead to a more efficient brain organization and might thus be a key developmental step toward age-related increases in planning and value-based choice.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Changes in model-based decision-making are paralleled by extensive maturation in cognition and brain structure across development. Still, to date the neuroanatomical underpinnings of these changes remain unclear. Here, we demonstrate for the first time that parietal GMD mediates age-dependent increases in model-based control. Age-related increases in positive feedback learning were mediated by reduced GMD in the parietal, medial, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. A manipulation of motivational context did not have an impact on age-related changes in model-based control. These findings highlight that brain maturation in distinct and overlapping cortical regions constitutes a key developmental step toward improved value-based choices.


Subject(s)
Brain , Gray Matter , Male , Humans , Female , Adolescent , Child , Young Adult , Adult , Middle Aged , Gray Matter/diagnostic imaging , Feedback , Cognition , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Reward , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods
5.
Appetite ; 183: 106477, 2023 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36764221

ABSTRACT

Animal studies indicate that a high-fat/high-sugar diet (HFS) can change dopamine signal transmission in the brain, which could promote maladaptive behavior and decision-making. Such diet-induced changes may also explain observed alterations in the dopamine system in human obesity. Genetic variants that modulate dopamine transmission have been proposed to render some individuals more prone to potential effects of HFS. The objective of this study was to investigate the association of HFS with dopamine-dependent cognition in humans and how genetic variations might modulate this potential association. Using a questionnaire assessing the self-reported consumption of high-fat/high-sugar foods, we investigated the association with diet by recruiting healthy young men that fall into the lower or upper end of that questionnaire (low fat/sugar group: LFS, n = 45; high fat/sugar group: HFS, n = 41) and explored the interaction of fat and sugar consumption with COMT Val158Met and Taq1A genotype. During functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning, male participants performed a working memory (WM) task that probes distractor-resistance and updating of WM representations. Logistic and linear regression models revealed no significant difference in WM performance between the two diet groups, nor an interaction with COMT Val158Met or Taq1A genotype. Neural activation in task-related brain areas also did not differ between diet groups. Independent of diet group, higher BMI was associated with lower overall accuracy on the WM task. This cross-sectional study does not provide evidence for diet-related differences in WM stability and flexibility in men, nor for a predisposition of COMT Val158Met or Taq1A genotype to the hypothesized detrimental effects of an HFS diet. Previously reported associations of BMI with WM seem to be independent of HFS intake in our male study sample.


Subject(s)
Catechol O-Methyltransferase , Dopamine , Humans , Male , Self Report , Cross-Sectional Studies , Catechol O-Methyltransferase/genetics , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Genotype , Diet, Fat-Restricted , Sugars
6.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev ; 129: 330-350, 2021 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34280427

ABSTRACT

Recurring episodes of excessive food intake in binge eating disorder can be understood through the lens of behavioral control systems: patients repeat maladaptive behaviors against their explicit intent. Self-report measures show enhanced impulsivity and compulsivity in binge eating (BE) but are agnostic as to the processes that might lead to impulsive and compulsive behavior in the moment. Task-based neurocognitive investigations can tap into those processes. In this systematic review, we synthesize neurocognitive research on behavioral impulsivity and compulsivity in BE in humans and animals, published between 2010-2020. Findings on impulsivity are heterogeneous. Findings on compulsivity are sparse but comparatively consistent, indicating an imbalance of goal-directed and habitual control as well as deficits in reversal learning. We urge researchers to address heterogeneity related to mood states and the temporal dynamics of symptoms, to systematically differentiate contributions of body weight and BE, and to ascertain the validity and reliability of tasks. Moreover, we propose to further scrutinize the compulsivity findings to unravel the computational mechanisms of a potential reinforcement learning deficit.


Subject(s)
Binge-Eating Disorder , Animals , Compulsive Behavior , Humans , Impulsive Behavior , Motivation , Reproducibility of Results
7.
Front Neurosci ; 15: 632922, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33828446

ABSTRACT

Sustained attention is the ability to continually concentrate on task-relevant information, even in the presence of distraction. Understanding the neural mechanisms underlying this ability is critical for comprehending attentional processes as well as neuropsychiatric disorders characterized by attentional deficits, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). In this study, we aimed to investigate how trait-like critical oscillations during rest relate to the P300 evoked potential-a biomarker commonly used to assess attentional deficits. We measured long-range temporal correlations (LRTC) in resting-state EEG oscillations as index for criticality of the signal. In addition, the attentional performance of the subjects was assessed as reaction time variability (RTV) in a continuous performance task following an oddball paradigm. P300 amplitude and latencies were obtained from EEG recordings during this task. We found that, after controlling for individual variability in task performance, LRTC were positively associated with P300 amplitudes but not latencies. In line with previous findings, good performance in the sustained attention task was related to higher P300 amplitudes and earlier peak latencies. Unexpectedly, we observed a positive relationship between LRTC in ongoing oscillations during rest and RTV, indicating that greater criticality in brain oscillations during rest relates to worse task performance. In summary, our results show that resting-state neuronal activity, which operates near a critical state, relates to the generation of higher P300 amplitudes. Brain dynamics close to criticality potentially foster a computationally advantageous state which promotes the ability to generate higher event-related potential (ERP) amplitudes.

8.
Health Qual Life Outcomes ; 14: 97, 2016 Jun 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27349641

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Health-related quality of life (HRQoL) is widely used as an outcome measure in the evaluation of treatment interventions in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD). In order to address challenges associated with existing fixed-length measures (e.g., too long to be used routinely, too short to ensure both content validity and reliability), a COPD-specific item bank (COPD-SIB) was developed. METHODS: Items were selected based on literature review and interviews with Dutch COPD patients, with a strong focus on both content validity and item comprehension. The psychometric quality of the item bank was evaluated using Mokken Scale Analysis and parametric Item Response Theory, using data of 666 COPD patients. RESULTS: The final item bank contains 46 items that form a strong scale, tapping into eight important themes that were identified based on literature review and patient interviews: Coping with disease/symptoms, adaptability; Autonomy; Anxiety about the course/end-state of the disease, hopelessness; Positive psychological functioning; Situations triggering or enhancing breathing problems; Symptoms; Activity; Impact. CONCLUSIONS: The 46-item COPD-SIB has good psychometric properties and content validity. Items are available in Dutch and English. The COPD-SIB can be used as a stand-alone instrument, or to inform computerised adaptive testing.


Subject(s)
Outcome Assessment, Health Care/methods , Pulmonary Disease, Chronic Obstructive/psychology , Quality of Life , Adaptation, Psychological , Adult , Aged , Anxiety , Female , Health Status , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Netherlands , Outcome Assessment, Health Care/standards , Psychometrics/methods , Psychometrics/standards , Reproducibility of Results , Surveys and Questionnaires
9.
J Antimicrob Chemother ; 57(3): 527-35, 2006 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16431856

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Although a paediatric dosage has not been established, caspofungin is occasionally used in paediatric patients. We conducted a multicentre retrospective survey to obtain data on immunocompromised paediatric patients considered to require caspofungin therapy. METHODS: The survey identified 64 patients (median age: 11.5 years; 25 females, 39 males) with haematological malignancies (48), marrow failure (9), solid tumours (3), haematological disorders (2) and congenital immunodeficiency (2) who received caspofungin for proven (17), probable (14) and possible (17) invasive fungal infections or empirically (16). Caspofungin was administered until intolerance or maximum efficacy at dosages individually determined by the responsible physician for refractory infection (38), intolerance of other agents (10) or as best therapeutic option (16). RESULTS: The 64 patients received caspofungin for a median of 37 days (range 3-218) as single agent (20) or in combination (44). The median daily maintenance dosage was 1.07 mg/kg (95% CI 1.09-1.35; range 0.40-2.92) or 34.3 mg/m2 (95% CI 32.3-37.3; range 16.3-57.5). In none of the patients was therapy discontinued due to adverse events (AEs). Clinical AEs were mild to moderate and observed in 34 patients (53.1%). While mean glutamate pyruvate transaminase and glutamate oxalate transaminase values were slightly (P < 0.005) higher at the end of treatment (EOT), serum bilirubin, alkaline phosphatase and creatinine values were not different from baseline. Complete responses, partial responses or stabilization were observed in 5/7/3 of 17 patients with proven, in 3/4/3 of 14 patients with probable and in 7/6/1 of 15 evaluable patients with possible invasive infections. Thirteen of 16 patients on empirical therapy completed without breakthrough infection. Overall survival was 75% at the EOT and 70% at 3 months post-EOT, respectively. CONCLUSIONS: Caspofungin displayed favourable safety and tolerance and may have useful antifungal efficacy in severely immunocompromised paediatric patients.


Subject(s)
Antifungal Agents/therapeutic use , Peptides, Cyclic/therapeutic use , Adolescent , Antifungal Agents/adverse effects , Caspofungin , Child , Child, Preschool , Echinocandins , Female , Humans , Immunocompromised Host , Infant , Lipopeptides , Male , Mycoses/drug therapy , Peptides, Cyclic/adverse effects , Retrospective Studies
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...