Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 29
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Percept Psychophys ; 63(5): 847-61, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11521851

ABSTRACT

The correspondence problem arises in motion perception when more than one motion path is possible for discontinuously presented visual elements. Ullman's (1979) "minimal mapping" solution to the correspondence problem, for which costs are assigned to competing motion paths on the basis of element affinities (e.g., greater affinity for elements that are closer together), is distinguished from a solution based on the differential activation of directionally selective motion detectors. The differential activation account was supported by evidence that path length affects detector activation in a paradignm for which motion correspondence is not a factor. Effects on detector activation in this paradigm also were the basis for the successful prediction of path luminance effects on solutions to the motion correspondence problem. Finally, the differential activation account was distinguished from minimal mapping theory by an experiment showing that the perception of an element moving simultaneously in two directions does not depend on whether the two motions are matched in path-length determined affinity; it is sufficient that the activation of detectors responding to each of the two motion directions is above the threshold level required for the motions to be perceived. Implications of the differential activation solution are discussed for the stability of perceived motions once they are established, and the adaptation of perceived and unperceived motions.


Subject(s)
Attention , Motion Perception , Problem Solving , Adult , Distance Perception , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds
2.
Percept Psychophys ; 61(6): 1055-65, 1999 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10497427

ABSTRACT

Evidence for bistability in the perception of motion and stationarity was obtained for a displaced dot-figure embedded in a background of randomly moving noise dots. In the temporal symmetry condition, the figure was presented for the same duration in its two locations; either figure motion or random motion was perceived, depending on the number of noise dots. In the temporal asymmetry condition, the figure was presented for different durations in its two locations; figure motion, a single, stationary figure in a fixed position, or random noise was perceived, again depending on the number of noise dots. Competition between these percepts was established by an analysis of switching rates and by an experiment demonstrating the presence of hysteresis as noise levels were gradually increased and decreased across the figure-motion and figure-stationarity boundaries. This evidence for bistability in the perception of figure motion and figure stationarity (one or the other, but not both, was perceived for the same stimulus) suggested the presence of strong inhibitory competition between motion- and position-detecting mechanisms.


Subject(s)
Attention , Field Dependence-Independence , Motion Perception , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Orientation , Perceptual Masking , Psychophysics , Reaction Time
3.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 70(3): 187-206, 1998 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9742179

ABSTRACT

It is well established that children use study behaviors such as card sorting, category naming, and item-by-item rehearsal to assist subsequent word recall. In this article, we provide evidence that these behaviors are organized into coherent temporal patterns. Fourier analyses of individual behaviors over a sequence of five consecutive study/recall trials indicated that sorting was synchronized with the start of each trial, whereas rehearsal tended to occur later in each trial. Fourier analyses of pairs of behaviors indicated that sorting and category naming, both concerned with categorization of the to-be-remembered words, co-occurred early in each trial at a greater rate than expected based on their individual frequencies of occurrence (i.e., they were used cooperatively). In contrast, verbal rehearsal of individual words co-occurred with both sorting and category naming at a lesser rate than would be expected based on their individual frequencies of occurrence. The results thus point to a global strategy in which children learn the items' categories before they learn them individually. There was little apparent qualitative difference in temporal organization for second- and fourth-grade children. However, sorting early in each trial was more pronounced for children with better word recall (regardless of grade), and the suppressed co-occurrence of rehearsal with sorting and category naming (i.e., keeping category learning temporally separate from item learning) was more pronounced for the fourth-grade children.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Child , Child Development/physiology , Female , Humans , Learning/physiology , Male , Mental Recall/physiology , Time Factors
4.
Vision Res ; 38(23): 3743-58, 1998 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9893804

ABSTRACT

Prior to the presentation of a test stimulus, subjects' attentional state was either narrowly focused on a particular location or broadly spread over a large spatial region. In previous studies, it was found that broadly spread attention enhances the sensitivity of relatively large spatial filters (increasing the perceiver's spatial scale), thereby diminishing spatial resolution and enhancing sensitivity to global stimulus structure. In this study it is shown that attentional spread also affects the self-organization of unidirectional versus oscillatory motion patterns for the directionally ambiguous, counterphase presentation of rows of evenly-spaced visual elements (lines segments; dots); i.e. qualitatively different motion patterns can be formed for the same stimulus at different spatial scales. Although the degree to which attention is spread along a spatial axis can be controlled by the perceiver, the effects of spread attention are not limited to a single axis. These results, as well as previously observed effects of attentional spread on spatial resolution, are accounted for by a neural model involving large, foveally-centered receptive fields with co-operatively interacting subunits (probably at the level of MST or higher).


Subject(s)
Attention , Motion Perception/physiology , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Light , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
5.
Percept Psychophys ; 59(7): 1077-88, 1997 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9360480

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have indicated that the formation of coherent patterns for multielement motion displays depends on global cooperative interactions among large ensembles of spatially distributed motion detectors. These interactions enhance certain motion directions and suppress others. It is reported here that perceiving one element moving between two nearby locations likewise is subject to cooperative influences (possibly facilitating and inhibiting interactions within a local ensemble of overlapping detectors). Thresholds depending on luminance contrast were measured for a generalized single-element apparent-motion stimulus, and evidence for spontaneous switching and hysteresis effects indicated that motion perception near the 50% threshold was bistable. That is, for conditions in which motion and nonmotion were perceived half the time, the two percepts were distinct; when one was perceived, it clearly was discriminable from the other. These results indicated that (1) single-element apparent-motion thresholds depended on the immediately preceding state of the ensemble of motion detectors responding to the stimulus, and (2) the stimulus activation of individual motion detectors always might be influenced by recurrent, cooperative interactions resulting from the detectors' being embedded within interconnected ensembles.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Adult , Differential Threshold , Female , Humans , Male
6.
Vision Res ; 37(11): 1499-510, 1997 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9205711

ABSTRACT

The effects of attentional spread were studied by having subjects detect a luminance increment along a row of evenly spaced dots. The increment could occur for the central, fixated dot (Narrow Attention) or for either the fixation dot or one of the four dots to its left or right (Broad Attention). Narrow Attention enhanced the detection of luminance increments for the fixated dot, and also enhanced spatial resolution near the fixation dot for judgments of vernier alignment and separation. This indicated that the sensitivity of small spatial filters in the fovea was increased more by narrowly focused than broadly spread attention. Effects of attentional spread on spatial resolution were not obtained for judgments of the separation between two peripherally located targets, perhaps because of their dependence on eccentricity (position) rather than separation.


Subject(s)
Attention , Space Perception/physiology , Differential Threshold , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Lighting , Male , Psychometrics
7.
Percept Psychophys ; 59(4): 509-22, 1997 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9158326

ABSTRACT

Spontaneous perceptual change was studied by measuring the probabilities of the first two spontaneous pattern switches as a function of time following the onset of a bistable apparent quartet for which either horizontal or vertical motion is perceived. Contrary to the classical satiation hypothesis (Köhler & Wallach, 1944), differential time-dependent adaptation of the perceived compared with the unperceived motion directions was not necessary to account for the first spontaneous switch. In addition, adaptation of the perceived motion accompanied by recovery from adaptation of the unperceived motion was not necessary to account for the increased probability of the second spontaneous switch. It was concluded that regardless of possible adaptation effects, stochastic fluctuations are necessary for the actual reversal of activation levels that produces the spontaneous switch. When the difference in detector activation is reduced by differential adaptation of competing motion detectors (or by the occurrence of a prior spontaneous pattern change), smaller stochastic fluctuations are sufficient to reverse the relative activation of competing detectors. Thus, adaptation can increase the probability of spontaneous switches without directly causing them.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular , Attention , Motion Perception , Optical Illusions , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Probability Learning , Psychomotor Performance , Psychophysics , Reaction Time , Stochastic Processes
8.
Vision Res ; 36(20): 3311-23, 1996 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8944289

ABSTRACT

Adaptation was studied in a paradigm in which the adapting stimulus was a variably biased version of a bistable apparent motion stimulus, a motion quartet, and the post-adaptation test stimulus was a "neutral" motion quartet. Either horizontal or vertical motion was perceived, never both at the same time. When only one of these was perceived during the entire adaptation phase of a trial, and the perceived motion was highly stable, adaptation effects were greater for the perceived than the unperceived motion directions (i.e., adaptation was selective to the perceived motion). However, when the perceived motion during adaptation was relatively unstable (i.e., when the perceived motion was more likely to spontaneously change directions), similar levels of adaptation were obtained for perceived as well as unperceived, but possible motion directions. Thus, adaptation occurs prior to the determination of which of the competing motion directions will be perceived. The relationship between the stability of the adapting percept and the selectivity of adaptation is explained in terms of differences in the activation of mutually inhibitory horizontal and vertical motion detectors.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Humans , Male , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors
9.
Perception ; 25(8): 887-900, 1996.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8938003

ABSTRACT

Evidence is reported that stationarity rather than motion can be perceived for displaced stimuli, not because of insufficient motion energy for the stimulus to activate individual motion detectors, but because of cooperative interactions that actively suppress the perception of motion. A long row of evenly spaced dots was presented in counterphase; the dots presented during each 180 ms frame were located midway between the dots presented during the previous frame. When either a blank interval as brief as 15 ms was inserted between successive frames or the luminance polarity of the dots was reversed on successive frames, the unidirectional motion pattern perceived for small interdot distances (small displacements) was replaced by the perception of stationarity. However, when under the same conditions a single dot was displaced over the same small distances, motion rather than stationarity was perceived. The contrasting results for the long row of displaced dots and the single displaced dot indicated that when the activation of motion detectors is weakened (by nonzero interframe intervals and/or the reversal of luminance polarity), the perception of motion can be actively suppressed by the collective effects of inhibitory interactions among the large ensemble of detectors that is activated by the long row of dots.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Field Dependence-Independence , Humans , Lighting , Models, Neurological , Photoreceptor Cells/physiology
10.
Vision Res ; 35(5): 635-46, 1995 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7900302

ABSTRACT

The effects of sustained and transient responses to inducing lines on the perceived position of nearby test lines were measured as constant errors in the vernier alignment of the test lines. The sustained response to the continued presence of the inducing lines had no effect on the perceived position of the test lines for very small inducing/test-line distances, and a repulsive effect for larger distances. Transient responses to the onset of the inducing lines had attractive effects for small distances, whereas transient responses to the offset of the inducing lines had repulsive effects for all distances tested. Attraction and repulsion effects on perceived position are accounted for by facilitating and inhibiting interactions that influence the relative sensitivities of an ensemble of position-selective detecting units.


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Visual Acuity , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Models, Neurological , Neural Inhibition , Time Factors
11.
Vision Res ; 34(14): 1843-61, 1994 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7941387

ABSTRACT

A long row of evenly spaced dots is displaced on successive frames by half the distance between the dots. Although these stimuli are directionally ambiguous, spatially and temporally coherent unidirectional and oscillatory motion patterns are perceived as a result of the temporal persistence of competing in-phase and anti-phase directional biases, respectively. The perceiver's spatial scale is critical is determining whether dots are near enough to favor an in-phase bias or far enough apart to favor an anti-phase bias. The results are explained by a differential-gradient model of cooperative interaction, which specifies that the strength of facilitating (excitatory) interactions among motion detectors with similar directional selectivity falls off with distance at a greater rate than the strength of competing inhibiting interactions.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Distance Perception/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Models, Biological , Neural Inhibition/physiology , Observer Variation , Time Factors
12.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 19(1): 63-80, 1993 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8440989

ABSTRACT

In a paradigm for which 2 distinct patterns are perceived for the same stimulus, perceptual hysteresis (persistence of a percept despite parameter change to values favoring the alternative pattern) and temporal stability (persistence despite intrinsic propensities toward spontaneous change) are interdependent. Greater persistence during parameter change reduces temporal stability, slowing the rate of parameter change reduces hysteresis by increasing opportunity for spontaneous change, and increasing temporal stability (by enlarging the stimulus) increases hysteresis. Hysteresis results in the perception of parametrically disfavored patterns; a parameter can influence a percept without specifying it. The visual system thus exhibits time-dependent behavior analogous to dynamical behavior observed in other systems, both physical and biological, for which there is competition among alternative patterns that vary in relative stability.


Subject(s)
Attention , Motion Perception , Optical Illusions , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Psychophysics
13.
Vision Res ; 32(6): 1043-54, 1992 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1509695

ABSTRACT

When common movement is superimposed on relative movement (changes in separation between two dots), relative movement thresholds increase nonlinearly as a function of initial dot separation. For large separation (greater than 2.0 deg), thresholds increase gradually with increased separation. It is shown that this reflects judgments based on perceived relative motion. For small separations (less than 2.0 deg), thresholds increase sharply with increased separation. It is shown that this reflects judgments based on perceived changes in relative position. Evidence is presented that superimposed common movement reduces sensitivity to relative movement by reducing sensitivity to relative motion. This provides a "window", in the range of small dot separations, for relative movement judgements to be based on the perception of changes in relative position, even though motion is perceived for individual dots.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Humans , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
14.
Mem Cognit ; 17(4): 490-502, 1989 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2761407

ABSTRACT

When presented a series of patterns inside a frame, subjects unintentionally retained information concerning the frame-relative location of the elements composing the patterns; they could use this superficial information to estimate the frequency with which the elements occurred at various locations within the frame. There were two aspects of the results that supported the hypothesis that this superficial element-location information was abstractively encoded and, therefore, retrievable independent of the patterns comprising the elements: (1) Correlations between actual and estimated frequency remained significant after the effects of pattern recall were partialled out, and (2) correlations were enhanced by assuming that the frequency estimate for each location was affected by the imprecise coding of position for elements falling in surrounding locations. Additional experiments indicated that an orienting task emphasizing pattern recall resulted in very inaccurate estimation of element-location frequency, and intentional instructions improved the precision of position coding for individual elements.


Subject(s)
Attention , Concept Formation , Form Perception , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Humans , Mental Recall
15.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 14(1): 75-84, 1988 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2963895

ABSTRACT

A series of experiments provided evidence that the representational structure of categories comprising dot patterns is based on pattern parts and pattern configuration rather than on pattern elements. We found that similarity judgments and postacquisition classification data could not be explained in terms of element-level perceptual units, even for categories of dot patterns with seven of their eight dots in the exact same relative location. The importance of higher order perceptual units was indicated by evidence that the long-term retention of information specific to previously learned category exemplars, which is typical of natural objects, can also be obtained for artificial dot patterns, providing their structure reflects the perceptual characteristics identified in Tversky and Hemenway's (1984) study of natural objects: Members of the same category had to be perceptually distinctive at the level of pattern configuration and perceptually similar at the level of pattern parts. The level of within-category similarity for a set of categories (relative to between-categories similarity) did not predict whether item-specific information would be retained; long-term retention appears to require both within-category similarity and dissimilarity, but at different levels of perceptual structure.


Subject(s)
Attention , Discrimination Learning , Form Perception , Memory , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Retention, Psychology , Concept Formation , Humans , Mental Recall , Paired-Associate Learning
18.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 12(2): 232-40, 1986 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2939180

ABSTRACT

Subjects' knowledge of how often various events occur was used to assess the retention of memory units for word-like strings of letters. A series of strings was presented at one of three exposure durations. Within the series, the frequencies of occurrence of different strings and of the letters composing the strings were varied orthogonally. At relatively long exposure durations, subjects could discriminate the frequency of occurrence for both strings and their constituent letters. The formation of global-level (string) memory units was indicated by judgments of string frequency being unaffected by either the frequencies of their component letters or experimental conditions (brief exposures) that prohibited accurate judgment of letter frequency. Although judgments of letter frequency were sometimes biased by the frequency of the strings containing the letters, the success with which the judgments discriminated different levels of letter frequency did not depend on the activation of string-level memory units. Furthermore, subjects' frequency judgments for letters were not predictable from their recall of the strings containing the letters. These results, which could not be explained by Tversky and Kahneman's (1973) "availability heuristic," provided evidence for the formation of element-level (letter) memory units. A converging experiment established that element-level frequency information could be abstracted from words as well as nonwords, and further, that this information was stored in long-term memory.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological , Memory , Reading , Retention, Psychology , Visual Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Humans , Judgment , Models, Psychological , Psychophysics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...