Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 16 de 16
Filter
1.
J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol ; 51(2): 183-194, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33945361

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: This study tested whether the longitudinal effects of mothers' and fathers' corporal punishment on children's aggression would vary depending on the context of the parent-child relationship (gender of the parent-child dyad, other parenting practices). METHOD: Participants were 325 children (Mage = 7.44 years, SD = .50) and their parents from Estonia who participated in a longitudinal study. Both mothers and fathers filled out measures of parenting practices in Grade 1 (corporal punishment, warmth, behavioral and psychological control). Teachers rated children's aggressive behavior in Grade 1 and 3. RESULTS: We found that the effects of both mothers' and fathers' corporal punishment on changes in children's aggressive behavior were conditional. In contexts that were characterized by a lack of parental warmth or overuse of behavioral control, corporal punishment was associated with increases in aggression; in parent-child relationships with high levels of parental affection or low levels of behavioral control, it was unrelated to, or associated with, decreases in aggression. Some of these conditional effects were specific to the gender of the parent and the child. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings suggest that we should try to better understand the complexities of different parent-child relationships that magnify vs. minimize the negative consequences of corporal punishment.


Subject(s)
Parenting , Punishment , Aggression/psychology , Child , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Parent-Child Relations , Parenting/psychology , Punishment/psychology
2.
J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol ; 47(5): 745-756, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27096740

ABSTRACT

The goal of the current study was to validate a new measure of the friendship self-disclosure process that assesses the likelihood of disclosing a negative peer experience and expectations for friends' responses to disclosure (EFRD) of this experience. Participants for Study 1 were 572 adolescents (age M = 14.82; 53% female; 66% Caucasian) from a public school sample who completed the self-disclosure survey and a measures of depressive symptoms at one time point. Participants of Study 2 comprised 180 obese adolescents (age M = 12.78; 67% female; 58% African American) from an urban children's hospital. The obese sample completed the self-disclosure survey, as well as measures of friendship quality, peer victimization, and depressive symptoms at two time points, 6 months apart. For both studies, 3 dimensions of EFRD were examined: protection, blame, and negative responses. Each EFRD dimension was replicated across 2 samples, over time, and had good interitem reliability as well as convergent and discriminant validity. In Study 2, high rates of expected negativity (only for boys) and blame (for boys and girls) predicted increases in depressive symptoms. In addition, victimization led to increases in depression for obese adolescents who expected little in the way of protective responses from their friends. In contrast, changes in depression were not predictable from victimization for those who expected friends to use protective responses. EFRD are clearly important mechanisms in the self-disclosure process that may serve to protect against changes in adjustment in response to negative peer experiences, such as peer victimization.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Friends/psychology , Obesity/psychology , Peer Group , Schools/standards , Self Disclosure , Adolescent , Bullying/psychology , Child , Crime Victims/psychology , Depression/diagnosis , Depression/epidemiology , Depression/psychology , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Obesity/diagnosis , Obesity/epidemiology , Reproducibility of Results , Surveys and Questionnaires
3.
Cogn Emot ; 30(5): 1008-16, 2016 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26042460

ABSTRACT

The effects of anger and effortful control on aggressogenic thought-behaviour associations were investigated among a total of 311 Finnish fifth and sixth graders (mean age = 11.9 years). Self-reported aggressive cognitions (i.e., normative- and self-efficacy beliefs about aggression) were expected to be associated with higher peer-reported aggressive behaviour. Teacher reported anger and effortful control were hypothesised, and found, to moderate the effects of aggressive cognitions on aggression, such that the effects were strongest for children who were high in anger and low in effortful control, as compared to other conditions. Furthermore, under the conditions of high anger and high effortful control, self-efficacy was negatively related to aggression. Thus, aggression is a result of a complex, hierarchically organised motivational system, being jointly influenced by aggressive cognitions, anger and effortful control. The findings support the importance of examining cognitive and emotional structures jointly when predicting children's aggressive behaviour.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Anger , Child Behavior/psychology , Self-Control/psychology , Thinking , Child , Female , Finland , Humans , Male
4.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 124: 112-23, 2014 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24780299

ABSTRACT

This study was designed to test whether aggression toward easy or challenging targets is more likely to be associated with popularity. More specifically, we tested two alternative hypotheses with a sample of 224 adolescents (12- and 13-year-olds): (a) whether aggression toward highly disliked peers is associated with popularity (the easy target hypothesis) or (b) whether aggression toward highly liked peers is associated with popularity (the challenging target hypothesis). Support was found only for the challenging target hypothesis. In particular, our results indicate that aggressiveness toward peers who are liked by many others has social benefits in the form of greater popularity (particularly for highly preferred adolescents) without social costs (i.e., is unrelated to social preference). In contrast, aggressiveness toward peers who are disliked by many others is associated with lower social preference but bears no association with popularity. These results highlight the importance of studying contextualized aggression in order to understand the conditions under which aggression is most, and least, likely to be associated with social power and dominance.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Psychological Distance , Social Identification , Adolescent , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Peer Group , Psychology, Adolescent , Psychology, Child
5.
Dev Psychol ; 50(3): 941-6, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23895166

ABSTRACT

In this short-term longitudinal study, we systematically examined the distinctiveness of guilt- and shame-proneness in early adolescents (N = 395, mean age = 11.8 years) in terms of differential relations with peer reported prosocial behavior, withdrawal, and aggression. Results from structural equation modeling indicated that guilt-proneness concurrently predicted more aggressive and less prosocial behavior as well as subsequent increases in prosocial behavior. Shame-proneness predicted subsequent decreases in prosocial behavior. Although girls reported a greater proneness to experience guilt and shame than boys, the associations between the two dispositional emotions and social behaviors were found to be similar across time and gender.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Guilt , Moral Development , Social Behavior , Adolescent , Developmental Disabilities , Female , Finland , Humans , Male , Predictive Value of Tests , Self Report , Statistics as Topic , Time Factors
6.
J Sch Psychol ; 51(3): 421-34, 2013 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23816233

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to simultaneously investigate student-, classroom-, and school-level risk factors for victimization. Both peer nominations and students' self-reports of victimization were utilized. The sample consisted of 6731 Finnish elementary school students (3386 girls and 3345 boys) nested in 358 classrooms in 74 schools. The participants were from Grades 3, 4, and 5 (mean age 11years). The results of multilevel analyses indicated that there was considerable variability in, and distinctive risk factors associated with, both peer- and self-reported victimization at all the three levels investigated. Social anxiety and peer rejection synergistically predicted victimization at the student level. At the classroom level, negative social outcome expectations of defending the victim were associated with an increased risk of a student being bullied. Victimization was also common in classrooms and schools where students perceived their teachers to have less disapproving attitudes toward bullying. Furthermore, the effects of the student-level predictors were found to vary across classrooms, and classroom size moderated the effects of social anxiety and peer rejection on victimization. By identifying the risk factors at the multiple levels, and looking into cross-level interactions among these factors, research can help to target interventions at the key ecological factors contributing to victimization, making it possible to maximize the effectiveness of interventions.


Subject(s)
Bullying/psychology , Crime Victims/psychology , Schools , Social Environment , Students/psychology , Adolescent , Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Attitude , Child , Child Behavior/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Peer Group , Risk Factors
7.
J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol ; 42(4): 442-53, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23384048

ABSTRACT

We examined the occurrence of electronic-only, traditional-only, and traditional and electronic bullying, and the antecedents and consequences of electronic versus traditional victimization. A large data set including 17,625 students from elementary (Grades 3-5) and middle school (Grades 7-8) was utilized to examine the prevalence of students with diverse victimization profiles. A longitudinal subsample of 7,850 students was used to test hypotheses regarding the antecedents and consequences of electronic victimization when occurring in isolation from traditional forms versus accompanied by them. According to the main findings, (a) the victims of electronic bullying were in most cases bullied in traditional ways as well; (b) being a target of electronic-only victimization was not predicted by either intrapersonal (depression) or interpersonal (low social acceptance) risk factors; and (c) electronic victimization, when occurring in isolation from traditional victimization, did not contribute to increases in depression over time. Electronic victimization is rare, and is almost always accompanied by traditional victimization. It leads to increases in depression only when combined with traditional victimization. Rather than shifting attention from traditional to electronic victimization, educators should continue their efforts on reducing victimization in general.


Subject(s)
Bullying/psychology , Crime Victims/psychology , Interpersonal Relations , Peer Group , Students/psychology , Adolescent , Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Child , Child Behavior/psychology , Crime Victims/statistics & numerical data , Female , Humans , Internet , Male , Prevalence , Risk Factors , Schools , Surveys and Questionnaires
8.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 114(1): 131-45, 2013 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22784854

ABSTRACT

Children and adolescents encounter different hurtful experiences in school settings. How these events are processed (e.g., whether they think that the transgressor was hostile) is likely to depend on the relationship with the transgressor. In this study, we examined how adolescents (58 girls and 35 boys, mean age=14.03 years, SD=0.60) dealt with the hurt caused by someone they liked or disliked. Our findings show that the hurt caused by a disliked transgressor is likely to lead to more negative cognitive (e.g., hostile attributions), affective (e.g., feelings of anger), and motivational (e.g., avoidance/revenge) outcomes than the hurt caused by a liked peer. In addition, we found that associations between cognitive processes and avoidance/revenge were mediated by feelings of anger, but only when the transgression occurred in the context of disliking. These results highlight the importance of studying how adolescents process hurtful experiences in different relational contexts.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Forgiveness/physiology , Interpersonal Relations , Social Perception , Adolescent , Anger/physiology , Female , Friends/psychology , Hostility , Humans , Male , Peer Group
9.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 101(2): 386-400, 2011 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21604893

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this longitudinal study was to examine the relationship specificity of aggressogenic thought-behavior processes and to investigate the role of self-esteem in translating or inhibiting aggressogenic thought into aggression toward personally liked and disliked targets. Participants (186 Finnish boys and girls; 11-12 years old at Time 1) completed measures twice over a 1-year interval. We assessed children's attributions of hostility, relational goals, expectations of anger, and self-efficacy at Time 1 as well as aggression, at both time points, toward their previously identified liked and disliked peers. Our results mostly supported our hypothesis that cognitions guide behavior mainly within the relationship context. Moreover, high self-esteem potentiated cognition-behavior links toward children's own liked peer but inhibited the actualization of aggressogenic thought toward children's disliked peer. These findings highlight the importance of taking a Person × Situation approach when studying cognition-behavior processes.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Thinking , Adolescent , Anger , Child , Female , Goals , Hostility , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Peer Group , Self Concept , Self Efficacy , Social Behavior , Surveys and Questionnaires
10.
Psychol Rev ; 117(2): 601-22, 2010 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20438239

ABSTRACT

This article outlines a model of the structure and the dynamics of gender cognition in childhood. The model incorporates 3 hypotheses featured in different contemporary theories of childhood gender cognition and unites them under a single theoretical framework. Adapted from Greenwald et al. (2002), the model distinguishes three constructs: gender identity, gender stereotypes, and attribute self-perceptions. The model specifies 3 causal processes among the constructs: Gender identity and stereotypes interactively influence attribute self-perceptions (stereotype emulation hypothesis); gender identity and attribute self-perceptions interactively influence gender stereotypes (stereotype construction hypothesis); and gender stereotypes and attribute self-perceptions interactively influence identity (identity construction hypothesis). The model resolves nagging ambiguities in terminology, organizes diverse hypotheses and empirical findings under a unifying conceptual umbrella, and stimulates many new research directions.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Self Concept , Social Behavior , Female , Gender Identity , Humans , Male , Sex Factors
11.
Soc Dev ; 19(3): 447-469, 2010 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26985138

ABSTRACT

Aggressive behavior has been well-studied in terms of interindividual differences among aggressors and victims, but has been understudied, especially within naturalistic contexts, in terms of aggressor-victim relationships. The Social Relations Model (SRM) is a powerful conceptual and analytic tool for studying dyadic phenomena, and we describe the use of multivariate SRM to study aggression. Boys and girls (N = 210) in middle schools (sixth and seventh grades) completed a newly created Dyadic Aggression and Victimization Inventory (DAVI). Results support the reliability and validity of the DAVI in assessing interindividual and interdyadic differences in aggression and victimization. Occurrences of aggression were accounted for primarily by interdyadic variability, indicating the importance of considering aggressor-victim relationships. We discuss the implications of this relationship focus for future research and intervention efforts.

12.
Child Dev ; 79(1): 170-85, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18269516

ABSTRACT

This study examined whether the affect children feel toward peers would influence children's social-cognitive evaluations and behaviors. The sample consisted of 209 fifth-grade children (11- to 12-year-olds; 119 boys and 90 girls). For each child, 3 target peers (liked, disliked, and neutral) were identified via a sociometric nomination procedure. The names of the targets were then inserted into hypothetical vignettes in which the target peer's behavior had a negative consequence for the child. After each vignette, questions about intent, outcome expectations, and self-efficacy beliefs were asked. In addition, self-reports regarding relationship-specific proactive and reactive aggression and regarding victimization were collected. The results demonstrate that children social-cognitively differentiate between the relationship types and that relationship-specific evaluations are associated with relationship-specific behaviors.


Subject(s)
Affect , Peer Group , Social Behavior , Social Desirability , Awareness , Child , Culture , Female , Friends/psychology , Humans , Male , Personality Development , Sex Factors , Sociometric Techniques
13.
Child Dev ; 78(6): 1627-39, 2007.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17988311

ABSTRACT

Two hypotheses--high self-esteem leads children to act on antisocial cognitions (disposition-activating hypothesis) and high self-esteem leads children to rationalize antisocial conduct (disposition-rationalizing hypothesis)--were investigated in two longitudinal studies. In Study 1 (N= 189; mean age = 11.1 years), antisocial behavior was aggression; in Study 2 (N= 407; mean age = 10.8 years) it was avoidance of the mother. In both studies, there was little evidence for the disposition-activating hypothesis but considerable support for the disposition-rationalizing hypothesis. Over time, aggressive children with high self-esteem increasingly valued the rewards that aggression offers and belittled their victims, and avoidant children with high self-esteem increasingly viewed their mother as harassing and uninvolved. For antisocial children, high self-esteem carries costs.


Subject(s)
Antisocial Personality Disorder/psychology , Self Concept , Adolescent , Antisocial Personality Disorder/diagnosis , Avoidance Learning , Character , Child , Culture , Defense Mechanisms , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Mother-Child Relations , Motivation , Object Attachment , Power, Psychological , Rationalization , Rejection, Psychology , Risk Factors , Social Adjustment , Sociometric Techniques
14.
Dev Psychol ; 43(4): 889-900, 2007 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17605522

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis that children's hostile attributions and behavioral strategies in response to peer provocation and rebuff situations will depend on the relationship with the target peer (i.e., friend, enemy, neutral). The sample consisted of 144 fourth graders (75 boys and 69 girls; mean age=10.47 years, SD=0.55 years). The participants were randomly selected from a pool of 442 children who completed a measure of social adjustment in Grade 3. In Grade 4, children participated in the individual interviews in which they were verbally presented with 6 hypothetical situations. The target peers were identified on the basis of the relationship descriptions. The results clearly demonstrate that children do indeed differentiate between relationship types in regard to hostile attributions and hostile strategies, with more hostility attributed and more hostile responses proposed toward enemies than toward other peers (ps<.05). In addition, this relationship effect holds even when the social and behavioral reputation of the target peers is accounted for (ps<.05).


Subject(s)
Attitude , Child Behavior/psychology , Hostility , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Child , Female , Friends , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Peer Group , Rejection, Psychology , Social Adjustment
15.
Dev Psychol ; 43(1): 261-6, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17201524

ABSTRACT

The generality of S. K. Egan and D. G. Perry's (2001) model of gender identity and adjustment was evaluated by examining associations between gender identity (felt gender typicality, felt gender contentedness, and felt pressure for gender conformity) and social adjustment in 863 White, Black, and Hispanic 5th graders (mean age = 11.1 years). Relations between gender identity and adjustment varied across ethnic/racial groups, indicating that S. K. Egan and D. G. Perry's model requires amendment. It is suggested that the implications of gender identity for adjustment depend on the particular meanings that a child attaches to gender (e.g., the specific attributes the child regards as desirable for each sex); these meanings may vary across and within ethnic/racial groups. Cross-ethnic/racial investigation can aid theory building by pointing to constructs that are neglected in research with a single ethnic/racial group but that are crucial components of basic developmental processes.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Black People/psychology , Gender Identity , Hispanic or Latino/psychology , White People/psychology , Character , Child , Crime Victims/psychology , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Female , Humans , Internal-External Control , Male , Peer Group , Self Concept , Social Conformity , Sociometric Techniques , Stereotyping
16.
Dev Psychol ; 42(6): 1327-38, 2006 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17087564

ABSTRACT

Similarity in early adolescent friends' general aggressiveness is well known, but questions remain regarding the degree to which friends aggress against the same victims. The authors examined this by administering the newly created Dyadic Aggression and Victimization Inventory to 417 sixth- through eighth-grade boys and girls (53%). Friends were found to share more targets for aggression than nonfriends, even after general levels of aggression were controlled (all ps < .05). Moreover, greater sharing of targets with friends relative to nonfriends was more pronounced among aggressive youths than nonaggressive youths, especially among aggressive youths' best friends relative to their other friends. Generally, these findings were similar across boys and girls as well as among older and younger youths. Implications of these findings and directions for future research are discussed.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Cooperative Behavior , Friends/psychology , Adolescent , Age Factors , Child , Crime Victims/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Psychometrics , Sex Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...