Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 19 de 19
Filter
1.
Front Neurosci ; 17: 1266201, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37954874

ABSTRACT

Purpose: To classify CVI subtypes and compare the added value of an extensive test battery over a limited test battery in subtype classification of cerebral visual impairment (CVI) in children. Methods: Seventy-five children with a clinical diagnosis of CVI (median [IQR] age: 9 [7-12] years) were identified from the medical records. The extensive test battery included visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, ocular alignment, eye movement analysis, visual field analysis, optic nerve head evaluation, and evaluation of visual perception. The limited test battery included visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, ocular alignment, and evaluation of visual perception. Principal component analysis (PCA) followed by cluster analysis was done, for both test batteries separately, to determine the optimum subtype classification for CVI. Results: Fifty-one participants with an extensive test battery with mild to moderate visual impairment were included in the main analysis. This resulted in four CVI subtypes for the extensive test battery (subtle characteristics, higher-level visual function deficits, lower-level visual function deficits, and higher- and lower- level visual function deficits) and three CVI subtypes for the limited test battery (subtle characteristics, higher-level visual function deficits, and higher- and lower- level visual function deficits). There were significant differences between the subtypes for 9 out of 10 measures of the extensive and all 4 measures of the limited test battery (p < 0.05). The subtle characteristics subtype (extensive n = 19, limited n = 15) showed near normal lower and higher-level visual functions in both test batteries. The higher-level visual function deficits subtype (extensive n = 18, limited n = 24) showed near normal visual acuity combined with significant visual perceptual deficits in both test batteries; accompanied by visual pathways defects and abnormal eye movement behavior in the extensive test battery. The higher- and lower- level visual function deficits subtype (extensive n = 4, limited n = 12) showed both higher and lower-level visual function deficits in both test batteries, but application of the extensive test battery revealed additional visual pathways defects and abnormal eye movement behavior. The lower-level visual function deficits CVI subtype (extensive n = 10) was a new subtype identified by the extensive test battery. This subtype showed lower-level visual function deficits together with abnormal eye movement measures. Conclusion: This data-driven study has provided meaningful CVI subtype classifications based on the outcomes of various key functional and structural measures in CVI diagnosis. Comparison of the extensive test battery to the limited test battery revealed the added value of an extensive test battery in classifying CVI. The outcomes of this study, therefore, have provided a new direction in the area of CVI classification.

2.
Front Psychol ; 13: 898719, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35783772

ABSTRACT

Viewing strategies are strategies used to support visual information processing. These strategies may differ between children with cerebral visual impairment (CVI), children with ocular visual impairment, and children with normal vision since visual impairment might have an impact on viewing behavior. In current visual rehabilitation practice a variety of strategies is used without consideration of the differences in etiology of the visual impairment or in the spontaneous viewing strategies used. This systematic scoping review focuses on viewing strategies used during near school-based tasks like reading and on possible interventions aimed at viewing strategies. The goal is threefold: (1) creating a clear concept of viewing strategies, (2) mapping differences in viewing strategies between children with ocular visual impairment, children with CVI and children with normal vision, and (3) identifying interventions that can improve visual processing by targeting viewing strategies. Four databases were used to conduct the literature search: PubMed, Embase, PsycINFO and Cochrane. Seven hundred and ninety-nine articles were screened by two independent reviewers using PRISMA reporting guidelines of which 30 were included for qualitative analysis. Only five studies explicitly mentioned strategies used during visual processing, namely gaze strategies, reading strategies and search strategies. We define a viewing strategy as a conscious and systematic way of viewing during task performance. The results of this review are integrated with different attention network systems, which provide direction on how to design future interventions targeting the use of viewing strategies to improve different aspects of visual processing.

3.
Front Neurosci ; 15: 651205, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34489619

ABSTRACT

Current treatments for infantile nystagmus (IN), focused on dampening the oscillating eye movements, yield little to no improvement in visual functioning. It makes sense, however, to treat the visual impairments associated with IN with tailored sensory training. Recently, we developed such a training, targeting visual crowding as an important bottleneck in visual functioning with an eye-movement engaging letter discrimination task. This training improved visual performance of children with IN, but most children had not reached plateau performance after 10 supervised training sessions (3,500 trials). Here, we evaluate the effects of prolonged perceptual learning (14,000 trials) in 7-18-year-old children with IN and test the feasibility of tablet-based, at-home intervention. Results demonstrate that prolonged home-based perceptual training results in stable, long lasting visual acuity improvements at distance and near, with remarkably good transfer to reading and even stereopsis. Improvements on self-reported functional vision scores underline the clinical relevance of perceptual learning with e-health apps for individuals with IN.

4.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 8873, 2020 06 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32483176

ABSTRACT

There is evidence that a pen-and-paper training based on perceptual learning principles improves near visual acuity in young children with visual impairment. The aim of the present study is to measure specificity and retention of its training effects during one year. Sixteen visually impaired children aged 4-8 years were divided in two age- and acuity-matched groups: an early (n = 9) and late treatment group (n = 7). Training consisted of 12 sessions (2× per week for 6 weeks). Studied variables were uncrowded and crowded binocular near visual acuity (40 cm), distance visual acuity (3.0 m) and fine motor skills (Beery VMI, subtest Motor Control). In the early treatment group, we measured at 0 months (pre-training), at 2 months (post-training), at 8 months (6 months post-training) and at 14 months (12 months post-training) since inclusion. In the late treatment group, three pre-training measurements were performed at 0, 2 and 8 months, and two measurements at 0 and 6 months post-training. In the short term, training improved uncrowded and crowded near visual acuity at 0.4 m by 0.13 ± 0.03 and 0.09 ± 0.03 logMAR, respectively (mean ± SEM). Training did not affect distance acuities or Beery scores. Learning effects on uncrowded and crowded near visual acuities remained intact 6-12 months after training. We conclude that the pen-and-paper training specifically improves near visual acuities but does not transfer to distance acuities or fine motor skills. Improvements in near visual acuity are retained over time, bolstering its clinical value.


Subject(s)
Photic Stimulation/methods , Vision, Low/rehabilitation , Visual Perception/physiology , Child , Child, Preschool , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Vision, Low/physiopathology , Visual Acuity , Visually Impaired Persons
6.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 58(10): 4162-4172, 2017 08 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28829849

ABSTRACT

Purpose: To identify predictors of sensitivity to perceptual learning on a computerized, near-threshold letter discrimination task in children with infantile nystagmus (idiopathic IN: n = 18; oculocutaneous albinism accompanied by IN: n = 18). Methods: Children were divided into two age-, acuity-, and diagnosis-matched training groups: a crowded (n = 18) and an uncrowded training group (n = 18). Training consisted of 10 sessions spread out over 5 weeks (grand total of 3500 trials). Baseline performance, age, diagnosis, training condition, and perceived pleasantness of training (training joy) were entered as linear regression predictors of training-induced changes on a single- and a crowded-letter task. Results: An impressive 57% of the variability in improvements of single-letter visual acuity was explained by age, training condition, and training joy. Being older and training with uncrowded letters were associated with larger single-letter visual acuity improvements. More training joy was associated with a larger gain from the uncrowded training and a smaller gain from the crowded training. Fifty-six percent of the variability in crowded-letter task improvements was explained by baseline performance, age, diagnosis, and training condition. After regressing out the variability induced by training condition, baseline performance, and age, perceptual learning proved more effective for children with idiopathic IN than for children with albinism accompanied by IN. Training gains increased with poorer baseline performance in idiopaths, but not in children with albinism accompanied by IN. Conclusions: Age and baseline performance, but not training joy, are important prognostic factors for the effect of perceptual learning in children with IN. However, their predictive value for achieving improvements in single-letter acuity and crowded letter acuity, respectively, differs between diagnostic subgroups and training condition. These findings may help with personalized treatment of individuals likely to benefit from perceptual learning.


Subject(s)
Learning/physiology , Nystagmus, Congenital/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Albinism, Oculocutaneous/physiopathology , Child , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Nystagmus, Congenital/rehabilitation , Regression Analysis , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Acuity
7.
BMC Ophthalmol ; 16(1): 215, 2016 Dec 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27931205

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The inclusion for rehabilitation of visually impaired children is partly based on the measurement of near vision, but guidelines for near visual acuity assessment are currently lacking. The twofold purpose of this systematic review was to: (i) provide an overview of the impact of the chart design on near visual acuity measured, and (ii) determine the method of choice for near vision assessments in children of different developmental ages. METHODS: A literature search was conducted by using the following electronic databases: PubMed, Cochrane Library, and EMBASE. The last search was run on March 26th 2016. Additional studies were identified by contacting experts and searching for relevant articles in reference lists of included studies. Search terms were: vision test(s), vision assessment(s), visual acuity, chart(s) and near. RESULTS: For children aged 0-3 years the golden standard is still the preferential looking procedure. Norms are available for this procedure for 6-36 month old children. For 4-7 year olds, we recommend using the LEA symbols, because these symbols have been properly validated and can be used in preliterate children. Responses can be verbal or by matching the target symbol. In children aged 8-13 years, the recommended method is the ETDRS letter chart, because letter acuity is more predictive for functional vision and reading than symbol acuity. In 8-13 year olds, letter acuity is 0.1-0.2 logMAR poorer than symbol acuity. CONCLUSIONS: Chart design, viewing distance, and threshold choice have a serious impact on near visual acuity measurements. Near visual acuity measured with symbols is lower than near visual acuity measured with gratings, and near visual acuity measured with letters is lower than near visual acuity measured with symbols. Viewing distance, chart used, and letter spacing should be adapted to the child's development and reported in order to allow comparisons between measurements.


Subject(s)
Optometry/methods , Vision Screening/methods , Vision, Low/diagnosis , Adolescent , Child , Child, Preschool , Equipment Design , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Reading , Vision Screening/instrumentation , Visual Acuity
8.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 57(10): 4216-28, 2016 08 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27548895

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To evaluate whether computerized training with a crowded or uncrowded letter-discrimination task reduces visual impairment (VI) in 6- to 11-year-old children with infantile nystagmus (IN) who suffer from increased foveal crowding, reduced visual acuity, and reduced stereopsis. METHODS: Thirty-six children with IN were included. Eighteen had idiopathic IN and 18 had oculocutaneous albinism. These children were divided in two training groups matched on age and diagnosis: a crowded training group (n = 18) and an uncrowded training group (n = 18). Training occurred two times per week during 5 weeks (3500 trials per training). Eleven age-matched children with normal vision were included to assess baseline differences in task performance and test-retest learning. Main outcome measures were task-specific performance, distance and near visual acuity (DVA and NVA), intensity and extent of (foveal) crowding at 5 m and 40 cm, and stereopsis. RESULTS: Training resulted in task-specific improvements. Both training groups also showed uncrowded and crowded DVA improvements (0.10 ± 0.02 and 0.11 ± 0.02 logMAR) and improved stereopsis (670 ± 249″). Crowded NVA improved only in the crowded training group (0.15 ± 0.02 logMAR), which was also the only group showing a reduction in near crowding intensity (0.08 ± 0.03 logMAR). Effects were not due to test-retest learning. CONCLUSIONS: Perceptual learning with or without distractors reduces the extent of crowding and improves visual acuity in children with IN. Training with distractors improves near vision more than training with single optotypes. Perceptual learning also transfers to DVA and NVA under uncrowded and crowded conditions and even stereopsis. Learning curves indicated that improvements may be larger after longer training.


Subject(s)
Depth Perception/physiology , Learning/physiology , Nystagmus, Congenital/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Visual Acuity , Child , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Nystagmus, Congenital/rehabilitation
9.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 57(10): 4229-38, 2016 08 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27548896

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To determine changes in oculomotor behavior after 10 sessions of perceptual learning on a letter discrimination task in children with infantile nystagmus (IN). METHODS: Children with IN (18 children with idiopathic IN and 18 with oculocutaneous albinism accompanied by IN) aged 6 to 11 years were divided into two training groups matched on diagnosis: an uncrowded training group (n = 18) and a crowded training group (n = 18). Target letters always appeared briefly (500 ms) at an eccentric location, forcing subjects to quickly redirect their gaze. Training occurred twice per week for 5 consecutive weeks (3500 trials total). Norm data and test-retest values were collected from children with normal vision (n = 11). Outcome measures were: nystagmus characteristics (amplitude, frequency, intensity, and the expanded nystagmus acuity function); fixation stability (the bivariate contour ellipse area and foveation time); and saccadic eye movements (latencies and accuracy) made during a simple saccade task and a crowded letter-identification task. RESULTS: After training, saccadic responses of children with IN improved on the saccade task (latencies decreased by 14 ± 4 ms and gains increased by 0.03 ± 0.01), but not on the crowded letter task. There were also no training-induced changes in nystagmus characteristics and fixation stability. Although children with normal vision had shorter latencies in the saccade task (47 ± 14 ms at baseline), test-retest changes in their saccade gains and latencies were almost equal to the training effects observed in children with IN. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that the improvement in visual performance after perceptual learning in children with IN is primarily due to improved sensory processing rather than improved two-dimensional oculomotor behavior.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Learning/physiology , Nystagmus, Congenital/physiopathology , Oculomotor Muscles/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Child , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Nystagmus, Congenital/rehabilitation
10.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 57(10): 4239-46, 2016 08 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27548897

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Perceptual learning improves visual acuity and reduces crowding in children with infantile nystagmus (IN). Here, we compare reading performance of 6- to 11-year-old children with IN with normal controls, and evaluate whether perceptual learning improves their reading. METHODS: Children with IN were divided in two training groups: a crowded training group (n = 18; albinism: n = 8; idiopathic IN: n = 10) and an uncrowded training group (n = 17; albinism: n = 9; idiopathic IN: n = 8). Also 11 children with normal vision participated. Outcome measures were: reading acuity (the smallest readable font size), maximum reading speed, critical print size (font size below which reading is suboptimal), and acuity reserve (difference between reading acuity and critical print size). We used multiple regression analyses to test if these reading parameters were related to the children's uncrowded distance acuity and/or crowding scores. RESULTS: Reading acuity and critical print size were 0.65 ± 0.04 and 0.69 ± 0.08 log units larger for children with IN than for children with normal vision. Maximum reading speed and acuity reserve did not differ between these groups. After training, reading acuity improved by 0.12 ± 0.02 logMAR and critical print size improved by 0.11 ± 0.04 logMAR in both IN training groups. The changes in reading acuity, critical print size, and acuity reserve of children with IN were tightly related to changes in their uncrowded distance acuity and the changes in magnitude and extent of crowding. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings are the first to show that visual acuity is not the only factor that restricts reading in children with IN, but that crowding also limits their reading performance. By targeting both of these spatial bottlenecks in children with IN, our perceptual learning paradigms significantly improved their reading acuity and critical print size. This shows that perceptual learning can effectively transfer to reading.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Learning/physiology , Nystagmus, Congenital/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reading , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Nystagmus, Congenital/rehabilitation
11.
J Vis ; 15(1): 15.1.23, 2015 Jan 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25613761

ABSTRACT

In typically developing children, crowding decreases with increasing age. The influence of target-distractor similarity with respect to orientation and element spacing on visual search performance was investigated in 29 school-age children with normal vision (4- to 6-year-olds [N = 16], 7- to 8-year-olds [N = 13]). Children were instructed to search for a target E among distractor Es (feature search: all flanking Es pointing right; conjunction search: flankers in three orientations). Orientation of the target was manipulated in four directions: right (target absent), left (inversed), up, and down (vertical). Spacing was varied in four steps: 0.04°, 0.5°, 1°, and 2°. During feature search, high target-distractor similarity had a stronger impact on performance than spacing: Orientation affected accuracy until spacing was 1°, and spacing only influenced accuracy for identifying inversed targets. Spatial analyses showed that orientation affected oculomotor strategy: Children made more fixations in the "inversed" target area (4.6) than the vertical target areas (1.8 and 1.9). Furthermore, age groups differed in fixation duration: 4- to 6-year-old children showed longer fixation durations than 7- to 8-year-olds at the two largest element spacings (p = 0.039 and p = 0.027). Conjunction search performance was unaffected by spacing. Four conclusions can be drawn from this study: (a) Target-distractor similarity governs visual search performance in school-age children, (b) children make more fixations in target areas when target-distractor similarity is high, (c) 4- to 6-year-olds show longer fixation durations than 7- to 8-year-olds at 1° and 2° element spacing, and (d) spacing affects feature but not conjunction search-a finding that might indicate top-down control ameliorates crowding in children.


Subject(s)
Crowding , Eye Movements/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Distortion , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Visual Acuity/physiology
12.
Front Psychol ; 5: 988, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25309473

ABSTRACT

Perceptual learning (PL) can improve near visual acuity (NVA) in 4-9 year old children with visual impairment (VI). However, the mechanisms underlying improved NVA are unknown. The present study compares feature search and oculomotor measures in 4-9 year old children with VI accompanied by nystagmus (VI+nys [n = 33]) and children with normal vision (NV [n = 29]). Children in the VI+nys group were divided into three training groups: an experimental PL group, a control PL group, and a magnifier group. They were seen before (baseline) and after 6 weeks of training. Children with NV were only seen at baseline. The feature search task entailed finding a target E among distractor E's (pointing right) with element spacing varied in four steps: 0.04°, 0.5°, 1°, and 2°. At baseline, children with VI+nys showed longer search times, shorter fixation durations, and larger saccade amplitudes than children with NV. After training, all training groups showed shorter search times. Only the experimental PL group showed prolonged fixation duration after training at 0.5° and 2° spacing, p's respectively 0.033 and 0.021. Prolonged fixation duration was associated with reduced crowding and improved crowded NVA. One of the mechanisms underlying improved crowded NVA after PL in children with VI+nys seems to be prolonged fixation duration.

13.
Vision Res ; 96: 65-74, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24456806

ABSTRACT

This study investigates the influence of oculomotor control, crowding, and attentional factors on visual search in children with normal vision ([NV], n=11), children with visual impairment without nystagmus ([VI-nys], n=11), and children with VI with accompanying nystagmus ([VI+nys], n=26). Exclusion criteria for children with VI were: multiple impairments and visual acuity poorer than 20/400 or better than 20/50. Three search conditions were presented: a row with homogeneous distractors, a matrix with homogeneous distractors, and a matrix with heterogeneous distractors. Element spacing was manipulated in 5 steps from 2 to 32 minutes of arc. Symbols were sized 2 times the threshold acuity to guarantee visibility for the VI groups. During simple row and matrix search with homogeneous distractors children in the VI+nys group were less accurate than children with NV at smaller spacings. Group differences were even more pronounced during matrix search with heterogeneous distractors. Search times were longer in children with VI compared to children with NV. The more extended impairments during serial search reveal greater dependence on oculomotor control during serial compared to parallel search.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Nystagmus, Congenital/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Analysis of Variance , Case-Control Studies , Child , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Acuity/physiology
14.
Strabismus ; 21(4): 216-24, 2013 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24195763

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND/AIMS: To compare interocular acuity differences, crowding ratios, and binocular summation ratios in 4- to 8-year-old children with albinism (n = 16), children with infantile nystagmus syndrome (n = 10), and children with normal vision (n = 72). METHODS: Interocular acuity differences and binocular summation ratios were compared between groups. Crowding ratios were calculated by dividing the single Landolt C decimal acuity with the crowded Landolt C decimal acuity mono- and binocularly. A linear regression analysis was conducted to investigate the contribution of 5 predictors to the monocular and binocular crowding ratio: nystagmus amplitude, nystagmus frequency, strabismus, astigmatism, and anisometropia. RESULTS: Crowding ratios were higher under mono- and binocular viewing conditions for children with infantile nystagmus syndrome than for children with normal vision. Children with albinism showed higher crowding ratios in their poorer eye and under binocular viewing conditions than children with normal vision. Children with albinism and children with infantile nystagmus syndrome showed larger interocular acuity differences than children with normal vision (0.1 logMAR in our clinical groups and 0.0 logMAR in children with normal vision). Binocular summation ratios did not differ between groups. Strabismus and nystagmus amplitude predicted the crowding ratio in the poorer eye (p = 0.015 and p = 0.005, respectively). The crowding ratio in the better eye showed a marginally significant relation with nystagmus frequency and depth of anisometropia (p = 0.082 and p = 0.070, respectively). The binocular crowding ratio was not predicted by any of the variables. CONCLUSIONS: Children with albinism and children with infantile nystagmus syndrome show larger interocular acuity differences than children with normal vision. Strabismus and nystagmus amplitude are significant predictors of the crowding ratio in the poorer eye.


Subject(s)
Abnormalities, Multiple , Albinism/physiopathology , Child Development , Nystagmus, Congenital/physiopathology , Oculomotor Muscles/growth & development , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Retrospective Studies , Syndrome
15.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 54(9): 6208-16, 2013 Sep 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23950157

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: This study investigated whether visual perceptual learning can improve near visual acuity and reduce foveal crowding effects in four- to nine-year-old children with visual impairment. METHODS: Participants were 45 children with visual impairment and 29 children with normal vision. Children with visual impairment were divided into three groups: a magnifier group (n = 12), a crowded perceptual learning group (n = 18), and an uncrowded perceptual learning group (n = 15). Children with normal vision also were divided in three groups, but were measured only at baseline. Dependent variables were single near visual acuity (NVA), crowded NVA, LH line 50% crowding NVA, number of trials, accuracy, performance time, amount of small errors, and amount of large errors. Children with visual impairment trained during six weeks, two times per week, for 30 minutes (12 training sessions). RESULTS: After training, children showed significant improvement of NVA in addition to specific improvements on the training task. The crowded perceptual learning group showed the largest acuity improvements (1.7 logMAR lines on the crowded chart, P < 0.001). Only the children in the crowded perceptual learning group showed improvements on all NVA charts. CONCLUSIONS: Children with visual impairment benefit from perceptual training. While task-specific improvements were observed in all training groups, transfer to crowded NVA was largest in the crowded perceptual learning group. To our knowledge, this is the first study to provide evidence for the improvement of NVA by perceptual learning in children with visual impairment. (http://www.trialregister.nl number, NTR2537.).


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning/physiology , Learning/physiology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time
16.
Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol ; 251(7): 1813-9, 2013 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23504083

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: This study compares the influence of two different types of magnification (magnifier versus large print) on crowded near vision task performance. METHODS: Fifty-eight visually impaired children aged 4-8 years participated. Participants were divided in two groups, matched on age and near visual acuity (NVA): [1] the magnifier group (4-6 year olds [n = 13] and 7-8 year olds [n = 19]), and [2] the large print group (4-6 year olds [n = 12] and 7-8 year olds [n = 14]). At baseline, single and crowded Landolt C acuity were measured at 40 cm without magnification. Crowded near vision was measured again with magnification. A 90 mm diameter dome magnifier was chosen to avoid measuring the confounding effect of navigational skills. The magnifier provided 1.7× magnification and the large print provided 1.8× magnification. Performance measures: [1] NVA without magnification at 40 cm, [2] near vision with magnification, and [3] response time. Working distance was monitored. RESULTS: There was no difference in performance between the two types of magnification for the 4-6 year olds and the 7-8 year olds (p's = .291 and .246, respectively). Average NVA in the 4-6 year old group was 0.95 logMAR without and 0.42 logMAR with magnification (p < .001). Average NVA in the 7-8 year was 0.71 logMAR without and 0.01 logMAR with magnification (p < .001). Stronger crowding effects predicted larger improvements of near vision with magnification (p = .021). CONCLUSIONS: A magnifier is equally effective as large print in improving the performance of young children with a range of visual acuities on a crowded near vision task. Visually impaired children with stronger crowding effects showed larger improvements when working with magnification.


Subject(s)
Lenses , Mass Media , Sensory Aids , Task Performance and Analysis , Vision, Low/physiopathology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Visually Impaired Persons , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Reading , Vision, Low/rehabilitation
17.
BMC Ophthalmol ; 12: 27, 2012 Jul 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22824242

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: This systematic review gives an overview of foveal crowding (the inability to recognize objects due to surrounding nearby contours in foveal vision) and possible interventions. Foveal crowding can have a major effect on reading rate and deciphering small pieces of information from busy visual scenes. Three specific groups experience more foveal crowding than adults with normal vision (NV): 1) children with NV, 2) visually impaired (VI) children and adults and 3) children with cerebral visual impairment (CVI). The extent and magnitude of foveal crowding as well as interventions aimed at reducing crowding were investigated in this review. The twofold goal of this review is : [A] to compare foveal crowding in children with NV, VI children and adults and CVI children and [B] to compare interventions to reduce crowding. METHODS: Three electronic databases were used to conduct the literature search: PubMed, PsycINFO (Ovid), and Cochrane. Additional studies were identified by contacting experts. Search terms included visual perception, contour interaction, crowding, crowded, and contour interactions. RESULTS: Children with normal vision show an extent of contour interaction over an area 1.5-3× as large as that seen in adults NV. The magnitude of contour interaction normally ranges between 1-2 lines on an acuity chart and this magnitude is even larger when stimuli are arranged in a circular configuration. Adults with congenital nystagmus (CN) show interaction areas that are 2× larger than those seen adults with NV. The magnitude of the crowding effect is also 2× as large in individuals with CN as in individuals with NV. Finally, children with CVI experience a magnitude of the crowding effect that is 3× the size of that experienced by adults with NV. CONCLUSIONS: The methodological heterogeneity, the diversity in paradigms used to measure crowding, made it impossible to conduct a meta-analysis. This is the first systematic review to compare crowding ratios and it shows that charts with 50% interoptotype spacing were most sensitive to capture crowding effects. The groups that showed the largest crowding effects were individuals with CN, VI adults with central scotomas and children with CVI. Perceptual Learning seems to be a promising technique to reduce excessive foveal crowding effects.


Subject(s)
Fovea Centralis/physiology , Learning/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Adolescent , Child , Child, Preschool , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Humans , Visual Acuity/physiology
18.
Strabismus ; 20(2): 44-8, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22612351

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: This study evaluated the effect of an evidence-based magnifier training on viewing behavior in visually impaired children aged 3 to 6½ years. METHODS: Effects of a training with a stand magnifier were evaluated by analyzing recordings of 21 visually impaired children, obtained from a miniature camera mounted in the magnifier. In a pre-test, post-test design, 11 of the children trained without magnifier and 10 children trained with magnifier. Three measures were compared from pre- to post-test assessment: 1) observation time in seconds through the magnifier during task performance; 2) the eye that was used during task performance with the magnifier (right eye/left eye as recorded by the camera); and 3) the self-chosen eye-to-chart distance (in cm) in near visual acuity measurement. RESULTS: Three important changes were found by analyzing the eye-camera recordings: (1) There was a significant shift in average observation time (i.e., the duration of looking through the magnifier in a single glance), before and after training. In the pre-test children used less than 10 s for a glance through the magnifier, whereas in the post-test this was 10-30 s. (2) In 5 children there was no preference with respect to the number of glances through the magnifier with right or left eye during pre-test measurement. However, such a task-specific dominance was clearly observed in this subgroup after training (post-test measurement). (3) The eye-to-chart distance, as measured during near-vision testing with LH-single and LH-line test, decreased significantly over the training period (from 9.5 cm to 7.9 cm, pre- to post-test). There were no differences in the outcome measures between the with-magnifier and without-magnifier training groups. We can conclude that (1) the magnifier training had a positive effect on viewing behavior and the development of dominance, and (2) camera observations provide valuable data on children's viewing behavior.


Subject(s)
Audiovisual Aids , Photography/instrumentation , Sensory Aids , Vision, Low/rehabilitation , Visually Impaired Persons/rehabilitation , Child , Child Behavior , Child, Preschool , Dominance, Ocular/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Task Performance and Analysis , Video Recording , Vision, Low/psychology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Visually Impaired Persons/psychology
19.
Strabismus ; 20(2): 55-62, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22612353

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND/AIMS: To investigate crowding ratios in children with a visual impairment due to ocular disease (n = 58) and normally sighted children (n = 75) aged 4 to 8 years using several variants of two clinically available tests with different optotype spacing (fixed or proportional to the optotype size). METHODS: Crowding ratios, calculated by dividing the single acuity by the linear acuity, were measured binocularly with the C-test and the LH line chart. Ratios >1.00 indicate crowding. RESULTS: The charts with fixed spacing revealed significantly higher crowding ratios for visually impaired children than normally sighted children (both for measurements at 40 cm and 5 m). The age-related reduction of the crowding ratios seen in normally sighted children when tested with near-vision charts with fixed spacing was not present in the visually impaired group. Visually impaired children with nystagmus showed higher crowding ratios than visually impaired children without nystagmus. The chart with proportional intersymbol spacing (ISS) did not reveal differences between the normally sighted and visually impaired children; nor did it show group, age, or nystagmus effects. CONCLUSION: Visually impaired children showed higher crowding ratios than normally sighted children when measured with charts with fixed ISS. This study illustrates that test design and target/flanker interference as a manifestation of crowding are critical issues to bear in mind when assessing crowding ratios in children.


Subject(s)
Vision, Low/physiopathology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Visually Impaired Persons , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Research Design , Vision Tests
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...