Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 19 de 19
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Neurosci ; 23(7): 3066-75, 2003 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12684493

ABSTRACT

Sensory-motor integration has frequently been studied using a single-step change in a control variable such as prismatic lens angle and has revealed human visuomotor adaptation to often be partial and inefficient. We propose that the changes occurring in everyday life are better represented as the accumulation of many smaller perturbations contaminated by measurement noise. We have therefore tested human performance to random walk variations in the visual feedback of hand movements during a pointing task. Subjects made discrete targeted pointing movements to a visual target and received terminal feedback via a cursor the position of which was offset from the actual movement endpoint by a random walk element and a random observation element. By applying ideal observer analysis, which for this task compares human performance against that of a Kalman filter, we show that the subjects' performance was highly efficient with Fisher efficiencies reaching 73%. We then used system identification techniques to characterize the control strategy used. A "modified" delta-rule algorithm best modeled the human data, which suggests that they estimated the random walk perturbation of feedback in this task using an exponential weighting of recent errors. The time constant of the exponential weighting of the best-fitting model varied with the rate of random walk drift. Because human efficiency levels were high and did not vary greatly across three levels of observation noise, these results suggest that the algorithm the subjects used exponentially weighted recent errors with a weighting that varied with the level of drift in the task to maintain efficient performance.


Subject(s)
Algorithms , Psychomotor Performance , Vision, Ocular , Adult , Feedback , Female , Hand/physiology , Humans , Kinetics , Male , Models, Theoretical , Movement
2.
Exp Brain Res ; 133(4): 491-500, 2000 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10985683

ABSTRACT

It is not yet certain which sources of information are most important in judging the weight of a held object. In order to study this question further, a "deafferented" man and five controls flexed their wrist to lift a container weighing 1,000 g. Direct vision of the arm and weight was denied; the container's vertical position was displayed to the subjects on an oscilloscope at the start of each trial and, then, in most experimental conditions, this display was removed. The weight was then either gradually increased or decreased over 20 s or left unchanged, on a pseudorandom basis. A verbal judgement of its change was required at the end of each trial, lasting 20 or 40 s. Under these conditions, the "deafferented" subject was unable to correctly judge the weight changes (38% accuracy, n.s. chi2, compared with 77% in control subjects), and even the control subjects, when exposed to muscle vibration, made many errors (54% accuracy). However, in many trials, including those in which the weight was unchanged, the vertical height of the container was not held constant by the subjects, but drifted up or down (mean absolute drift: approximately 2 cm). Hence, the change in muscular activation or stiffness could be estimated by the observers in the majority of trials. This allowed the verbal judgements of both the "deafferented" man and of control subjects undergoing muscle vibration to be correlated with the muscle activation produced, independent of the actual weight being tested. Post-hoc predictions of controls' responses during vibration, based on the direction of the change in muscle activity which these drifts in position implied, were 77% and 66% accurate for +/-750 g and +/-375 g tasks and 73% accurate for forearm-vibration trials (P<0.0001, chi2). Predictions of the "deafferented" subject's responses were 64% accurate (P=0.0002, chi2), even though his own responses were at a chance level with respect to the actual weight change. The judgements made by these subjects might have been based upon a peripheral sensory input, as small afferent fibres are still present in the "deafferented" man and vibration only partly blocked sensory function in the control subjects. Care was taken to minimise all other possible cues to the weight changes, e.g. vestibular, thermal, pressure or pain cues. However, peripheral inputs may not be the only signals used in the subjects' perceptual judgements. They might, instead, be based upon a centrally originating, but illusory changing sense of body position or, possibly, a changing sense of effort. In both cases, a perceived discordance between voluntary muscle activation and body image could underlie the subjects' responses. Our data do not yet allow us to distinguish between these alternative peripheral and central hypotheses, but do highlight the need to include perceptions of body position and motion into judgements of force control.


Subject(s)
Forearm/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Peripheral Nervous System/physiology , Weight Perception/physiology , Weight-Bearing/physiology , Case-Control Studies , Chi-Square Distribution , Confidence Intervals , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Muscle Denervation , Peripheral Nervous System/injuries , Proprioception/physiology
3.
Exp Brain Res ; 132(1): 114-26, 2000 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10836641

ABSTRACT

The role of proprioception in the control and adaptation of visuomotor relationships is still unclear. We have studied a deafferented subject, IW, and control subjects in a task in which they used single joint elbow extension to move to a visual target, with visual feedback of the terminal position provided by a cursor displayed in the plane of their movements. We report the differences in movement accuracy between the deafferented subject and controls in the normal task and when challenged with a cognitive load, counting backwards. All subjects were less accurate when counting; this was a small effect for the controls (<10% change) but much greater for the deafferented subject (>60% change). We also examined changes in movement kinematics when the instructed amplitude was altered via a changed gain between final arm position and presentation of the feedback cursor. The deafferented subject maintained temporal movement parameters stable and altered amplitude by scaling force (i.e. changed peak velocity), whereas the controls scaled both movement velocity and duration. Finally, we compared the subjects' adaptation of movement amplitude after a period of exposure to the changed visuomotor gain. The deafferented subject was able to adapt, but his adaptation was severely impaired by the counting task. These results suggest that proprioception is not an absolute requirement for adaptation to occur. Instead, proprioception has a more subtle role to play in the adjustment to visuomotor perturbations. It has an important role in the control of reaching movements, while in the absence of proprioception, attention appears necessary to monitor movements.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Attention/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Afferent Pathways/physiopathology , Biomechanical Phenomena , Denervation , Elbow/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Reference Values
4.
J Manag Med ; 13(1): 51-8, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10557661

ABSTRACT

It is becoming more difficult to provide health care that meets the needs of patients within tight budget constraints. This article suggests that one way forward is to channel the energies of people more constructively to work as teams. Some definitions are shown for teams and teamwork, and four benefits suggested of effective teamwork: learning and development, resource management, task performance and communications. In each of these four areas, examples are given from the international world of health care of how teamwork can be achieved. Finally, it is concluded that teamwork can be difficult, but that it is worth pursuing.


Subject(s)
Group Processes , Institutional Management Teams , Patient Care Team , State Medicine/organization & administration , Humans , Interprofessional Relations , Learning , Task Performance and Analysis , United Kingdom
5.
Neurosci Lett ; 264(1-3): 121-4, 1999 Apr 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10320029

ABSTRACT

Visuomotor control of the arm was assessed in a single case study of a subject with focal lesions in the cerebellum and brainstem. A dissociation between 'on-line' and 'off-line' visuomotor control was revealed: impairments in 'on-line' visuomotor control included inaccuracy of tracking velocity, increase in spatial pointing variability and a delay in simple reaction time; whereas the patient was able to adapt to a gain change in 'off-line' visual feedback during a pointing task, and his adaptation was less affected than that of control subjects by trial-to-trial random fluctuations in 'off-line' visual feedback. We conclude that focal damage in the cerebellar peduncles may be principally responsible for this dissociation.


Subject(s)
Brain Stem/physiopathology , Cerebellum/physiopathology , Multiple Sclerosis/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Brain/pathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Multiple Sclerosis/diagnosis , Reaction Time/physiology , Reference Values
6.
Surg Endosc ; 7(4): 300-3, 1993.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8351600

ABSTRACT

Laparoscopic cholecystectomy (LC) can be introduced into a community with morbidity and mortality rates equal to that of open cholecystectomy. The entire general surgical community of Greensboro, NC, learned the technique of LC on animal models prior to offering this innovation to the community. Over the ensuing 12 months, they served as surgeons or assistant surgeons to each other on 762 LCs with morbidity and mortality rates comparable to open cholecystectomy. This retrospective study examined the first 1 year of experience beginning 8/13/90. This work represents all of the LCs performed in Greensboro, and all of the surgeons participated in this review. All of the surgeries were done with an electrocautery and utilized a 0 degree forward-viewing scope. Cases were performed at two hospitals with a surgeon as both operator and assistant, and no effort was made to exclude high-risk or elderly patients from this procedure. Patients averaged 50 years of age and ranged from 14 to 96 years. Static cholangiograms were performed in 27% of patients. Conversion to open cholecystectomy was seen in 4.8%. There were two cardiac deaths (0.26%) and significant complications were seen in 3.4%. Seven patients required reoperations. There were no major common bile duct injuries. This retrospective review indicates that this new procedure can be introduced into a community setting by novice laparoscopic surgeons acting both as operators and assistant with a morbidity and mortality rate comparable to that reported for open cholecystectomy.


Subject(s)
Cholecystectomy, Laparoscopic/statistics & numerical data , Cholelithiasis/surgery , Cholangiography , Cholecystectomy , Cholelithiasis/epidemiology , Female , Hospitals, Community , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , North Carolina/epidemiology , Postoperative Complications/epidemiology , Retrospective Studies
7.
Scand J Gastroenterol ; 23(4): 449-57, 1988 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3381066

ABSTRACT

This study was performed to determine whether the level of neurotensin in mesenteric venous blood after lipid perfusion is sufficient to establish neurotensin as a mediator of lipid-induced mesenteric vasodilation. In anesthetized dogs, arterial flow to segments of the ileum was recorded, and blood was collected for measurement of neurotensin-like immunoreactivity, and neurotensin and metabolites. Perfusion of the lumen with micellar lipid resulted in an increase in blood blow from 37.7 +/- 4.1 to 44.5 +/- 3.9 ml/min/100 g (p less than 0.01; n = 8); flow to a control segment did not change. Venous plasma neurotensin-like immunoreactivity doubled, and neurotensin also increased (to 11.3 +/- 3.9 fmol/ml; p less than 0.05). Close intra-arterial infusion of neurotensin at 5 pmol/min increased blood flow to 44.3 +/- 3.4 ml/min/100 g (p less than 0.025; n = 5); flow to a control segment did not change. Neurotensin-like immunoreactivity increased to the same extent as with lipid perfusion, and neurotensin increased to 28.6 +/- 6.1 fmol/ml (p less than 0.05). No accumulation of metabolites was detected in either experiment. Thus, infused neurotensin caused increased ileal blood flow at a level in venous plasma comparable to that present after lipid perfusion, suggesting that neurotensin may have a role in the local regulation of ileal blood flow.


Subject(s)
Ileum/blood supply , Lipids/pharmacology , Neurotensin/pharmacology , Animals , Chromatography, High Pressure Liquid , Dogs , Female , Infusions, Intra-Arterial , Male , Neuropeptides/blood , Neurotensin/metabolism , Perfusion , Radioimmunoassay , Regional Blood Flow/drug effects , Vasodilation/drug effects
8.
QRB Qual Rev Bull ; 13(11): 372-6, 1987 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3122149

ABSTRACT

Quality assurance (QA) activities in public health programs typically are fragmented. The Division of Public Health of Maricopa County in Phoenix decided to develop a comprehensive QA program in 1981 that involved both the development of clinical protocols and the formalization of procedures for planning, executing, and following up on QA studies. A clinical protocols committee was formed to generate protocol manuals that serve as the principal criteria source for QA studies. A QA committee was also established to set guidelines for studies and to supervise their execution. Thus far, the QA program has resulted in a significant increase in QA activities within the public health division and improvements in many aspects of patient care.


Subject(s)
Public Health/trends , Quality Assurance, Health Care/trends , Arizona , Child Health Services/standards , Clinical Protocols , Community Health Services/standards , Female , Humans , Infant, Newborn , Maternal Health Services/standards , Pregnancy
10.
Hosp Trustee ; 8(1): 7-8, 1984.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10265012
16.
Practitioner ; 196(174): 525-31, 1966 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-5908934
17.
Proc R Soc Med ; 59(3): 215-23, 1966 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-5930033
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...