Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 16 de 16
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Cogn Psychol ; 43(3): 171-216, 2001 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11689021

ABSTRACT

Two tasks were used to evaluate the grain of visual attention, the minimum spacing at which attention can select individual items. First, observers performed a tracking task at many viewing distances. When the display subtended less than 1 degrees in size, tracking was no longer possible even though observers could resolve the items and their motions: The items were visible but could not be individuated one from the other. The limiting size for selection was roughly the same whether tracking one or three targets, suggesting that the resolution limit acts independently of the capacity limit of attention. Second, the closest spacing that still allowed individuation of single items in dense, static displays was examined. This critical spacing was about 50% coarser in the radial direction compared to the tangential direction and was coarser in the upper as opposed to the lower visual field. The results suggest that no more than about 60 items can be arrayed in the central 30 degrees of the visual field while still allowing attentional access to each individually. Our data show that selection has a coarse grain, much coarser than visual resolution. These measures of the resolution of attention are based solely on the selection of location and are not confounded with preattentive feature interactions that may contribute to measures from flanker and crowding tasks. The results suggest that the parietal area is the most likely locus of this selection mechanism and that it acts by pointing to the spatial coordinates (or cortical coordinates) of items of interest rather than by holding a representation of the items themselves.


Subject(s)
Attention , Discrimination, Psychological , Signal Detection, Psychological , Visual Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Neurological , Psychophysics
2.
J Neuroophthalmol ; 21(4): 250-5, 2001 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11756853

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To compare saccadic dynamics in superior oblique palsy and Brown's syndrome. METHODS: Vertical saccades in adduction and in abduction were studied in two subjects with superior oblique palsy and one with Brown's syndrome. Using large numbers of centrifugal saccades over a wide range of amplitudes, we measured peak velocity, duration, and the peak velocity/mean velocity ratio (PV/MV) as a function of saccadic amplitude. We compared vertical saccades in 30 degrees of abduction with those in 30 degrees of adduction. RESULTS: Superior oblique palsy caused a 15-18% reduction in peak velocities in adduction compared with abduction. Saccadic duration was also increased in adduction, with the result that there was no net change in the PV/MV ratio. In the patient with Brown's syndrome, velocities and durations of upward saccades were similar in abduction and adduction, but the PV/MV ratio was significantly elevated in adduction. We also observed an unusual high-speed lateral 'snap' of about 5 degrees that frequently interrupted vertical saccades in the midline but not elsewhere. CONCLUSION: Both paresis and restriction of the superior oblique alter vertical saccades. The effects of restriction on saccadic dynamics are distinct from the effects of paresis.


Subject(s)
Oculomotor Muscles/physiopathology , Ophthalmoplegia/physiopathology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Diagnostic Techniques, Ophthalmological , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Syndrome
3.
Neuron ; 32(6): 985-95, 2001 Dec 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11754832

ABSTRACT

Patients with right parietal damage demonstrate a variety of attentional deficits in their left visual field contralateral to their lesion. We now report that patients with right lesions also show a severe loss in the perception of apparent motion in their "good" right visual field ipsilateral to their lesion. Three tests of attention were conducted, and losses were found only in the contralesional fields for a selective attention and a multiple object tracking task. Losses in apparent motion, however, were bilateral in all cases. The deficit in apparent motion in the parietal patients supports previous claims that this relatively effortless percept is mediated by attention. However, the bilateral deficit suggests that the disruption is due to a bilateral loss in the temporal resolution of attention to transient events that drive the apparent motion percept.


Subject(s)
Functional Laterality/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Attention/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance , Visual Fields/physiology
4.
Neuroreport ; 11(12): 2639-42, 2000 Aug 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10976935

ABSTRACT

A patient with a lesion of the posterior half of the corpus callosum correctly named simple (e.g. colors) and complex (e.g. faces) stimuli whether presented to one or both hemifields. Although proficient at these tasks, and also able to make same/different judgements for stimuli within an hemifield, he failed dramatically when required to compare stimuli between the two hemifields. These results indicate that, while the posterior portion of the corpus callosum may not be essential for naming visual stimuli, it is essential for comparing stimuli between the two visual fields.


Subject(s)
Corpus Callosum/physiopathology , Mental Processes/physiology , Names , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Brain Diseases/diagnosis , Brain Diseases/surgery , Corpus Callosum/surgery , Cysts/surgery , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Postoperative Period
5.
Percept Psychophys ; 62(8): 1619-24, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11140182

ABSTRACT

When a bright white disk revolves around a fixation point on a gray background, observers perceive a "spoke": a dark gray region that connects the disk with the fixation point. Our first experiment suggests that motion across the retina is both necessary and sufficient for spokes: The illusion occurs when a disk moves across the retina even though it is perceived to be stationary, but the illusion does not occur when the disk appears to move while remaining stationary on the retina. A second experiment shows that the strength of the illusion decreases with decreasing luminance contrast until subjective equiluminance, where little or no spoke is perceived. These results suggest that spokes originate at an early, predominantly luminance-based stage of motion processing, before the visual system discounts retinal motion caused by smooth pursuit.


Subject(s)
Attention , Contrast Sensitivity , Motion Perception , Optical Illusions , Attention/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Field Dependence-Independence , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Psychophysics , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Retina/physiology
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 37(3): 267-77, 1999 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10199641

ABSTRACT

We examined bisection of lines viewed in only one hemifield by normal subjects. Subjects first performed a traditional version of line bisection, by indicating the perceived midpoint of a line on paper with a penmark. Bisection was accurate when they were allowed to shift their gaze over the stimulus, but it was biased towards the central visual field (centripetally) when gaze was fixed so that the line was seen in only one hemifield. In a second experiment, lines with transectors at various locations were presented briefly on a screen and subjects had to indicate on which side of the perceived midpoint the transector was located. A centripetal bias was still found, indicating that it has a perceptual origin. The interaction between bias and effects of tangent line presentation suggested that subjects were performing an angle bisection rather than a line bisection. Also, there was bias in not only right and left hemifields but also upper and lower hemifields. In a third experiment, increasing the width of the stimulus bars peripherally did not eliminate this bias. Bias was size-invariant along the horizontal meridian. This spatial version of Weber's law was modeled by a magnification function using an exponential equation. The slope of this function is much shallower than those currently known for V1, V4 and V5. We conclude that a centripetal bias exists for hemifield line bisection and that this bias likely contributes to the contralateral bias of line bisection by hemianopic patients found in other studies.


Subject(s)
Hemianopsia/physiopathology , Visual Fields , Visual Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Theoretical
7.
Perception ; 28(10): 1231-42, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10694971

ABSTRACT

Under certain conditions, high-contrast moving figures induce adjacent illusory regions, 'wakes' and 'spokes', which have contrast polarity opposite the inducing figures. In this paper we document properties of these novel phenomena. When the illusions are induced by a moving bar, spokes appear on the side of the bar closer to fixation and connect the bar to the fixation point, regardless of the momentary position of the bar or whether it is moving to the left or to the right. Although spokes often extend up to the fixation point, they never extend beyond it. This is not due to blocking of the spoke's spread by the fixation point, because in another experiment spokes extend directly through an intervening figure. Whereas spokes emanate from the end of a horizontally moving bar closest to fixation, wakes emanate from the end farthest from fixation. In contrast to spokes, wakes do not show a towards-fixation bias. Instead, the wake's end trails the position of the bar, like a ship's wake. The higher the bar velocity, the more the end of the wake appears to trail it, suggesting that wakes are caused by a process which spreads from the edge of moving figures. Wakes and spokes, as distinct illusions, should provide significant constraints on theories of human motion and brightness perception processes.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity , Motion , Optical Illusions , Humans , Psychological Tests
8.
Nat Neurosci ; 1(3): 242-7, 1998 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10195150

ABSTRACT

It is widely held that color and motion are processed by separate parallel pathways in the visual system, but this view is difficult to reconcile with the fact that motion can be detected in equiluminant stimuli that are defined by color alone. To examine the relationship between color and motion, we tested three patients who had lost their color vision following cortical damage (central achromatopsia). Despite their profound loss in the subjective experience of color and their inability to detect the motion of faint colors, all three subjects showed surprisingly strong responses to high-contrast, moving color stimuli--equal in all respects to the performance of subjects with normal color vision. The pathway from opponent-color detectors in the retina to the motion analysis areas must therefore be independent of the damaged color centers in the occipitotemporal area. It is probably also independent of the motion analysis area MT/V5, because the contribution of color to motion detection in these patients is much stronger than the color response of monkey area MT.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Color Vision Defects/physiopathology , Motion Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reference Values
9.
Vision Res ; 37(3): 273-81, 1997 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9135861

ABSTRACT

Using a perceptual learning paradigm, we evaluated whether information from the attributes: color, luminance and motion is combined to provide orientation coding. Four observers were trained to discriminate the orientation between color-defined bars, four between luminance-defined bars, and four between motion-defined bars. Before and after training, they were tested with each of the three attributes separately and all superimposed, at the same and at a different location as the one seen during training. A similar improvement was found whether the bars seen after training were defined by the same, or by a different attribute as the one seen during training, or by the three attributes superimposed. This improvement was significantly more substantial at the location where the bars were presented during training. Moreover, orientation discrimination was always better when the bars were defined by three attributes than by any one alone. Because the improvement was retinotopic and not restricted to the attribute seen during training, we suggest that training changed the sensitivity of orientation-selective cells responsive to color, luminance and motion. Moreover, the overall better performance with additional attributes supported an integration of information from color, luminance, and motion at a common site for orientation coding.


Subject(s)
Learning , Visual Perception/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Color Perception/physiology , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Visual Cortex/physiology
10.
Spat Vis ; 11(1): 33-41, 1997.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9304751

ABSTRACT

A method that allows the creation of moving random-dot patterns with any desired level of motion coherence is presented. In the present context, a random-dot pattern with 100% coherence is a sliding sheet of dots (all dots move at the same speed and in the same direction). A random-dot pattern with 0% coherence is visual dynamic noise (similar to detuned TV snow). Coherences between 100% and 0% have a specific percentage of the dots moving coherently together, while the remaining 'twinkle' in a random fashion. The use of look-up table animation allows the dot patterns to move smoothly and rapidly even on computers that lack computational speed. Additionally, the coherence of the motion in a particular region of the screen is determined by the spatial arrangement of pixel indices in that local region. Therefore, this technique allows different regions of the screen to have different levels of coherence which makes the creation of motion-defined shapes straightforward. The basics of look-up table animation are discussed and a general algorithm for creating motion with any level of coherence is presented.


Subject(s)
Computer Terminals , Motion Perception/physiology , Algorithms , Color Perception/physiology , Computer Graphics , Computer Storage Devices , Humans , Photic Stimulation
11.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 1(3): 115-21, 1997 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21223875

ABSTRACT

Attention can enhance selectively the visual information processing of particular locations or objects. Recent studies have shown that this enhancement has limited spatial resolution, the smallest regions that can be isolated by attention are much coarser than the smallest details that can be resolved by vision. Multiple similar objects spaced more finely than the limit of attentional resolution cannot be individuated for further processing and can only be perceived as a grouped texture. As a result, at any given time, only part of the spatial and temporal information registered by the early sensory systems is available to conscious perception. It is likely that attentional resolution is limited at a stage beyond V1 and that it has a finer grain in the lower visual field than in the upper field. The spatial aperture of attention is elongated along the radial axis relative to fixation. The briefest temporal window of attention is also much broader than visual temporal resolution. Many perceptual phenomena related to rapid serial visual presentation may reflect the limited temporal resolution of attention.

12.
Nature ; 383(6598): 334-7, 1996 Sep 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8848045

ABSTRACT

Visual spatial resolution is limited by factors ranging from optics to neuronal filters in the visual cortex, but it is not known to what extent it is also limited by the resolving power of attention. To investigate this, we studied adaptation to lines of specific orientation, a process that occurs in primary visual cortex. When a single grating is presented in the periphery of the visual field, human observers are aware of its orientation, but when it is flanked by other similar gratings ('crowding'), its orientation becomes impossible to discern. Nevertheless, we show that orientation-specific adaptation is not affected by crowding, implying that spatial resolution is limited by an attentional filter acting beyond the primary visual cortex. Consistent with this, we find that attentional resolution is greater in the lower than in the upper visual field, whereas there is no corresponding asymmetry in the primary visual cortex. We suggest that the attentional filter acts in one or more higher visual cortical areas to restrict the availability of visual information to conscious awareness.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Awareness/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Space Perception/physiology
13.
Neuroreport ; 7(3): 786-8, 1996 Feb 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8733745

ABSTRACT

Two different types of reading, one in each hemifield, were exhibited by a patient with a lesion of the posterior half of the corpus callosum. The patient read normally when words and non-words were presented to his right visual field. However, with left visual field presentations, the patient could not read non-words and vocalized real words very slowly, especially abstract words, inflected verbs and function words. He often replaced concrete words by semantic associates. Such an abnormal reading pattern is similar to that known as deep dyslexia. This unilateral deficit reveals the competence of the right hemisphere to initiate some semantic processing and its inability to manage phonological coding. The hypothesis that deep dyslexia is due to right hemisphere reading is reinforced by the present case.


Subject(s)
Corpus Callosum/physiology , Dyslexia, Acquired/physiopathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Reading , Adult , Corpus Callosum/pathology , Corpus Callosum/surgery , Dyslexia, Acquired/pathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Visual Fields/physiology
15.
Biol Psychol ; 37(3): 207-18, 1994 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7948466

ABSTRACT

Background EEG was recorded from 24 subjects under eyes open/closed conditions for 5 min. The P3(00) event-related brain potential (ERP) was elicited with auditory stimuli when eyes were open/closed in the same subjects. Target stimulus probability was manipulated (0.20, 0.40, 0.60, 0.80) in different blocks under each eyes open/closed condition. Spectral analysis indicated that EEG power between 8 and 12 Hz demonstrated a similar scalp distribution as the P3 component of the ERP for the electrode sites employed. Spectral power and mean frequency were modestly correlated with P3 amplitude and peak latency primarily in the slower EEG bands, with associations observed across probability conditions and often strongest when target stimulus probability was .20. The results suggest that differences between individuals for EEG variation may contribute to P3 component variability, especially at the parietal recording site and under low target stimulus probability conditions when the P3 is largest and most stable.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Attention/physiology , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Alpha Rhythm , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Female , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Individuality , Male , Probability Learning , Reference Values , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted
16.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 4(1): 96-105, 1992.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23967860

ABSTRACT

We distinguish between strong and weak cognitive neuropsychology, with the former attempting to provide direct insights into the nature of information processing and the latter having the more modest goal of providing constraints on such theories. We argue that strong cognitive neuropsychology, although possible, is unlikely to succeed and that researchers will fare better by combining behavioral, computational, and neural investigations. Arguments offered by Caramazza (1992) in defense of strong neuropsychology are analyzed, and examples are offered to illustrate the power of alternative points of view.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...