Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 81
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Oral Rehabil ; 41(5): 367-73, 2014 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24612326

ABSTRACT

Jaw actions adapt to the changing properties of food that occur during a masticatory sequence. In the present study, we investigated how the time-varying activation profile of the masseter muscle changes during natural chewing in humans and how food hardness affects the profile. We recorded surface electromyography (EMG) of the masseter muscle together with the movement of the lower jaw in 14 healthy young adults (mean age 22) when chewing gelatin-based model food of two different hardness. The muscle activity and the jaw kinematics were analysed for different phases of the chewing cycles. The increase in the excitatory drive of the masseter muscle was biphasic during the jaw-closing phase showing early and late components. The transition between these components occurred approximately at the time of tooth-food contact. During the masticatory sequence, when the food was particularised, the size of the early component as well as the peak amplitude of the EMG significantly decreased along with a reduction in the duration of the jaw-closing phase. Except for amplitude scaling, food hardness did not appreciably affect the muscle's activation profile. In conclusion, when chewing food during natural conditions, masseter muscle activation adapted throughout the masticatory sequence, principally during the jaw-closing phase and influenced both early and late muscle activation components. Furthermore, the adaptation of jaw actions to food hardness was affected by amplitude scaling of the magnitude of the muscle activity throughout the masticatory sequence.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Electromyography , Masseter Muscle/physiology , Mastication/physiology , Temporal Muscle/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Food , Hardness , Humans , Male
2.
J Plast Reconstr Aesthet Surg ; 59(7): 706-16, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16782566

ABSTRACT

Understanding how the loss of digital sensibility affects manual dexterity could have important implications for rehabilitation after hand injury. We investigated precision grip function during lifting tasks in seven patients after hand replantation, in five after single digital nerve injury and in four volunteers subjected to digital anaesthesia. Using their affected hand, all participants could successfully lift test objects with parallel and vertical grip surfaces and they all reliably increased the grip force with increasing object weight (0.11-0.55 kg). However, the grip forces used were frequently significantly higher than those applied by the unaffected hand. This was partly due to participants compensating for loss of sensibility with high grip force safety margins against slips, and partly related to misalignments of the fingertips on the grasp surfaces. The latter was most prominent after hand replantation. In a second series of lifting experiments we changed the shape of the grip surfaces in order to investigate the participants' ability to adapt grip forces based on tactile recognition of object shape. An important finding from this series was that in patients with poor clinical outcomes, the contralateral unaffected hand tended to mirror the abnormal grasp patterns of the injured hand. This suggests that control strategies developed for the impaired hand can influence the control of the contralateral uninjured hand.


Subject(s)
Finger Injuries/surgery , Hand Strength/physiology , Hand/surgery , Peripheral Nerve Injuries , Replantation/methods , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Biomechanical Phenomena , Child , Finger Injuries/physiopathology , Finger Injuries/rehabilitation , Hand/physiopathology , Humans , Lifting , Middle Aged , Replantation/rehabilitation , Treatment Outcome
3.
J Neurophysiol ; 95(3): 1518-26, 2006 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16267110

ABSTRACT

Few studies have analyzed activity-induced changes in EMG activity in individual human motor units. We studied the changes in human thenar motor unit EMG that accompany the potentiation of twitch force and fatigue of tetanic force. Single motor unit EMG and force were recorded in healthy subjects in response to selective stimulation of their motor axons within the median nerve just above the elbow. Twitches were recorded before and after a series of pulse trains delivered at frequencies that varied between 5 and 100 Hz. This stimulation induced significant increases in EMG amplitude, duration, and area. However, in relative terms, all of these EMG changes were substantially smaller than the potentiation of twitch force. Another 2 min of stimulation (13 pulses at 40 Hz each second) induced additional potentiation of EMG amplitude, duration, and area, but the tetanic force from every unit declined. Thus activity-induced changes in human thenar motor unit EMG do not indicate the alterations in force or vice versa. These data suggest that different processes underlie the changes in EMG and force that occur during human thenar motor unit activity.


Subject(s)
Action Potentials/physiology , Hand/physiology , Long-Term Potentiation/physiology , Motor Activity/physiology , Motor Neurons/physiology , Muscle Contraction/physiology , Muscle Fatigue/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Electric Stimulation , Electromyography/methods , Humans , Muscle Fibers, Skeletal/physiology , Stress, Mechanical
4.
J Neurosci ; 21(20): 8222-37, 2001 Oct 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11588194

ABSTRACT

In most manipulations, we use our fingertips to apply time-varying forces to the target object in controlled directions. Here we used microneurography to assess how single tactile afferents encode the direction of fingertip forces at magnitudes, rates, and directions comparable to those arising in everyday manipulations. Using a flat stimulus surface, we applied forces to a standard site on the fingertip while recording impulse activity in 196 tactile afferents with receptive fields distributed over the entire terminal phalanx. Forces were applied in one of five directions: normal force and forces at a 20 degrees angle from the normal in the radial, distal, ulnar, or proximal directions. Nearly all afferents responded, and the responses in most slowly adapting (SA)-I, SA-II, and fast adapting (FA)-I afferents were broadly tuned to a preferred direction of force. Among afferents of each type, the preferred directions were distributed in all angular directions with reference to the stimulation site, but not uniformly. The SA-I population was biased for tangential force components in the distal direction, the SA-II population was biased in the proximal direction, and the FA-I population was biased in the proximal and radial directions. Anisotropic mechanical properties of the fingertip and the spatial relationship between the receptive field center of the afferent and the stimulus site appeared to influence the preferred direction in a manner dependent on afferent type. We conclude that tactile afferents from the whole terminal phalanx potentially contribute to the encoding of direction of fingertip forces similar to those that occur when subjects manipulate objects under natural conditions.


Subject(s)
Fingers/physiology , Mechanoreceptors/physiology , Neurons, Afferent/physiology , Touch/physiology , Adult , Female , Fingers/diagnostic imaging , Fingers/innervation , Humans , Male , Median Nerve/physiology , Microelectrodes , Neurons, Afferent/classification , Physical Stimulation/instrumentation , Physical Stimulation/methods , Radiography , Reaction Time/physiology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Stress, Mechanical
5.
J Neurosci ; 21(17): 6917-32, 2001 Sep 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11517279

ABSTRACT

We analyzed the coordination between gaze behavior, fingertip movements, and movements of the manipulated object when subjects reached for and grasped a bar and moved it to press a target-switch. Subjects almost exclusively fixated certain landmarks critical for the control of the task. Landmarks at which contact events took place were obligatory gaze targets. These included the grasp site on the bar, the target, and the support surface where the bar was returned after target contact. Any obstacle in the direct movement path and the tip of the bar were optional landmarks. Subjects never fixated the hand or the moving bar. Gaze and hand/bar movements were linked concerning landmarks, with gaze leading. The instant that gaze exited a given landmark coincided with a kinematic event at that landmark in a manner suggesting that subjects monitored critical kinematic events for phasic verification of task progress and subgoal completion. For both the obstacle and target, subjects directed saccades and fixations to sites that were offset from the physical extension of the objects. Fixations related to an obstacle appeared to specify a location around which the extending tip of the bar should travel. We conclude that gaze supports hand movement planning by marking key positions to which the fingertips or grasped object are subsequently directed. The salience of gaze targets arises from the functional sensorimotor requirements of the task. We further suggest that gaze control contributes to the development and maintenance of sensorimotor correlation matrices that support predictive motor control in manipulation.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Hand/physiology , Motor Activity/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Female , Fingers/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Movement/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Space Perception/physiology
6.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 55(2): 87-95, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11433790

ABSTRACT

When people lift objects of different size but equal weight, they initially employ too much force for the large object and too little force for the small object. However, over repeated lifts of the two objects, they learn to suppress the size-weight association used to estimate force requirements and appropriately scale their lifting forces to the true and equal weights of the objects. Thus, sensorimotor memory from previous lifts comes to dominate visual size information in terms of force prediction. Here we ask whether this sensorimotor memory is transient, preserved only long enough to perform the task, or more stable. After completing an initial lift series in which they lifted equally weighted large and small objects in alternation, participants then repeated the lift series after delays of 15 minutes or 24 hours. In both cases, participants retained information about the weights of the objects and used this information to predict the appropriate fingertip forces. This preserved sensorimotor memory suggests that participants acquired internal models of the size-weight stimuli that could be used for later prediction.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Weight Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Hand/physiology , Humans , Male , Time Factors , Visual Perception/physiology
7.
J Neurophysiol ; 84(6): 2984-97, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11110826

ABSTRACT

Most objects that we manipulate have curved surfaces. We have analyzed how subjects during a prototypical manipulatory task use visual and tactile sensory information for adapting fingertip actions to changes in object curvature. Subjects grasped an elongated object at one end using a precision grip and lifted it while instructed to keep it level. The principal load of the grasp was tangential torque due to the location of the center of mass of the object in relation to the horizontal grip axis joining the centers of the opposing grasp surfaces. The curvature strongly influenced the grip forces required to prevent rotational slips. Likewise the curvature influenced the rotational yield of the grasp that developed under the tangential torque load due to the viscoelastic properties of the fingertip pulps. Subjects scaled the grip forces parametrically with object curvature for grasp stability. Moreover in a curvature-dependent manner, subjects twisted the grasp around the grip axis by a radial flexion of the wrist to keep the desired object orientation despite the rotational yield. To adapt these fingertip actions to object curvature, subjects could use both vision and tactile sensibility integrated with predictive control. During combined blindfolding and digital anesthesia, however, the motor output failed to predict the consequences of the prevailing curvature. Subjects used vision to identify the curvature for efficient feedforward retrieval of grip force requirements before executing the motor commands. Digital anesthesia caused little impairment of grip force control when subjects had vision available, but the adaptation of the twist became delayed. Visual cues about the form of the grasp surface obtained before contact was used to scale the grip force, whereas the scaling of the twist depended on visual cues related to object movement. Thus subjects apparently relied on different visuomotor mechanisms for adaptation of grip force and grasp kinematics. In contrast, blindfolded subjects used tactile cues about the prevailing curvature obtained after contact with the object for feedforward adaptation of both grip force and twist. We conclude that humans use both vision and tactile sensibility for feedforward parametric adaptation of grip forces and grasp kinematics to object curvature. Normal control of the twist action, however, requires digital afferent input, and different visuomotor mechanisms support the control of the grasp twist and the grip force. This differential use of vision may have a bearing to the two-stream model of human visual processing.


Subject(s)
Fingers/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Stereognosis/physiology , Touch/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Friction , Humans , Kinesis/physiology , Lifting , Male , Task Performance and Analysis , Torque
8.
J Physiol ; 527 Pt 3: 641-52, 2000 Sep 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10990548

ABSTRACT

It has been shown that subjects maintain grasp stability by automatically regulating grip force in response to loads applied tangentially to a manipulandum held using a precision grip. Signals from cutaneous mechanoreceptors convey the information necessary for both the initiation and scaling of responses. The central neural pathways that support these grip reactions are unknown. However, the latency of the increase in force is similar to that of 'long-latency' transcortical reflexes recorded from muscles following muscle stretch or electrical stimulation of digital nerves. This study assessed the importance of fast transcortical pathways for reactive grip responses by examining these responses in subjects with X-linked Kallmann's syndrome (XKS). Subjects were selected whose corticospinal projection, as assessed by magnetic brain stimulation, is essentially ipsilateral, and in whom the long-latency reflex components following digital nerve stimulation are only found contralateral to the stimulated side. Despite this anomaly of the fast corticospinal pathway, these XKS subjects responded in the same way as control subjects; grip response latencies were similar and responses were appropriately scaled. However, the non-operating hand of these XKS subjects often mirrored the grip force changes of the operating hand. Reflex force mirroring was most marked during the first 50 ms and the force output was always less than 20 % of that of the operating hand. We conclude, firstly, that somatosensory driven precision grip responses that support grasp stability do not depend on fast conducting corticospinal pathways in these subjects and, secondly, that such responses do not use those 'long-latency' reflex pathways probed by cutaneomuscular reflexes elicited by electrical stimulation of digital nerves.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Kallmann Syndrome/physiopathology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Reflex/physiology , Adult , Aged , Electric Stimulation , Electromyography , Female , Fingers/innervation , Fingers/physiology , Genetic Linkage/genetics , Humans , Kallmann Syndrome/genetics , Male , Mechanoreceptors/physiology , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/physiology , Reflex, Stretch/physiology , X Chromosome/genetics
9.
Mov Disord ; 15(2): 244-54, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10752573

ABSTRACT

In previous studies of fingertip forces during precision grip in subjects with Parkinson's disease (PD), we observed regular oscillations in isometric force. The present study characterizes the nature of these oscillations. Fingertip forces were recorded from the index finger and thumb during precision grip-lifts with a 300 g and 900 g object in 10 subjects with PD and 20 healthy control subjects. Fourier analysis confirmed that all subjects with PD exhibited force oscillations with a clearly definable frequency (approximately 7-11 Hz). Five of these subjects also exhibited a second lower frequency peak (approximately 5 Hz). Approximately half of the 20 control subjects displayed a single frequency peak (approximately 8-12 Hz), which was generally lower in amplitude than in the subjects with PD (representing enhanced physiological tremor), whereas the remaining control subjects had low-amplitude, broad-based spectra (representing physiological tremor). The amplitude of the force oscillations was higher for lifts with the heavier object in both the control subjects and subjects with PD. L-Dopa resulted in a decreased tremor amplitude but did not influence the frequency. The force oscillations of the two opposing digits normal to the grip surfaces were in phase, whereas the oscillations tangential to the grip surfaces were often out of phase. The results suggest that the multipeaked force rate trajectories reported previously are caused by action tremor. The similarity of force oscillations in subjects with PD and healthy control subjects suggests common tremor-generating mechanisms and supports the notion that the parkinsonian action tremor (AT) is an exaggerated form of physiological tremor. These findings provide insight into the impaired hand function observed in individuals with PD.


Subject(s)
Motor Skills/physiology , Parkinson Disease/diagnosis , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Tremor/diagnosis , Adult , Aged , Female , Fourier Analysis , Hand Strength/physiology , Humans , Isometric Contraction/physiology , Male , Middle Aged , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted/instrumentation , Transducers, Pressure , Tremor/physiopathology , Weight-Bearing/physiology
10.
J Neurophysiol ; 83(1): 528-36, 2000 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10634893

ABSTRACT

Most manual grips can be divided in precision and power grips on the basis of phylogenetic and functional considerations. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to compare human brain activity during force production by the right hand when subjects used a precision grip and a power grip. During the precision-grip task, subjects applied fine grip forces between the tips of the index finger and the thumb. During the power-grip task, subjects squeezed a cylindrical object using all digits in a palmar opposition grasp. The activity recorded in the primary sensory and motor cortex contralateral to the operating hand was higher when the power grip was applied than when subjects applied force with a precision grip. In contrast, the activity in the ipsilateral ventral premotor area, the rostral cingulate motor area, and at several locations in the posterior parietal and prefrontal cortices was stronger while making the precision grip than during the power grip. The power grip was associated predominately with contralateral left-sided activity, whereas the precision-grip task involved extensive activations in both hemispheres. Thus our findings indicate that in addition to the primary motor cortex, premotor and parietal areas are important for control of fingertip forces during precision grip. Moreover, the ipsilateral hemisphere appears to be strongly engaged in the control of precision-grip tasks performed with the right hand.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Isometric Contraction/physiology , Adult , Fingers , Functional Laterality , Hand , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Motor Cortex/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Posture , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Thalamus/physiology
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 82(6): 3188-95, 1999 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10601452

ABSTRACT

We assessed the sequence of nerve impulses that maximize force output from individual human thenar motor units. When these motor units were stimulated intraneurally by a variable sequence of seven pulses, the pattern of pulses that elicited maximum force always started with a short (5-15 ms) interpulse interval termed a "doublet. " The twitch force summation caused by this "doublet" elicited, on average, 48 +/- 13% (SD) of the maximum tetanic force. The peak amplitude of "doublet" forces was 3.5 times that of the initial twitches, and twitch potentiation appeared to have little influence on twitch force summation elicited by the "doublets." For some units, the second optimal interpulse interval was also short. Peak forces elicited by the third to sixth interpulse intervals did not change substantially when the last interpulse interval was varied between 5 to 55 ms, so maximum force could not be attributed to any unique interpulse interval. Each successive pulse contributed a smaller force increment. When five to seven pulses were delivered in an optimal sequence, the evoked force was close to that recorded during maximal tetanic stimulation. In contrast, maximal force-time integral was evoked with one short interpulse interval (5-15 ms) then substantially longer interpulse intervals (>100 ms). Maximum force and force-time integrals were therefore elicited by different patterns of stimuli. We conclude that a brief initial interpulse interval (5-15 ms) is required to elicit maximum "doublet" force from human thenar motor units and that near-maximal tetanic forces can be elicited by only five or six additional post-"doublet" pulses if appropriately spaced in time. However, the rate at which these post-"doublet" stimuli must be provided is fairly uncritical. In contrast, maximum post-"doublet" force-time integrals were obtained at intervals corresponding to motoneuronal firing rates of approximately 7 Hz, rates close to that typically used to recruit motor units and to maintain weak voluntary contractions.


Subject(s)
Motor Neurons/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Adult , Axons/physiology , Electric Stimulation , Electromyography , Electrophysiology , Female , Humans , Male , Muscle Contraction/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/cytology , Muscle, Skeletal/innervation
12.
J Neurophysiol ; 82(5): 2393-405, 1999 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10561413

ABSTRACT

Control of grasp stability under different frictional conditions has primarily been studied in manipulatory tasks involving two digits only. Recently we found that many of the principles for control of forces originally demonstrated for two-digit grasping also apply to various three-digit grasps. Here we examine the control of grasp stability in a multidigit task in which subjects used the tips of the thumb, index, and middle finger to lift an object. The grasp resembled those used when lifting a cylindrical object from above. The digits either all contacted the same surface material or one of the digits contacted a surface material that was more, or less, slippery than that contacted by the other two digits. The three-dimensional forces and torques applied by each digit and the contact positions were measured along with the position and orientation of the object. The distribution of forces among the digits strongly reflected constraints imposed by the geometric relationship between the object's center of mass and the contact surfaces. On top of this distribution, we observed changes in force coordination related to changes in the combination of surface materials. When all digits contacted the same surface material, the ratio between the normal force and tangential load (F(n):L ratio) was similar across digits and scaled to provide an adequate safety margin against slip. With different contact surfaces subjects adapted the F(n):L ratios at the individual digits to the local friction with only small influences by the friction at the other two digits. They accomplished this by scaling the normal forces similarly at all digits and changing the distribution of load among the digits. The surface combination did not, however, influence digit position, tangential torque, or object tilting systematically. The change in load distribution, rather, resulted from interplay between these factors, and the nature of this interplay varied between trials. That is, subjects achieved grasp stability with various combinations of fingertip actions and appeared to exploit the many degrees of freedom offered by the multidigit grasp. The results extend previous findings based on two-digit tasks to multidigit tasks by showing that subjects adjust fingertip forces at each digit to the local friction. Moreover, our findings suggest that subjects adapted the load distribution to the current frictional condition by regulating the normal forces to allow slips to occur early in the lift task, prior to object lift-off.


Subject(s)
Fingers/innervation , Hand Strength/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Stress, Mechanical
13.
Exp Brain Res ; 128(1-2): 20-30, 1999 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10473736

ABSTRACT

We analyzed the control of grasp stability during a major manipulative function of the human hand: rotation of a grasped object by pronation and supination movement. We investigated the regulation of grip forces used to stabilize an object held by a precision grip between the thumb and index finger when subjects rotated it around a horizontal axis. Because the center of mass was located distal to the grip axis joining the fingertips, destabilizing torque tangential to the grasp surfaces developed when the grip axis rotated relative to the field of gravity. The torque load was maximal when the grip axis was horizontal and minimal when it was vertical. An instrumented test object, with a mass distribution that resulted in substantial changes in torque load during the rotation task, measured forces and torques applied by the digits. The mass distribution of the object was unpredictably changed between trials. The grip force required to stabilize the object increased directly with increasing torque load. Importantly, the grip force used by the subjects also changed in proportion to the torque load such that subjects always employed adequate safety margins against rotational slips, i.e., some 20-40% of the grip force. Rather than driven by sensory feedback pertaining to the torque load, the changes in grip force were generated as an integral part of the motor commands that accounted for the rotation movement. Subjects changed the grip force in parallel with, or even slightly ahead of, the rotation movement, whereas grip force responses elicited by externally imposed torque load changes were markedly delayed. Moreover, blocking sensory information from the digits did not appreciably change the coordination between movement and grip force. We thus conclude that the grip force was controlled by feedforward rather than by feedback mechanisms. These feedforward mechanisms would thus predict the consequences of the rotation movement in terms of changes in fingertip loads when the orientation of the grip axis changed in the field of gravity. Changes in the object's center of mass between trials resulted in a parametric scaling of the motor commands prior to their execution. This finding suggests that the sensorimotor memories used in manipulation to adapt the motor output for the physical properties of environmental objects also encompass information related to an object's center of mass. This information was obtained by somatosensory cues when subjects initially grasped the object with the grip axis vertical, i. e., during minimum tangential torque load.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Motor Skills/physiology , Movement/physiology , Pronation/physiology , Supination/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Reference Values , Rotation , Torque
14.
Exp Brain Res ; 127(1): 43-53, 1999 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10424413

ABSTRACT

The tuning of fingertip forces to the physical properties of objects during manipulation may be controlled partly by digit-specific mechanisms using local afferent information and partly by controllers that support interdigital coordination and use sensory information from more than one digit. In the present study we addressed digital interactions when humans used the tips of two fingers to restrain a pair of horizontally oriented plates from moving when subjected to tangential force loads in the distal direction. Subjects used the right and left index fingers in a bimanual grasp, and the right index and middle fingers in an unimanual grasp. The plates were loaded at unpredictable times by identical force profiles consisting of a force increase of up to a 3-N force plateau. The plates were concurrently loaded in 85% of the trials and each plate was loaded separately in 7% of the trials. For each plate, we measured its movement and the normal and tangential forces applied by the finger to restrain it. When a finger was loaded, the subject automatically responded by a normal force increase to a level that remained fairly constant during the subsequent load plateau. The initial part of this finger grasp response was affected by simultaneous loading of its partner finger; the magnitude of the response was boosted with a bimanual grasp, whereas the onset latency tended to be shorter with a unimanual grasp. Responses also occurred at a non-loaded finger during both bimanual and unimanual grasps, but these responses were weaker than those evoked when the same finger was loaded. In the bimanual grasp, they were largely characterized by a brief force pulse whose onset was delayed by some 15 ms compared with the response onset of the loaded finger, i.e., there was no sustained response. In the unimanual grasp, the onset of the response coincided in time with that of the accompanying (loaded) finger, and the dynamic response was stronger and prolonged, with more than one force rate peak. There was also a significant static response present. We conclude that during unimanual as well as bimanual reactive restrain tasks there are interactions between digits engaged in terms of neural control that facilitate the response of a digit when an accompanying digit is simultaneously loaded. However, digit-specific afferent inputs are necessary for eliciting the full-size reactive grasp responses required to successfully restrain the manipulandum.


Subject(s)
Fingers/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Adult , Female , Fingers/innervation , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
15.
J Neurophysiol ; 81(4): 1706-17, 1999 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10200206

ABSTRACT

Previous studies of control of fingertip forces in skilled manipulation have focused on tasks involving two digits, typically the thumb and index finger. Here we examine control of fingertip actions in a multidigit task in which subjects lifted an object using unimanual and bimanual grasps engaging the tips of the thumb and two fingers. The grasps resembled those used when lifting a cylindrical object from above; the two fingers were some 4.25 cm apart and the thumb was approximately 5.54 cm from either finger. The three-dimensional forces and torques applied by each digit and the digit contact positions were measured along with the position and orientation of the object. The vertical forces applied tangential to the grasp surfaces to lift the object were synchronized across the digits, and the contribution by each digit to the total vertical force reflected intrinsic object properties (geometric relationship between the object's center of mass and the grasped surfaces). Subjects often applied small torques tangential to the grasped surfaces even though the object could have been lifted without such torques. The normal forces generated by each digit increased in parallel with the local tangential load (force and torque), providing an adequate safety margin against slips at each digit. In the present task, the orientations of the force vectors applied by the separate digits were not fully constrained and therefore the motor controller had to choose from a number of possible solutions. Our findings suggest that subjects attempt to minimize (or at least reduce) fingertip forces while at the same time ensure that grasp stability is preserved. Subjects also avoid horizontal tangential forces, even at a small cost in total force. Moreover, there were subtle actions exerted by the digits that included changes in the distribution of vertical forces across digits and slight object tilt. It is not clear to what extent the brain explicitly controlled these actions, but they could serve, for instance, to keep tangential torques small and to compensate for variations in digit contact positions. In conclusion, we have shown that when lifting an object with a three-digit grip, the coordination of fingertip forces, in many respects, matches what has been documented previously for two-digit grasping. At the same time, our study reveals novel aspects of force control that emerge only in multidigit manipulative tasks.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Thumb/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Humans , Kinetics , Male , Middle Aged , Torque , Touch/physiology , Weight-Bearing/physiology
16.
J Neurosci ; 18(24): 10724-34, 1998 Dec 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9852607

ABSTRACT

When we manipulate objects in everyday tasks, there are variations in the shape of the grasped surfaces, and the loads that potentially destabilize the grasp include time-varying linear forces and torques tangential to the grasped surfaces. Previous studies of the control of fingertip forces for grasp stability have dealt principally with flat grip surfaces and linear force loads. Here, we studied the regulation of grip force with changes in curvature of grasped surfaces and changes in tangential torque applied by the index finger and thumb when humans lifted an object and rotated it about the horizontal grip axis through an angle of 65 degrees. The curvatures of the matched pair of spherical surfaces varied from -50 m-1 (concave with radius 20 mm) to 200 m-1 (convex with radius 5 mm). The applied tangential torque at the orientation of 65 degrees was varied sixfold. Regardless of the values of curvature and end torque, grip force and tangential torque were coordinated, increasing in parallel throughout the tilt with an approximately linear relationship; the slope of the line increased progressively with increasing surface curvature. This parametric scaling of grip force was directly related to the minimum grip force required to prevent rotational slip, resulting in an adequate safety margin against slip in all cases. We conclude that surface curvature parametrically influences grip force regulation when the digits are exposed to torsional loads. Furthermore, the sensorimotor programs that control the grip force apparently predict the effect of the total load comprising linear forces and tangential torques.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Movement/physiology , Torque , Adult , Female , Friction , Hand/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Rotation , Surface Properties
17.
J Neurophysiol ; 80(4): 1989-2002, 1998 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9772255

ABSTRACT

Previous studies on adaptation of fingertip forces to local friction at individual digit-object interfaces largely focused on static phases of manipulative tasks in which humans could rely on anticipatory control based on the friction in previous trials. Here we instead analyze mechanisms underlying this adaptation after unpredictable changes in local friction between consecutive trials. With the tips of the right index and middle fingers or the right and left index fingers, subjects restrained a manipulandum whose horizontal contact surfaces were located side by side. At unpredictable moments a tangential force was applied to the contact surfaces in the distal direction at 16 N/s to a plateau at 4 N. The subjects were free to use any combination of normal and tangential forces at the two fingers, but the sum of the tangential forces had to counterbalance the imposed load. The contact surface of the right index finger was fine-grained sandpaper, whereas that of the cooperating finger was changed between sandpaper and the more slippery rayon. The load increase automatically triggered normal force responses at both fingers. When a finger contacted rayon, subjects allowed slips to occur at this finger during the load force increase instead of elevating the normal force. These slips accounted for a partitioning of the load force between the digits that resulted in an adequate adjustment of the normal:tangential force ratios to the local friction at each digit. This mechanism required a fine control of the normal forces. Although the normal force at the more slippery surface had to be comparatively low to allow slippage, the normal forces applied by the nonslipping digit at the same time had to be high enough to prevent loss of the manipulandum. The frictional changes influenced the normal forces applied before the load ramp as well as the size of the triggered normal force responses similarly at both fingers, that is, with rayon at one contact surface the normal forces increased at both fingers. Thus to independently adapt fingertip forces to the local friction the normal forces were controlled at an interdigital level by using sensory information from both engaged digits. Furthermore, subjects used both short- and long-term anticipatory mechanisms in a manner consistent with the notion that the central nervous system (CNS) entertains internal models of relevant object and task properties during manipulation.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Fingers/physiology , Hand Strength/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Friction , Hand/physiology , Humans , Male , Models, Biological
18.
J Neurophysiol ; 79(4): 1643-52, 1998 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9535935

ABSTRACT

In previous investigations of the control of grasp stability, humans manipulated test objects with flat grasp surfaces. The surfaces of most objects that we handle in everyday activities, however, are curved. In the present study, we examined the influence of surface curvature on the fingertip forces used when humans lifted and held objects of various weights. Subjects grasped the test object between the thumb and the index finger. The matching pair of grasped surfaces were spherically curved with one of six different curvatures (concave with radius 20 or 40 mm; flat; convex with radius 20, 10, or 5 mm) and the object had one of five different weights ranging from 168 to 705 g. The grip force used by subjects (force along the axis between the 2 grasped surfaces) increased with increasing weight of the object but was modified inconsistently and incompletely by surface curvature. Similarly, the duration and rate of force generation, when the grip and load forces increased isometrically in the load phase before object lift-off, were not influenced by surface curvature. In contrast, surface curvature did affect the minimum grip forces required to prevent frictional slips (the slip force). The slip force was smaller for larger curvatures (both concave and convex) than for flatter surfaces. Therefore the force safety margin against slips (difference between the employed grip force and the slip force) was higher for the higher curvatures. We conclude that surface curvature has little influence on grip force regulation during this type of manipulation; the moderate changes in slip force resulting from changes in curvature are not fully compensated for by changes in grip force.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Lifting , Adult , Female , Fingers , Friction , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reference Values , Surface Properties , Weight-Bearing
19.
Novartis Found Symp ; 218: 45-59; discussion 59-63, 1998.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9949815

ABSTRACT

When we use our digits to manipulate objects the applied fingertip forces and torques tangential to the grip surfaces are a result of complex muscle activity. These patterns are acquired during our ontogenetic development and we select them according to the manipulative intent. But the basic force coordination expressed in these patterns has to be tuned to the physical properties of the current object, e.g. shape, surface friction and weight. This takes place primarily by parametric adjustments of the force output based on internal models of the target object, i.e. implicit memory systems that represent critical object properties. From visual or haptic information we identify objects and automatically retrieve the relevant models. These models are then used to adapt the motor commands prior to their execution. The formation of models and their swift updating with changes in object properties depend, however, on signals from tactile sensors in the fingertips.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Neurons, Afferent/physiology , Touch/physiology , Feedback/physiology , Humans
20.
J Neurophysiol ; 78(3): 1619-30, 1997 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9310447

ABSTRACT

When we manipulate small objects, our fingertips are generally subjected to tangential torques about the axis normal to the grasp surface in addition to linear forces tangential to the grasp surface. Tangential torques can arise because the normal force is distributed across the contact area rather than focused at a point. We investigated the effects of tangential torques and tangential forces on the minimum normal forces required to prevent slips (slip force) and on the normal forces actually employed by subjects to hold an object in a stationary position with the use of the tips of the index finger and thumb. By changing the location of the object's center of gravity in relation to the grasp surface, various levels of tangential torque (0-50 N x mm) were created while the subject counteracted object rotation. Tangential force (0-3.4 N) was varied by changing the weight of the object. The flat grasp surfaces were covered with rayon, suede, or sandpaper, providing differences in friction in relation to the skin. Under zero tangential force, both the employed normal force and the slip force increased in proportion to tangential torque with a slope that reflected the current frictional condition. Likewise, with pure tangential force, these forces increased in proportion to tangential force. The effects of combined tangential torques and tangential forces on the slip force were primarily additive, but there was a significant interaction of these variables. Specifically, the increase in slip force for a given increment in torque decreases as a function of tangential force. A mathematical model was developed that successfully predicted slip force from tangential torque, tangential force, and an estimate of coefficient of static friction in the digit-surface interface. The effects of combined tangential torques and forces on the employed normal force showed the same pattern as the effects on the slip force. The safety margin against frictional slips, measured as the difference between the employed normal force and the slip force, was relatively small and constant across all tangential force and torque levels except at small torques (< 10 N x mm). There was no difference in safety margin between the digits. In conclusion, tangential torque strongly influences the normal force required for grasp stability. When controlling normal force, people take into account, in a precise fashion, the slip force reflecting both tangential force and tangential torque and their interaction as well as the current frictional condition in the object-digit interface.


Subject(s)
Hand Strength/physiology , Hand/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Adult , Data Collection , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Fingers/innervation , Fingers/physiology , Hand/innervation , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Neurological , Muscle, Skeletal/innervation , Skin/innervation
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...