Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 19 de 19
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 70(5): 874-889, 2017 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27809666

ABSTRACT

Many models of face recognition incorporate the idea of a face recognition unit (FRU), an abstracted representation formed from each experience of a face which aids recognition under novel viewing conditions. Some previous studies have failed to find evidence of this FRU representation. Here, we report three experiments which investigated this theoretical construct by modifying the face learning procedure from that in previous work. During learning, one or two views of previously unfamiliar faces were shown to participants in a serial matching task. Later, participants attempted to recognize both seen and novel views of the learned faces (recognition phase). Experiment 1 tested participants' recognition of a novel view, a day after learning. Experiment 2 was identical, but tested participants on the same day as learning. Experiment 3 repeated Experiment 1, but tested participants on a novel view that was outside the rotation of those views learned. Results revealed a significant advantage, across all experiments, for recognizing a novel view when two views had been learned compared to single view learning. The observed view invariance supports the notion that an FRU representation is established during multi-view face learning under particular learning conditions.


Subject(s)
Face , Learning/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Drug Combinations , Ethinyl Estradiol , Female , Humans , Male , Norethindrone , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
2.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 70(2): 298-315, 2017 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27079768

ABSTRACT

Developmental prosopagnosia (DP) is commonly associated with the failure to properly perceive individuating facial properties, notably those conveying configural or holistic content. While this may indicate that the primary impairment is perceptual, it is conceivable that some cases of DP are instead caused by a memory impairment, with any perceptual complaint merely allied rather than causal. To investigate this possibility, we administered a battery of face perception tasks to 11 individuals who reported that their face recognition difficulties disrupt daily activity and who also performed poorly on two formal tests of face recognition. Group statistics identified, relative to age- and gender-matched controls, difficulties in apprehending global-local relations and the holistic properties of faces, and in matching across viewpoints, but these were mild in nature and were not consistently evident at the level of individual participants. Six of the 11 individuals failed to show any evidence of perceptual impairment. In the remaining five individuals, no single perceptual deficit, or combination of deficits, was necessary or sufficient for poor recognition performance. These data suggest that some cases of DP are better explained by a memorial rather than perceptual deficit, and highlight the relevance of the apperceptive/associative distinction more commonly applied to the allied syndrome of acquired prosopagnosia.


Subject(s)
Face , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Prosopagnosia/physiopathology , Recognition, Psychology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Association , Facial Expression , Famous Persons , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Signal Detection, Psychological
4.
PeerJ ; 3: e1184, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26312179

ABSTRACT

In face matching, observers have to decide whether two photographs depict the same person or different people. This task is not only remarkably difficult but accuracy declines further during prolonged testing. The current study investigated whether this decline in long tasks can be eliminated with regular rest-breaks (Experiment 1) or room-switching (Experiment 2). Both experiments replicated the accuracy decline for long face-matching tasks and showed that this could not be eliminated with rest or room-switching. These findings suggest that person identification in applied settings, such as passport control, might be particularly error-prone due to the long and repetitive nature of the task. The experiments also show that it is difficult to counteract these problems.

5.
Sci Total Environ ; 505: 1282-90, 2015 Feb 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24726512

ABSTRACT

Private wells in the United States are unregulated for drinking water standards and are the homeowner's responsibility to test and treat. Testing for water quality parameters such as arsenic (As) is a crucial first step for homeowners to take protective actions. This study seeks to identify key behavioral factors influencing homeowners' decisions to take action after receiving well As test results. A January 2013 survey of central Maine households (n=386, 73% response) who were notified 3-7 years earlier that their well water contained As above 10 µg/L found that 43% of households report installing As treatment systems. Another 30% report taking other mitigation actions such as drinking bottled water because of the As, but the remaining 27% of households did not act. Well water As level appears to be a motivation for mitigation: 31% of households with well water level between 10 and 50 µg/L did not act, compared to 11% of households with well water >50 µg/L. The belief that the untreated water is not safe to drink (risk) and that reducing drinking water As would increase home value (instrumental attitude) were identified as significant predictors of mitigating As. Mitigating As exposure is associated with less worry about the As level (affective attitude), possibly because those acting to reduce exposure feel less worried about As. Use of a treatment system specifically was significantly predicted by confidence that one can maintain a treatment system, even if there are additional costs (self-efficacy). An assessment of As treatment systems used by 68 of these households with well water As >10 µg/L followed up within August-November 2013 found that 15% of treatment units failed to produce water below As 10 µg/L, suggesting that there are continued risks for exposure even after the decision is made to treat.


Subject(s)
Arsenic/analysis , Environmental Exposure/statistics & numerical data , Information Dissemination , Water Pollutants, Chemical/analysis , Water Wells/chemistry , Environmental Restoration and Remediation , Humans , Maine , Risk Assessment , Water Purification/methods , Water Supply/statistics & numerical data
6.
Front Psychol ; 4: 943, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24391609

ABSTRACT

Using Garner's speeded classification task existing studies demonstrated an asymmetric interference in the recognition of facial identity and facial expression. It seems that expression is hard to interfere with identity recognition. However, discriminability of identity and expression, a potential confounding variable, had not been carefully examined in existing studies. In current work, we manipulated discriminability of identity and expression by matching facial shape (long or round) in identity and matching mouth (opened or closed) in facial expression. Garner interference was found either from identity to expression (Experiment 1) or from expression to identity (Experiment 2). Interference was also found in both directions (Experiment 3) or in neither direction (Experiment 4). The results support that Garner interference tends to occur under condition of low discriminability of relevant dimension regardless of facial property. Our findings indicate that Garner interference is not necessarily related to interdependent processing in recognition of facial identity and expression. The findings also suggest that discriminability as a mediating factor should be carefully controlled in future research.

7.
Behav Res Methods ; 42(2): 461-9, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20479176

ABSTRACT

In the present study, we presented picture-naming latencies along with ratings for a set of important characteristics of pictures and picture names: age of acquisition, frequency, picture-name agreement, name agreement, visual complexity, familiarity, and word length. The validity of these data was established by calculating correlations with previous studies. Regression analyses show that our ratings account for a larger amount of variance in RTs than do previous data. RTs were predicted by all variables except complexity and length. A complete database presenting details about all of these variables is available in the supplemental materials, downloadable from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.


Subject(s)
Language Tests/statistics & numerical data , Language Tests/standards , Names , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time , Adult , Age Factors , England , Female , Humans , Language , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Recognition, Psychology , Visual Perception
8.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 63(7): 1304-17, 2010 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20127547

ABSTRACT

In two experiments participants named pictures superimposed with unrelated words. The age of acquisition (AoA) of the picture names was manipulated. Additionally, the word frequency (WF, Experiment 1) or AoA (Experiment 2) of the interfering distractor words was manipulated. Early-acquired pictures were named faster than their late-acquired counterparts. Both WF and AoA modulated the degree of interference from the irrelevant word; low-frequency and late-acquired words produced most interference. In neither case did the WF or AoA of the distractor word interact with the AoA of the picture. The results show that in the context of word processing both WF and AoA have similar effects.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Attention/physiology , Discrimination Learning , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Vocabulary , Analysis of Variance , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Students , Universities
9.
Perception ; 38(5): 702-7, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19662945

ABSTRACT

The face-processing skills of people with schizophrenia were compared with those of a group of unimpaired individuals. Participants were asked to make speeded face-classification decisions to faces previously rated as being typical or distinctive. The schizophrenic group responded more slowly than the unimpaired group; however, both groups demonstrated the customary sensitivity to the distinctiveness of the face stimuli. Face-classification latencies made to typical faces were shorter than those made to distinctive faces. The implication of this finding with the schizophrenic group is discussed with reference to accounts of face-processing deficits attributed to these individuals.


Subject(s)
Face , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Schizophrenic Psychology , Adult , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
10.
Memory ; 17(5): 577-96, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19548173

ABSTRACT

Since the 1970s there has been a continuing interest in how people recognise familiar faces (Bruce, 1979; Ellis, 1975). This work has complemented investigations of how unfamiliar faces are processed and the findings from these two strands of research have given rise to accounts that propose qualitatively different forms of representation for familiar and unfamiliar faces. Evidence to suggest that we process familiar and unfamiliar faces in different ways is available from cognitive neuropsychology, brain scanning, and psychophysics. However, in this review we focus on the evidence, available from experimental investigations of how people recognise faces, for different types of representation existing for each type of face. Factors affecting recognition are evaluated in terms of how they apply to familiar and unfamiliar faces and categorised according to the nature of their impact. In the final section this evidence, along with recent advances in the field, is used to explore the way in which unfamiliar faces may become familiar and the factors that may be important for the development of familiar face representations.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning/physiology , Face , Form Perception/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Facial Expression , Humans
11.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 62(1): 50-62, 2009 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18609401

ABSTRACT

There are a number of theories that suggest that age of acquisition (AoA) effects are not uniform across different tasks. Catling and Johnston (2006a) found greater AoA effects within an object-naming task than in a semantic classification task. They explained these findings by suggesting that AoA effects might accumulate according to how many levels of representation a task necessitates access to. Brysbaert and Ghyselinck (2006) explain the difference in AoA effects by proposing two distinct types of AoA (frequency dependent and frequency independent), the first accounted for by a connectionist-type mechanism and the latter situated at the interface between semantics and word production. Moreover, Moore, Smith-Spark, and Valentine (2004) and Holmes and Ellis (2006) have suggested that there are two loci of AoA effects: at the phonological level and somewhere within the perceptual level of representation. Again, this could account for the varying degrees of AoA effects. This study sets about testing these ideas by assessing the effect size of AoA across a series of different tasks that necessitate access to various levels of representation. Experiments 1-4 demonstrate significant effects of AoA in a novel picture-picture verification task, an object classification task, a picture verification task, and an object-naming task. Experiment 5 showed no effects of initial phoneme on the naming of the critical objects used within Experiments 1-4. The implication of the varying AoA effect sizes found within Experiments 1-4 in relation to explanations of AoA are discussed.


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Retention, Psychology/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Time Factors , Young Adult
12.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 34(2): 282-301, 2008 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18315406

ABSTRACT

In 2 experiments, the authors explored age of acquisition (AoA) and word frequency (WF) effects in picture naming using the psychological refractory period paradigm. In Experiment 1, participants named a picture and then, a short time later, categorized 1 of 3 possible auditory tones as high, medium, or low. Both AoA (Experiment 1A) and WF (Experiment 1B) effects propagated onto tone discrimination reaction times (RTs), with the effects of AoA being stronger. In Experiment 2, the to-be-named picture followed the auditory tone by a varying interval. As the interval decreased, picture naming RTs increased. The relationship between the interval and AoA (Experiment 2A) was reliably underadditive; AoA effects were eliminated at the shortest interval. In contrast, WF (Experiment 2B) was additive with the effects of the interval. These results demonstrate an empirical dissociation between AoA and WF effects. AoA affects processing stages that precede those that are sensitive to WF. The implications for theories of picture naming are discussed.


Subject(s)
Attention , Language Development , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Pitch Discrimination , Semantics , Verbal Learning , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Neural Networks, Computer , Phonetics , Reaction Time , Refractory Period, Psychological , Verbal Behavior
13.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 25(1): 3-26, 2008 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18340601

ABSTRACT

The ability to recognize individual faces is of crucial social importance for humans and evolutionarily necessary for survival. Consequently, faces may be "special" stimuli, for which we have developed unique modular perceptual and recognition processes. Some of the strongest evidence for face processing being modular comes from cases of prosopagnosia, where patients are unable to recognize faces whilst retaining the ability to recognize other objects. Here we present the case of an acquired prosopagnosic whose poor recognition was linked to a perceptual impairment in face processing. Despite this, she had intact object recognition, even at a subordinate level. She also showed a normal ability to learn and to generalize learning of nonfacial exemplars differing in the nature and arrangement of their parts, along with impaired learning and generalization of facial exemplars. The case provides evidence for modular perceptual processes for faces.


Subject(s)
Face , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Prosopagnosia/diagnosis , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Field Dependence-Independence , Generalization, Stimulus/physiology , Humans , Intracranial Embolism/complications , Intracranial Embolism/physiopathology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Mental Recall/physiology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Prosopagnosia/physiopathology , Prosopagnosia/psychology , Reaction Time/physiology
14.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 125(3): 301-18, 2007 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17055443

ABSTRACT

The impact of age of acquisition (AoA) on object recognition was explored in three experiments measuring visual duration threshold (VDT) for the identification of pictures labelled with early and late acquired names. Participants viewed briefly displayed images preceded and followed by a pattern mask. The minimum display duration required for correct identification was shorter for pictures labelled with early names than for those labelled with late names. In Experiments 2 and 3 we explored the effects of two forms of visual degradation on VDT for pictures with early and late acquired names. Both degradation by superimposed visual elements, and degradation by contrast reduction extended VDT, but only the former interacted with AoA. We conclude that both AoA and degradation by superimposed visual elements affect the efficiency of visual object recognition, but only degradation by contrast and not AoA affects the efficiency of earlier pre-recognition processes.


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Humans , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology , Students , Time Factors , Visual Perception/physiology
15.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 59(2): 326-39, 2006 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16618637

ABSTRACT

Two experiments examined repetition priming on tasks that require access to semantic (or biographical) information from faces. In the second stage of each experiment, participants made either a nationality or an occupation decision to faces of celebrities, and, in the first stage, they made either the same or a different decision to faces (in Experiment 1) or the same or a different decision to printed names (in Experiment 2). All combinations of priming and test tasks produced clear repetition effects, which occurred irrespective of whether the decisions made were positive or negative. Same-domain (face-to-face) repetition priming was larger than cross-domain (name-to-face) priming, and priming was larger when the two tasks were the same. It is discussed how these findings are more readily accommodated by the Burton, Bruce, and Johnston (1990) model of face recognition than by episode-based accounts of repetition priming.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Face , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Semantics , Cues , Decision Making , Humans , Reaction Time
16.
Schizophr Res ; 85(1-3): 96-105, 2006 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16644185

ABSTRACT

It is well established that schizophrenia is associated with difficulties recognising facial expressions of emotion. It has been suggested that this impairment could be specific to moving faces [Archer, J., Hay, D., Young, A., 1994. Movement, face processing and schizophrenia: evidence of a differential deficit in expression analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 517-528]. The current study used point-light images to assess whether people with schizophrenia can interpret emotions from isolated patterns of facial movement in the absence of featural cues. Emotion recognition from moving and static images was assessed using a forced choice design with two sets of three emotions (anger, sadness and surprise; disgust, fear and happiness). The schizophrenia group was significantly better at recognising the emotions from moving images than static images. Although the control group was more accurate overall than the schizophrenia group, both groups presented the same characteristic patterns of performance across tasks. For example, in terms of which emotions were better recognised than others and the types of misidentifications that were made. Hence, it is concluded that people with schizophrenia are sensitive to the motion patterns which underlie individual expressions of emotion and can use this information to accurately recognise emotions.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/epidemiology , Expressed Emotion , Facial Expression , Motion Perception , Recognition, Psychology , Schizophrenia/epidemiology , Visual Perception , Adult , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
17.
Mem Cognit ; 33(5): 905-12, 2005 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16383177

ABSTRACT

In two experiments, we investigated the role of age of acquisition (AoA) in the categorizing of objects in semantic tasks that do not require access to the object names. In both a found inside or outside the house (Experiment 1A) and a smaller or larger than a loaf of bread (Experiment 2A) classification task, objects with earlier-acquired names were categorized more quickly than those with later-acquired names. Experiments 1B and 2B also showed AoA effects on object-naming times for the same pictures. We conclude that AoA operates within the identification process in a fashion not simply restricted to name retrieval. These effects may be better explained in terms of the connectionist model proposed by Ellis and Lambon Ralph (2000) or by accounts that locate AoA within the semantic system (e.g., Brysbaert, Van Wijnendaele, & De Deyne, 2000; van Loon-Vervoorn, 1989).


Subject(s)
Semantics , Verbal Learning , Visual Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Humans , Reaction Time
18.
Perception ; 33(2): 159-68, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15109159

ABSTRACT

We report an experiment in which participants made gender judgments (male or female) to faces. There were three groups of faces: unfamiliar, familiar (celebrities), and a set which had been learned earlier by the participants during the experimental session. The principal purpose of this study was to establish an indirect measure of assessing whether faces have become familiar through learning that does not require overt recognition. Bruce and Young's (1986 British Journal of Psychology 77 305-327) model of face recognition suggests face-processing tasks are independent of one another and so familiarity should have no impact on the time taken to perform gender decisions. However, recent studies have suggested that some face processes are not completely independent. A gender judgment is a simple task which would be useful in face-learning experiments. We examined whether exposure to previously novel faces facilitates a later gender decision to those faces. During a learning stage, participants viewed a set of unfamiliar faces. At test, participants were able to assign gender faster to previously familiar (famous) faces and learned faces than they were to unfamiliar faces. Therefore familiarity can influence the speed at which gender is analysed. We explain our findings with reference to the Burton et al (1990 British Journal of Psychology 81 361-380) interactive activation and competition (IAC) model of face recognition and discuss how the gender judgment might be employed as a means of tracking the acquisition of familiarity in face-learning studies.


Subject(s)
Face , Gender Identity , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Judgment , Learning , Male , Models, Psychological , Psychological Tests , Reaction Time , Recognition, Psychology
19.
Perception ; 31(8): 985-94, 2002.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12269591

ABSTRACT

An experiment is reported in which participants matched complete images of unfamiliar, moderately familiar, and highly familiar faces with simultaneously presented images of internal and external features. Participants had to decide if the two images depicted same or different individuals. Matches to internal features were made faster to highly familiar faces than both to moderately familiar and to unfamiliar faces, and matches to moderately familiar faces were made faster than to unfamiliar faces. For external feature matches, this advantage was only found for "different" decision matches to highly familiar faces compared to unfamiliar faces. The results indicate that the differences in familiar and unfamiliar face processing are not the result of all-or-none effects, but seem to have a graded impact on matching performance. These findings extend the earlier work of Young et al (1985 Perception 14 737-746), and we discuss the possibility of using the matching task as an indirect measure of face familiarity.


Subject(s)
Face , Form Perception/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Cues , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Psychophysics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...