Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 15 de 15
Filter
1.
J Clin Invest ; 130(12): 6616-6630, 2020 12 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33164988

ABSTRACT

Dysregulation of habit formation has been recently proposed as pivotal to eating disorders. Here, we report that a subset of patients suffering from restrictive anorexia nervosa have enhanced habit formation compared with healthy controls. Habit formation is modulated by striatal cholinergic interneurons. These interneurons express vesicular transporters for acetylcholine (VAChT) and glutamate (VGLUT3) and use acetylcholine/glutamate cotransmission to regulate striatal functions. Using mice with genetically silenced VAChT (VAChT conditional KO, VAChTcKO) or VGLUT3 (VGLUT3cKO), we investigated the roles that acetylcholine and glutamate released by cholinergic interneurons play in habit formation and maladaptive eating. Silencing glutamate favored goal-directed behaviors and had no impact on eating behavior. In contrast, VAChTcKO mice were more prone to habits and maladaptive eating. Specific deletion of VAChT in the dorsomedial striatum of adult mice was sufficient to phenocopy maladaptive eating behaviors of VAChTcKO mice. Interestingly, VAChTcKO mice had reduced dopamine release in the dorsomedial striatum but not in the dorsolateral striatum. The dysfunctional eating behavior of VAChTcKO mice was alleviated by donepezil and by l-DOPA, confirming an acetylcholine/dopamine deficit. Our study reveals that loss of acetylcholine leads to a dopamine imbalance in striatal compartments, thereby promoting habits and vulnerability to maladaptive eating in mice.


Subject(s)
Acetylcholine/metabolism , Corpus Striatum , Feeding and Eating Disorders/metabolism , Glutamic Acid/metabolism , Interneurons/metabolism , Adult , Animals , Corpus Striatum/metabolism , Corpus Striatum/physiopathology , Donepezil/pharmacology , Feeding Behavior/drug effects , Feeding and Eating Disorders/drug therapy , Feeding and Eating Disorders/genetics , Feeding and Eating Disorders/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Levodopa/pharmacology , Male , Mice , Mice, Knockout , Middle Aged , Vesicular Acetylcholine Transport Proteins/genetics , Vesicular Acetylcholine Transport Proteins/metabolism
2.
Cerebellum ; 17(2): 204-212, 2018 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29086357

ABSTRACT

Friedreich ataxia, an autosomal recessive mitochondrial disease, is the most frequent inherited ataxia. Many studies have attempted to identify cognitive and affective changes associated with the disease, but conflicting results have been obtained, depending on the tests used and because many of the samples studied were very small. We investigated personality and neuropsychological characteristics in a cohort of 47 patients with genetically confirmed disease. The neuropsychological battery assessed multiple cognition domains: processing speed, attention, working memory, executive functions, verbal memory, vocabulary, visual reasoning, emotional recognition, and social cognition. Personality was assessed with the Temperament and Character Inventory, and depressive symptoms were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory. We found deficits of sustained attention, processing speed, semantic capacities, and verbal fluency only partly attributable to motor deficit or depressed mood. Visual reasoning, memory, and learning were preserved. Emotional processes and social cognition were unimpaired. We also detected a change in automatic processes, such as reading. Personality traits were characterized by high persistence and low self-transcendence. The mild cognitive impairment observed may be a developmental rather than degenerative problem, due to early cerebellum dysfunction, with the impairment of cognitive and emotional processing. Disease manifestations at crucial times for personality development may also have an important impact on personality traits.


Subject(s)
Cerebellum/pathology , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Friedreich Ataxia/complications , Friedreich Ataxia/psychology , Personality , Adolescent , Adult , Age of Onset , Aged , Emotions/physiology , Female , Friedreich Ataxia/genetics , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Personality Inventory , Regression Analysis , Retrospective Studies , Statistics, Nonparametric , Young Adult
3.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci ; 27(1): 59-64, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25716488

ABSTRACT

The authors report the inter-rater reliability and factor structure of the Short Problem Behaviors Assessment (PBA-s), a semistructured interview to measure severity and frequency of behavioral problems in Huntington's disease. Video recordings of 410 PBA-s interviews were rescored by an independent rater, and Cohen's kappa calculated to assess inter-rater reliability. The mean kappa was 0.74 for severity and 0.76 for frequency scores, whereas weighted kappa (allowing scores to differ by 1 point) was 0.94 for severity and 0.92 for frequency scores. The results of factor analysis were consistent with previous studies using other measures. The authors conclude that the PBA-s is a reliable measure.


Subject(s)
Huntington Disease/complications , Mental Disorders/diagnosis , Mental Disorders/etiology , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Europe , Female , Humans , International Cooperation , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Registries , Reproducibility of Results , Severity of Illness Index , Statistics, Nonparametric , Video Recording
4.
J Huntingtons Dis ; 3(2): 197-207, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25062862

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Composite scores derived from joint statistical modelling of individual risk factors are widely used to identify individuals who are at increased risk of developing disease or of faster disease progression. OBJECTIVE: We investigated the ability of composite measures developed using statistical models to differentiate progressive cognitive deterioration in Huntington's disease (HD) from natural decline in healthy controls. METHODS: Using longitudinal data from TRACK-HD, the optimal combinations of quantitative cognitive measures to differentiate premanifest and early stage HD individuals respectively from controls was determined using logistic regression. Composite scores were calculated from the parameters of each statistical model. Linear regression models were used to calculate effect sizes (ES) quantifying the difference in longitudinal change over 24 months between premanifest and early stage HD groups respectively and controls. ES for the composites were compared with ES for individual cognitive outcomes and other measures used in HD research. The 0.632 bootstrap was used to eliminate biases which result from developing and testing models in the same sample. RESULTS: In early HD, the composite score from the HD change prediction model produced an ES for difference in rate of 24-month change relative to controls of 1.14 (95% CI: 0.90 to 1.39), larger than the ES for any individual cognitive outcome and UHDRS Total Motor Score and Total Functional Capacity. In addition, this composite gave a statistically significant difference in rate of change in premanifest HD compared to controls over 24-months (ES: 0.24; 95% CI: 0.04 to 0.44), even though none of the individual cognitive outcomes produced statistically significant ES over this period. CONCLUSIONS: Composite scores developed using appropriate statistical modelling techniques have the potential to materially reduce required sample sizes for randomised controlled trials.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Huntington Disease/complications , Adult , Aging/physiology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Disease Progression , Female , Humans , Huntington Disease/physiopathology , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Statistical , Neuropsychological Tests
5.
Neuropsychologia ; 51(4): 748-59, 2013 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23262076

ABSTRACT

Clinical experience and prior studies suggest that Huntington disease (HD) patients have low insight into their motor disturbances and poor real-time awareness (concurrent awareness) of chorea. This has been attributed to sensory deficits but, until now, concurrent awareness of choreic movements has not been compared to the degree of insight that presymptomatic carriers of the HD gene and healthy control subjects have into non-pathological involuntary movements. To further investigate loss of insight into motor dysfunction in HD patients, we administered a video-recorded interview and 4 experimental tasks to 68 subjects from the TRACK-HD cohort, including 28 high-functioning patients in early stages of HD, 28 premanifest mutation carriers and 12 controls. All underwent full neurological and neuropsychological evaluations and 3T MRI examinations. Subjects were asked to assess the presence, body location, frequency, practical consequences and probable causes of motor impairments, as well as the presence and body location of involuntary movements during 4 experimental tasks. The accuracy of their judgments, assessed by comparison with objective criteria, was used as a measure of their insight into motor disturbances and of their concurrent awareness of involuntary movements. Insight was poor in early HD patients: motor symptoms were nearly always underestimated. In contrast, concurrent awareness of involuntary movements, although also poor, was essentially indistinguishable across the 3 groups of subjects: non-pathological involuntary movements were as difficult to perceive by controls and premanifest carriers as was chorea for early HD patients. GLM analysis suggested that both concurrent awareness and perception of practical consequences of movement disorder had a positive effect on intellectual insight, and that mental flexibility is involved in concurrent awareness. Our results suggest that low insight into motor dysfunction in early HD, although marginally modulated by cognitive factors, is mainly non-pathological, and parallels a general tendency, shared by healthy subjects, to neglect self-generated involuntary movements in real time. This tendency, combined with the paucity of functional consequences of incipient chorea, could explain the difficulty of its discovery by the patients.


Subject(s)
Chorea/psychology , Huntington Disease/psychology , Movement/physiology , Adult , Aging/psychology , Attention/physiology , Awareness , Brain/pathology , Cognition/physiology , Depression/psychology , Female , Heterozygote , Humans , Huntington Disease/genetics , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Intelligence/physiology , Linear Models , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Middle Aged , Movement Disorders/psychology , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Recognition, Psychology , Self Concept , Sex Characteristics , Surveys and Questionnaires , Video Recording
6.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 34(9): 2141-53, 2013 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22438242

ABSTRACT

Neuropathological studies in Huntington disease (HD) have demonstrated neuronal loss in the striatum, as well as in other brain regions including the cortex. With diffusion tensor MRI we evaluated the hypothesis that the clinical dysfunction in HD is related to regionally specific lesions of circuit-specific cortico-basal ganglia networks rather than to the striatum only. We included 27 HD and 24 controls from the TRACK-HD Paris cohort. The following assessments were used: self-paced tapping tasks, trail B making test (TMT), University of Pennsylvania smell identification test (UPSIT), and apathy scores from the problem behaviors assessment. Group comparisons of fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity and correlations were performed using voxel-based analysis. In the cortex, HD patients showed significant correlations between: (i) self paced tapping and mean diffusivity in the parietal lobe at 1.8 Hz and prefrontal areas at 3 Hz, (ii) UPSIT and mean diffusivity in the parietal, and median temporal lobes, the cingulum and the insula, and fractional anisotropy in the insula and the external capsule, (iii) TMT B and mean diffusivity in the white matter of the superior frontal, orbital, temporal, superior parietal and post central areas, and (iv) apathy and fractional anisotropy in the white matter of the rectus gyrus. In the basal ganglia, we found correlations between the self paced tapping, UPSIT, TMT tests, and mean diffusivity in the anterior part of the putamen and the caudate nucleus. In conclusion, disruption of motor, associative and limbic cortico-striatal circuits differentially contribute to the clinical signs of the disease.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Brain/pathology , Brain/physiopathology , Huntington Disease/pathology , Huntington Disease/physiopathology , Apathy/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Cognition Disorders/pathology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Female , Humans , Huntington Disease/complications , Image Interpretation, Computer-Assisted , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
7.
Psychiatry Res ; 207(1-2): 118-26, 2013 May 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23051887

ABSTRACT

Facial emotion recognition impairments have been reported in Huntington's disease (HD). However, the nature of the impairments across the spectrum of HD remains unclear. We report on emotion recognition data from 344 participants comprising premanifest HD (PreHD) and early HD patients, and controls. In a test of recognition of facial emotions, we examined responses to six basic emotional expressions and neutral expressions. In addition, and within the early HD sample, we tested for differences on emotion recognition performance between those 'on' vs. 'off' neuroleptic or selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) medications. The PreHD groups showed significant (p<0.05) impaired recognition, compared to controls, on fearful, angry and surprised faces; whereas the early HD groups were significantly impaired across all emotions including neutral expressions. In early HD, neuroleptic use was associated with worse facial emotion recognition, whereas SSRI use was associated with better facial emotion recognition. The findings suggest that emotion recognition impairments exist across the HD spectrum, but are relatively more widespread in manifest HD than in the premanifest period. Commonly prescribed medications to treat HD-related symptoms also appear to affect emotion recognition. These findings have important implications for interpersonal communication and medication usage in HD.


Subject(s)
Antipsychotic Agents/therapeutic use , Facial Expression , Huntington Disease/complications , Huntington Disease/drug therapy , Memory Disorders/etiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adult , Antipsychotic Agents/pharmacology , Emotions/drug effects , Emotions/physiology , Face , Female , Humans , Male , Memory Disorders/drug therapy , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/drug effects , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Recognition, Psychology/drug effects , Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors/pharmacology , Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors/therapeutic use
8.
J Huntingtons Dis ; 2(2): 159-75, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25063513

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Given the multifaceted nature of this inherited neurodegenerative condition, typically affecting adults in mid-life, it is perhaps not surprising that studies indicate poorer Health Related Quality of Life (HrQoL) in those with the gene-expansion and, by association, in their families. OBJECTIVE: This study aimed to extend the current literature by exploring specific life domains, including at an earlier disease stage than usually reported in the HRQoL literature, and in a subgroup of gene-negative partners. METHODS: 355 participants from the TRACK-HD cohort (120 Controls, 118 Pre-HD and 117 early-HD) completed standardised self-report measures of HrQoL (SF36 and QoLI), underwent clinical assessments of capacity and motor function (UHDRS), semi structured interviews assessing neuropsychiatric symptoms (PBA-s), completed paper and computerized cognitive tasks and assessment of behaviours associated with damage to frontal brain circuits (FrSBe). RESULTS: Each gene-expanded group scored statistically significantly lower than gene-negative sibling controls on the SF36 General Health subscale; neuropsychiatric symptoms and executive dysfunction were associated with reduced HrQoL. Those with Stage II disease reported statistically significantly lower HrQoL than gene-negative controls across physical, emotional and social life domains. Those partnered with manifest participants reported lower HrQoL in the social domain compared to those partnered with at-risk participants furthest from disease onset; and perseverative symptoms in manifest partners were found to be related to lower HrQoL in their gene-negative partners. HrQoL in gene-negative partners of pre-manifest individuals was associated with pre-manifest individuals' neuropsychiatric and cognitive function. CONCLUSIONS: Understanding the nature and timing of disruption to the HrQoL in people who are pre-manifest and diagnosed with HD, and their gene-negative partners, can inform the development of appropriate strategies and interventions.


Subject(s)
Health Status , Huntington Disease/physiopathology , Prodromal Symptoms , Quality of Life , Spouses/psychology , Adult , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Huntington Disease/complications , Huntington Disease/psychology , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests
9.
Neuron ; 76(5): 998-1009, 2012 Dec 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23217747

ABSTRACT

The division of human learning systems into reward and punishment opponent modules is still a debated issue. While the implication of ventral prefrontostriatal circuits in reward-based learning is well established, the neural underpinnings of punishment-based learning remain unclear. To elucidate the causal implication of brain regions that were related to punishment learning in a previous functional neuroimaging study, we tested the effects of brain damage on behavioral performance, using the same task contrasting monetary gains and losses. Cortical and subcortical candidate regions, the anterior insula and dorsal striatum, were assessed in patients presenting brain tumor and Huntington disease, respectively. Both groups exhibited selective impairment of punishment-based learning. Computational modeling suggested complementary roles for these structures: the anterior insula might be involved in learning the negative value of loss-predicting cues, whereas the dorsal striatum might be involved in choosing between those cues so as to avoid the worst.


Subject(s)
Avoidance Learning/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Corpus Striatum/physiology , Punishment , Adult , Atrophy/pathology , Brain Neoplasms/pathology , Brain Neoplasms/physiopathology , Brain Neoplasms/psychology , Cerebral Cortex/blood supply , Corpus Striatum/blood supply , Female , Humans , Huntington Disease/pathology , Huntington Disease/physiopathology , Huntington Disease/psychology , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Oxygen
10.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 83(7): 687-94, 2012 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22566599

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Deterioration of cognitive functioning is a debilitating symptom in many neurodegenerative diseases, such as Huntington's disease (HD). To date, there are no effective treatments for the cognitive problems associated with HD. Cognitive assessment outcomes will have a central role in the efforts to develop treatments to delay onset or slow the progression of the disease. The TRACK-HD study was designed to build a rational basis for the selection of cognitive outcomes for HD clinical trials. METHODS: There were a total of 349 participants, including controls (n=116), premanifest HD (n=117) and early HD (n=116). A standardised cognitive assessment battery (including nine cognitive tests comprising 12 outcome measures) was administered at baseline, and at 12 and 24 months, and consisted of a combination of paper and pencil and computerised tasks selected to be sensitive to cortical-striatal damage or HD. Each cognitive outcome was analysed separately using a generalised least squares regression model. Results are expressed as effect sizes to permit comparisons between tasks. RESULTS: 10 of the 12 cognitive outcomes showed evidence of deterioration in the early HD group, relative to controls, over 24 months, with greatest sensitivity in Symbol Digit, Circle Tracing direct and indirect, and Stroop word reading. In contrast, there was very little evidence of deterioration in the premanifest HD group relative to controls. CONCLUSIONS: The findings describe tests that are sensitive to longitudinal cognitive change in HD and elucidate important considerations for selecting cognitive outcomes for clinical trials of compounds aimed at ameliorating cognitive decline in HD.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/etiology , Huntington Disease/complications , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Disease Progression , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Severity of Illness Index , Sex Factors , Time Factors , Young Adult
11.
J Huntingtons Dis ; 1(1): 97-106, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25063192

ABSTRACT

Working memory deficits have been found in Huntington's disease (HD) and in a small group of premanifest (PreHD) gene-carriers. However, the nature and extent of these deficits are unknown. In a large cross-sectional study, we aimed to determine the degree of visuospatial working memory dysfunction across multiple stages of HD. Specifically, visuospatial working memory capacity and response times across various degrees of difficulty were examined, as well as the relationship between visuospatial working memory and motor dysfunction. We examined 62 PreHD-A gene-carriers (>10.8 years from estimated disease onset), 58 PreHD-B gene-carriers (<10.8 years from estimated disease onset), 77 stage-1 HD patients (HD1), 44 stage-2 HD patients (HD2), and 122 healthy controls. Participants viewed coloured squares (in sets of 3, 5 and 7) on a screen and were to decide whether on a subsequent screen the encircled square has changed colour. Accuracy and response times were recorded. Compared to controls, significant group differences in visuospatial working memory capacity (accuracy) were seen in PreHD-B, HD1 and HD2 groups across the difficulty levels. Significant group differences on response times were found for all groups (PreHD-A to HD2) compared to controls; the most difficult level producing the only group difference in speed between PreHD-A and controls. Accuracy and speed were positively correlated only in the HD groups. These findings suggest that visuospatial working memory impairments are detectable in both premanifest and manifest HD; the manifest HD showed evidence for a "worse-worse phenomenon" whereby reductions were present in both motor speed and accuracy.


Subject(s)
Genetic Predisposition to Disease/genetics , Huntington Disease/genetics , Huntington Disease/physiopathology , Memory, Short-Term , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 49(2): 264-70, 2011 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21094653

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Visuomotor integration deficits have been documented in Huntington disease (HD), with disproportionately more impairment when direct visual feedback is unavailable. Visuomotor integration under direct and indirect visual feedback conditions has not been investigated in the stage before clinical onset ('premanifest'). However, given evidence of posterior cortical atrophy in premanifest HD, we predicted visuomotor integration would be adversely affected, with greater impairment under conditions of indirect visual feedback. METHODS: 239 subjects with the HD CAG expansion, ranging from more than a decade before predicted clinical onset until early stage disease, and 122 controls, completed a circle-tracing task, which included both direct and indirect visual feedback conditions. Measures included accuracy, speed, and speed of error detection and correction. Using brain images acquired with 3T magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), we generated grey and white matter volumes with voxel-based morphometry, and analyzed correlations with circle-tracing performance. RESULTS: Compared with controls, early HD was associated with lower accuracy and slower performance in both circle-tracing conditions. Premanifest HD was associated with lower accuracy in both conditions and fewer rotations in the direct condition. Comparing performance in the indirect condition with the direct condition, HD gene expansion-carriers exhibited a disproportionate increase in errors relative to controls. Premanifest and early HD groups required longer to detect and correct errors, especially in the indirect condition. Slower performance in the indirect condition was associated with lower grey matter volumes in the left somatosensory cortex in VBM analyses. CONCLUSIONS: Visuomotor integration deficits are evident many years before the clinical onset of HD, with deficits in speed, accuracy, and speed of error detection and correction. The visuomotor transformation demands of the indirect condition result in a disproportionate decrease in accuracy in the HD groups. Slower performance under indirect visual feedback was associated with atrophy of the left-hemisphere somatosensory cortex, which may reflect the proprioceptive demands of the task.


Subject(s)
Huntington Disease/complications , Motor Skills Disorders/etiology , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Adult , Brain/pathology , Disease Progression , Female , Humans , Huntington Disease/pathology , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Skills Disorders/complications , Motor Skills Disorders/pathology , Nerve Fibers, Myelinated/pathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/complications , Perceptual Disorders/pathology , Predictive Value of Tests , Statistics as Topic
13.
Investig. psicol ; 3(1): 55-65, 1998.
Article in Spanish | LILACS | ID: lil-754647

ABSTRACT

En este trabajo se desarrolla la problemática vincu-lada con la analogía del ordenador y sus implicancias. Se propone, siguiendo el modelo de algunos autores, un criterio tentativo para caracterizar a las ciencias cognitivas. También se analizan las cuestiones del sentido y alcance de la analo-gía, para luego exponer, sobre la base de ese análisis, una clasificación de las ciencias cognitivas, atendiendo a sus supuestos teóricos, sus objetivos y su metodología.


Subject(s)
Humans , Cognitive Science , Computers , Metaphor
14.
15.
Buenos Aires; Facultad de Psicología; 1a. ed; 1998. 32 p. 30 cm. (76266).
Monography in Spanish | BINACIS | ID: bin-76266
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...