Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 15 de 15
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Adapt Human Behav Physiol ; 6(4): 447-466, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33088674

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Although it is widely assumed that men's sexual desire and interest in casual sex (i.e., sociosexual orientation) are linked to steroid hormone levels, evidence for such associations is mixed. METHODS: We tested for both longitudinal and cross-sectional relationships between salivary testosterone, cortisol, reported sexual desire and sociosexuality in a sample of 61 young adult men, each of whom was tested weekly on up to five occasions. RESULTS: Longitudinal analyses showed no clear relationships between steroid hormones and self-reported sexual desire or sociosexual orientation. Cross-sectional analyses showed no significant associations between average hormone levels and self-reported sexual desire. However, some aspects of sociosexuality, most notably desire for casual sex, were related to men's average hormone levels. Men with higher average testosterone reported greater desire for casual sex, but only if they also had relatively low average cortisol levels. CONCLUSIONS: Our results support a Dual Hormone account of men's sociosexuality, in which the combined effects of testosterone and cortisol predict the extent of men's interest in casual sex. However, we did not detect compelling evidence for an association of within-subject hormone shifts and sexual desire or sociosexual orientation.

2.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 45(12): 1589-1595, 2019 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31556686

ABSTRACT

Facial attractiveness plays a critical role in social interaction, influencing many different social outcomes. However, the factors that influence facial attractiveness judgments remain relatively poorly understood. Here, we used a sample of 594 young adult female face images to compare the performance of existing theory-driven models of facial attractiveness and a data-driven (i.e., theory-neutral) model. Our data-driven model and a theory-driven model including various traits commonly studied in facial attractiveness research (asymmetry, averageness, sexual dimorphism, body mass index, and representational sparseness) performed similarly well. By contrast, univariate theory-driven models performed relatively poorly. These results (a) highlight the utility of data driven models of facial attractiveness and (b) suggest that theory-driven research on facial attractiveness would benefit from greater adoption of multivariate approaches, rather than the univariate approaches that they currently almost exclusively employ. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Beauty , Face , Judgment , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Photic Stimulation , Psychological Theory , Young Adult
4.
Am J Hum Biol ; 31(1): e23203, 2019 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30488525

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Ancestrally, strength is likely to have played a critical role in determining the ability to obtain and retain resources and the allocation of social status among humans. Responses to facial cues of strength are therefore thought to play an important role in human social interaction. Although many researchers have proposed that sexually dimorphic facial morphology is reliably correlated with physical strength, evidence for this hypothesis is somewhat mixed. Moreover, to date, only one study has investigated the putative relationship between facial masculinity and physical strength in women. Consequently, we tested for correlations between handgrip strength and objective measures of face-shape masculinity. METHODS: 531 women took part in the study. We measured each participant's handgrip strength (dominant hand). Sexual dimorphism of face shape was objectively measured from each face photograph using two methods: discriminant analysis and vector analysis. These methods use shape components derived from principal component analyses of facial landmarks to measure the probability of the face being classified as male (discriminant analysis method) or to locate the face on a female-male continuum (vector analysis method). RESULTS: Our analyses revealed that handgrip strength is, at best, only weakly correlated with facial masculinity in women. There was a weak significant association between handgrip strength and one measure of women's facial masculinity. The relationship between handgrip strength and our other measure of women's facial masculinity was not significant. DISCUSSION: Together, these results do not support the hypothesis that face-shape masculinity is an important cue of physical strength, at least in women.


Subject(s)
Face/anatomy & histology , Hand Strength , Masculinity , Adult , Female , Humans , Scotland , Young Adult
5.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 98: 1-5, 2018 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30077864

ABSTRACT

Putative associations between sex hormones and attractive physical characteristics in women are central to many theories of human physical attractiveness and mate choice. Although such theories have become very influential, evidence that physically attractive and unattractive women have different hormonal profiles is equivocal. Consequently, we investigated hypothesized relationships between salivary estradiol and progesterone and two aspects of women's physical attractiveness that are commonly assumed to be correlated with levels of these hormones: facial attractiveness (N = 249) and waist-to-hip ratio (N = 247). Our analyses revealed no compelling evidence that women with more attractive faces or lower (i.e., more attractive) waist-to-hip ratios had higher levels of estradiol or progesterone. One analysis did suggest that women with more attractive waist-to-hip ratios had significantly higher progesterone, but the relationship was weak and the relationship not significant in other analyses. These results do not support the influential hypothesis that between-women differences in physical attractiveness are related to estradiol and/or progesterone.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior/physiology , Marriage/psychology , Sexual Behavior/psychology , Estradiol/analysis , Face , Facial Recognition , Female , Fertility , Humans , Menstrual Cycle/physiology , Physical Appearance, Body/physiology , Progesterone/analysis , Saliva/chemistry , Sex Characteristics , Waist-Hip Ratio/psychology , Young Adult
6.
Evol Psychol ; 16(2): 1474704918776748, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29860865

ABSTRACT

When the adult sex ratio of the local population is biased toward women, men face greater costs due to increased direct intrasexual competition. In order to mitigate these costs, men may be more attuned to cues of other men's physical dominance under these conditions. Consequently, we investigated the relationships between the extent to which people ( N = 3,586) ascribed high dominance to masculinized versus feminized faces and variation in adult sex ratio across U.S. states. Linear mixed models showed that masculinized faces were perceived as more dominant than feminized faces, particularly for judgments of men's facial dominance. Dominance perceptions were weakly related to adult sex ratio, and this relationship was not moderated by face sex, participant sex, or their interaction. Thus, our results suggest that dominance perceptions are relatively unaffected by broad geographical differences in adult sex ratios.


Subject(s)
Facial Recognition/physiology , Femininity , Masculinity , Sex Ratio , Social Dominance , Social Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , United States , Young Adult
7.
Psychol Sci ; 29(6): 996-1005, 2018 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29708849

ABSTRACT

Although widely cited as strong evidence that sexual selection has shaped human facial-attractiveness judgments, findings suggesting that women's preferences for masculine characteristics in men's faces are related to women's hormonal status are equivocal and controversial. Consequently, we conducted the largest-ever longitudinal study of the hormonal correlates of women's preferences for facial masculinity ( N = 584). Analyses showed no compelling evidence that preferences for facial masculinity were related to changes in women's salivary steroid hormone levels. Furthermore, both within-subjects and between-subjects comparisons showed no evidence that oral contraceptive use decreased masculinity preferences. However, women generally preferred masculinized over feminized versions of men's faces, particularly when assessing men's attractiveness for short-term, rather than long-term, relationships. Our results do not support the hypothesized link between women's preferences for facial masculinity and their hormonal status.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Gonadal Steroid Hormones/metabolism , Masculinity , Menstrual Cycle/metabolism , Sexual Behavior/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Saliva , Young Adult
8.
Adapt Human Behav Physiol ; 4(3): 321-327, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30956939

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Many previous studies have investigated relationships between men's competitiveness and testosterone. For example, the extent of changes in men's testosterone levels following a competitive task predicts the likelihood of them choosing to compete again. Recent work investigating whether individual differences in men's testosterone levels predict individual differences in their competitiveness have produced mixed results. METHODS: In light of the above, we investigated whether men's (N = 59) scores on the Intrasexual Competitiveness Scale were related to either within-subject changes or between-subject differences in men's salivary testosterone levels. RESULTS: Men's responses on the Intrasexual Competitiveness Scale did not appear to track within-subject changes in testosterone. By contrast with one recent study, men's Intrasexual Competitiveness Scale also did not appear to be related to individual differences in testosterone. CONCLUSIONS: Our results present no evidence for associations between men's testosterone and their responses on the Intrasexual Competitiveness Scale.

9.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 88: 153-157, 2018 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29287282

ABSTRACT

Several recent longitudinal studies have investigated the hormonal correlates of both young adult women's general sexual desire and, more specifically, their desire for uncommitted sexual relationships. Findings across these studies have been mixed, potentially because each study tested only small samples of women (Ns = 43, 33, and 14). Here we report results from a much larger (N = 375) longitudinal study of hormonal correlates of young adult women's general sexual desire and their desire for uncommitted sexual relationships. Our analyses suggest that within-woman changes in general sexual desire are negatively related to progesterone, but are not related to testosterone or cortisol. We observed some positive relationships for estradiol, but these were generally only significant for solitary sexual desire. By contrast with our results for general sexual desire, analyses showed no evidence that changes in women's desire for uncommitted sexual relationships are related to their hormonal status. Together, these results suggest that changes in hormonal status contribute to changes in women's general sexual desire, but do not influence women's desire for uncommitted sexual relationships.


Subject(s)
Gonadal Steroid Hormones/analysis , Libido/physiology , Sexual Behavior/physiology , Adult , Estradiol/analysis , Female , Humans , Hydrocortisone/analysis , Interpersonal Relations , Longitudinal Studies , Menstrual Cycle/physiology , Menstrual Cycle/psychology , Progesterone/analysis , Saliva/chemistry , Sexual Behavior/psychology , Sexual Partners , Social Class , Testosterone/analysis , Young Adult
10.
Evol Psychol ; 15(1): 1474704917697332, 2017 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28277747

ABSTRACT

The benefits of minimizing the costs of engaging in violent conflict are thought to have shaped adaptations for the rapid assessment of others' capacity to inflict physical harm. Although studies have suggested that men's faces and voices both contain information about their threat potential, one recent study suggested that men's faces are a more valid cue of their threat potential than their voices are. Consequently, the current study investigated the interrelationships among a composite measure of men's actual threat potential (derived from the measures of their upper-body strength, height, and weight) and composite measures of these men's perceived facial and vocal threat potential (derived from dominance, strength, and weight ratings of their faces and voices, respectively). Although men's perceived facial and vocal threat potential were positively correlated, men's actual threat potential was related to their perceived facial, but not vocal, threat potential. These results present new evidence that men's faces may be a more valid cue of these aspects of threat potential than their voices are.


Subject(s)
Face , Fear , Men , Social Perception , Voice , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
11.
Adapt Human Behav Physiol ; 3(4): 275-281, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32010551

ABSTRACT

Although some researchers have suggested that the interaction between cortisol and testosterone predicts ratings of men's facial attractiveness, evidence for this pattern of results is equivocal. Consequently, the current study tested for a correlation between men's facial attractiveness and the interaction between their cortisol and testosterone levels. We also tested for corresponding relationships between the interaction between cortisol and testosterone and ratings of men's facial health and dominance (perceived traits that are correlated with facial attractiveness in men). We found no evidence that ratings of either facial attractiveness or health were correlated with the interaction between cortisol and testosterone. Some analyses suggested that the interaction between cortisol and testosterone levels may predict ratings of men's facial dominance, however, with testosterone being more closely related to facial dominance ratings among men with higher cortisol. Our results suggest that the relationship between men's facial attractiveness and the interaction between cortisol and testosterone is not robust.

12.
Horm Behav ; 87: 57-61, 2017 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27810343

ABSTRACT

Behaviors that minimize exposure to sources of pathogens can carry opportunity costs. Consequently, how individuals resolve the tradeoff between the benefits and costs of behavioral immune responses should be sensitive to the extent to which they are vulnerable to infectious diseases. However, although it is a strong prediction of this functional flexibility principle, there is little compelling evidence that individuals with stronger physiological immune responses show weaker behavioral immune responses. Here we show that men with the combination of high testosterone and low cortisol levels, a hormonal profile recently found to be associated with particularly strong physiological immune responses, show weaker preferences for color cues associated with carotenoid pigmentation. Since carotenoid cues are thought to index vulnerability to infectious illnesses, our results are consistent with the functional flexibility principle's prediction that individuals with stronger physiological immune responses show weaker behavioral immune responses.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior , Face , Immunity, Innate/physiology , Sexual Behavior , Testosterone/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Behavior/drug effects , Behavior/physiology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Cues , Female , Humans , Immunity, Innate/drug effects , Male , Marriage/psychology , Photic Stimulation , Sexual Behavior/physiology , Testosterone/blood , Young Adult
13.
PLoS One ; 11(11): e0166855, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27875569

ABSTRACT

Feminine physical characteristics in women are positively correlated with markers of their mate quality. Previous research on men's judgments of women's facial attractiveness suggests that men show stronger preferences for feminine characteristics in women's faces when their own testosterone levels are relatively high. Such results could reflect stronger preferences for high quality mates when mating motivation is strong and/or following success in male-male competition. Given these findings, the current study investigated whether a similar effect of testosterone occurs for men's preferences for feminine characteristics in women's voices. Men's preferences for feminized versus masculinized versions of women's and men's voices were assessed in five weekly test sessions and saliva samples were collected in each test session. Analyses showed no relationship between men's voice preferences and their testosterone levels. Men's tendency to perceive masculinized men's and women's voices as more dominant was also unrelated to their testosterone levels. Together, the results of the current study suggest that testosterone-linked changes in responses to sexually dimorphic characteristics previously reported for men's perceptions of faces do not occur for men's perceptions of voices.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Saliva/metabolism , Sex Characteristics , Testosterone/metabolism , Voice , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
14.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 56: 29-34, 2015 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25796069

ABSTRACT

Red facial coloration is an important social cue in many primate species, including humans. In such species, the vasodilatory effects of estradiol may cause red facial coloration to change systematically during females' ovarian cycle. Although increased red facial coloration during estrus has been observed in female mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), evidence linking primate facial color changes directly to changes in measured estradiol is lacking. Addressing this issue, we used a longitudinal design to demonstrate that red facial coloration tracks within-subject changes in women's estradiol, but not within-subject changes in women's progesterone or estradiol-to-progesterone ratio. Moreover, the relationship between estradiol and facial redness was observed in two independent samples of women (N = 50 and N = 65). Our results suggest that changes in facial coloration may provide cues of women's fertility and present the first evidence for a direct link between estradiol and female facial redness in a primate species.


Subject(s)
Color , Estradiol/metabolism , Face , Skin Pigmentation/physiology , Adolescent , Estradiol/analysis , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Progesterone/analysis , Progesterone/metabolism , Saliva/chemistry , Young Adult
15.
Arch Sex Behav ; 43(5): 965-71, 2014 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24366658

ABSTRACT

Previous research suggests that people's perceptions of own-sex individuals can change according to within-individual variation in their romantic partners' sexual strategies. For example, men are more likely to perceive other men's faces as looking particularly dominant during the fertile phase of their partner's menstrual cycle, when women tend to be more open to uncommitted sexual relationships. By contrast, little is known about how relatively stable between-individuals differences in partners' openness to uncommitted sexual relationships (i.e., their sociosexual orientation) predict perceptions of own-sex individuals. The revised Sociosexual Orientation Inventory (SOI-R) assesses individuals' openness to uncommitted sexual relationships and shows high test-retest reliability over long periods of time. Consequently, we tested whether the SOI-R scores of men and women in heterosexual romantic couples predicted their perceptions of own-sex faces displaying exaggerated sex-typical cues. Men's, but not women's, SOI-R was positively correlated with the extent to which both the man and woman within a couple ascribed high dominance and attractiveness to own-sex faces with exaggerated sex-typical cues. In other words, individuals in couples where the man reported being particularly open to uncommitted sexual relationships were more likely to ascribe dominance and attractiveness to own-sex individuals displaying a putative cue of good phenotypic condition. These findings suggest that both men's and women's perceptions of potential competitors for mates are sensitive to the male partner's sexual strategy. Such individual differences in perceptions may benefit men's ability to compete for extra-pair and/or replacement mates and benefit women's mate guarding behaviors.


Subject(s)
Cues , Face , Sex Characteristics , Sexual Behavior , Sexual Partners/psychology , Adult , Esthetics , Female , Heterosexuality , Humans , Individuality , Male , Perception , Sexual Behavior/physiology , Sexual Behavior/psychology , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...