Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 175
Filter
1.
Vision Res ; 135: 26-33, 2017 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28438680

ABSTRACT

Visual perceptual learning describes the improvement of visual perception with repeated practice. Previous research has established that the learning effects of perceptual training may be transferable to untrained stimulus attributes such as spatial location under certain circumstances. However, the mechanisms involved in transfer have not yet been fully elucidated. Here, we investigated the effect of altering training time course on the transferability of learning effects. Participants were trained on a motion direction discrimination task or a sinusoidal grating orientation discrimination task in a single visual hemifield. The 4000 training trials were either condensed into one day, or spread evenly across five training days. When participants were trained over a five-day period, there was transfer of learning to both the untrained visual hemifield and the untrained task. In contrast, when the same amount of training was condensed into a single day, participants did not show any transfer of learning. Thus, learning time course may influence the transferability of perceptual learning effects.


Subject(s)
Learning/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Transfer, Psychology/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Time Factors , Young Adult
2.
Eye (Lond) ; 29(2): 200-7, 2015 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25412716

ABSTRACT

Eye movements are a source of valuable information to both clinicians and scientists as abnormalities of them frequently act as clues to the localization of a disease process. Classically, they are divided into two main types: those that hold the gaze, keeping images steady on the retina (vestibulo-ocular and optokinetic reflexes) and those that shift gaze and redirect the line of sight to a new object of interest (saccades, vergence, and smooth pursuit). Here we will review some of the major ocular motor abnormalities present in neurodegenerative disorders.


Subject(s)
Neurodegenerative Diseases/physiopathology , Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Convergence, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Nystagmus, Optokinetic/physiology , Pursuit, Smooth/physiology , Reflex, Vestibulo-Ocular/physiology , Saccades/physiology
3.
Rev Neurol (Paris) ; 168(10): 754-61, 2012 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22981268

ABSTRACT

Homonymous visual field loss is a common consequence of stroke and traumatic brain injury. It is associated with an adverse functional prognosis and has implications on day-to-day activities such as driving, reading, and safe navigation. Early recovery is expected in around half of cases, and may be associated with a return in V1 activity. In stable disease, recovery is unlikely beyond 3 and certainly 6 months. Rehabilitative approaches generally target three main areas, encompassing a range of techniques with variable success: visual aids aim to expand or relocate the affected visual field; eye movement training builds upon compensatory strategies to improve explorative saccades; visual field restitution aims to improve visual processing within the damaged field itself. All these approaches seem to offer modest improvements with repeated practice, with none clearly superior to the rest. However, a number of areas are demonstrating particular promise currently, including simple web-based training initiatives, and work on neuroimaging and learning. The research interest in this area is encouraging, and it is to be hoped that future trials can better untangle and control for the number of complicated confounds, so that we will be in a much better position to evaluate and select the most appropriate therapy for patients.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/rehabilitation , Stroke Rehabilitation , Vision Disorders/etiology , Vision Disorders/rehabilitation , Visual Cortex/injuries , Brain Injuries/complications , Eye Movements/physiology , Humans , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Optical Devices/statistics & numerical data , Physical Therapy Modalities , Stroke/complications , Visual Fields/physiology
4.
Exp Brain Res ; 200(1): 91-107, 2010 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19756551

ABSTRACT

Recent behavioural findings using dual-task paradigms demonstrate the importance of both spatial and non-spatial working memory processes in inefficient visual search (Anderson et al. in Exp Psychol 55:301-312, 2008). Here, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we sought to determine whether brain areas recruited during visual search are also involved in working memory. Using visually matched spatial and non-spatial working memory tasks, we confirmed previous behavioural findings that show significant dual-task interference effects occur when inefficient visual search is performed concurrently with either working memory task. Furthermore, we find considerable overlap in the cortical network activated by inefficient search and both working memory tasks. Our findings suggest that the interference effects observed behaviourally may have arisen from competition for cortical processes subserved by these overlapping regions. Drawing on previous findings (Anderson et al. in Exp Brain Res 180:289-302, 2007), we propose that the most likely anatomical locus for these interference effects is the inferior and middle frontal cortex of the right hemisphere. These areas are associated with attentional selection from memory as well as manipulation of information in memory, and we propose that the visual search and working memory tasks used here compete for common processing resources underlying these mechanisms.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Cerebral Cortex/blood supply , Fixation, Ocular , Functional Laterality , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxygen/blood , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Time Factors , Verbal Behavior/physiology , Visual Pathways/blood supply , Visual Pathways/physiology , Young Adult
5.
Exp Brain Res ; 180(2): 289-302, 2007 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17310377

ABSTRACT

Visual search for target items embedded within a set of distracting items has consistently been shown to engage regions of occipital and parietal cortex, but the contribution of different regions of prefrontal cortex remains unclear. Here, we used fMRI to compare brain activity in 12 healthy participants performing efficient and inefficient search tasks in which target discriminability and the number of distractor items were manipulated. Matched baseline conditions were incorporated to control for visual and motor components of the tasks, allowing cortical activity associated with each type of search to be isolated. Region of interest analysis was applied to critical regions of prefrontal cortex to determine whether their involvement was common to both efficient and inefficient search, or unique to inefficient search alone. We found regions of the inferior and middle frontal cortex were only active during inefficient search, whereas an area in the superior frontal cortex (in the region of FEF) was active for both efficient and inefficient search. Thus, regions of ventral as well as dorsal prefrontal cortex are recruited during inefficient search, and we propose that this activity is related to processes that guide, control and monitor the allocation of selective attention.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Male , Oxygen/blood , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Prefrontal Cortex/blood supply , Reaction Time/physiology
6.
Neurology ; 67(3): 485-7, 2006 Aug 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16625001

ABSTRACT

The authors examined oculomotor function to identify a biomarker of disease progression in genetically confirmed preclinical and early clinical Huntington disease (HD). Initiation deficits of voluntary-guided, but not reflexive, saccades were characteristic of preclinical HD. Saccadic slowing and delayed reflexive saccades were demonstrated in clinical but not preclinical HD. Saccadic measures provide biomarkers of disease progression in both preclinical and early clinical stages of HD.


Subject(s)
Huntington Disease/complications , Ocular Motility Disorders/etiology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Biomarkers , Disease Progression , Female , Humans , Huntington Disease/pathology , Male , Middle Aged
7.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 1039: 176-83, 2005 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15826972

ABSTRACT

The analysis of saccades offers an opportunity to study a number of different cognitive processes, such as visuospatial attention, working memory, and volitional conflict. A study of saccades in patients with visuospatial hemineglect, who performed a visual search task, showed repeated fixations on targets previously discovered, yet they often failed to retain the information that a particular target had previously been discovered. High-resolution structural brain scanning showed that this abnormality was due either to a lesion in the right intraparietal sulcus or the right inferior frontal lobe. Detailed analysis of the scanpaths suggested that the former location was associated with an accumulating impairment in remapping target locations across saccades or impaired memory of previously inspected target locations, whereas the latter location was more consistent with a failure to inhibit responses to rightward locations. When combined with a spatial bias to the right, such deficits might explain why many neglect patients often reexamine rightward targets, at the expense of items to their left. The functions of the supplementary eye field (SEF), in the medial frontal lobe, in relation to saccade generation are controversial. A series of studies in a patient with a focal lesion of the right SEF has indicated an important role for the SEF in the rapid self-control of saccadic eye movements and in set-switching (i.e., implementing control in situations of response conflict when ongoing saccadic plans have to be changed rapidly), rather than monitoring errors. In a recent fMRI study of normal subjects, it was shown that the SEF is involved in implementing the resolution of any volitional conflict, whereas other presupplementary motor areas are involved in the generation of volitional plans and processing volitional conflict.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Saccades/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Brain Mapping , Frontal Lobe/pathology , Functional Laterality , Humans , Oculomotor Nerve/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Perceptual Disorders/psychology
9.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 75(10): 1443-8, 2004 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15377693

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: We describe a novel rehabilitation tool for patients with homonymous hemianopia based on a visual search (VS) paradigm that is portable, inexpensive, and easy to deploy. We hypothesised that by training patients to improve the efficiency of eye movements made in their blind field their disability would be alleviated. METHODS: Twenty nine patients with homonymous visual field defects (HVFD) without neglect practised VS paradigms in 20 daily sessions over one month. Search fields comprising randomly positioned target and distracter elements, differing by a single feature, were displayed for three seconds on a dedicated television monitor in the patients' homes. Improvements were assessed by examining response time (RT), error rates in VS, perimetric visual fields (VFs) and visual search fields (VSFs), before and after treatment. Functional improvements were measured using objective visual tasks which represented activities of daily living (ADL) and a subjective questionnaire. RESULTS: As a group the patients had significantly shorter mean RT in VS after training (p<0.001) and demonstrated a variety of mechanisms to account for this. Improvements were confined to the training period and maintained at follow up. Three patients had significantly longer RT after training. They had high initial error rates which improved with training. Patients performed ADL tasks significantly faster after training and reported significant subjective improvements. There was no concomitant enlargement of the VF, but there was a small but significant enlargement of the VSF. CONCLUSION: Patients can improve VS with practice. This usually involves shorter RTs, but occasionally a longer RT in a complex speed-accuracy trade-off. These changes translate to improved overall visual function, assessed objectively and subjectively, suggesting that they represent robust training effects. The underlying mechanism may involve the adoption of compensatory eye movement strategies.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements , Hemianopsia/rehabilitation , Activities of Daily Living , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reaction Time , Treatment Outcome , Visual Fields
11.
Neuropsychologia ; 40(12): 1891-901, 2002.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12207988

ABSTRACT

Recent accounts have proposed that orbitofrontal cerebral cortex mediates the control of behavior based on emotional feedback and its somatic correlates. Here, we describe the performance of a patient with circumscribed damage to orbitofrontal cortex during a task that requires switching between sensory-motor mappings, contingent on the occurrence of positive and negative reward feedbacks. In this test, normal subjects and other patients with prefrontal damage show an increase in latencies for eye movements towards locations at which a negative feedback was presented on the preceding trial. In contrast, our patient does not show this reward-dependent inhibition of return effect on saccades. She was also found to make an increased rate of ocular refixations during visual search and used a disorganized search strategy in a token foraging task. These findings suggest that orbital regions of the prefrontal cortex mediate an inhibitory effect on actions directed towards locations that have been subject to negative reinforcement. Further, this mechanism seems to play a role in controlling natural search and foraging behavior.


Subject(s)
Orbit/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Aged , Craniotomy , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Gambling/psychology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Meningioma/pathology , Meningioma/surgery , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Orbit/pathology , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reading , Reinforcement, Psychology , Reward , Tomography, X-Ray Computed , Visual Perception/physiology
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 40(10): 1729-36, 2002.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11992660

ABSTRACT

In the antisaccade paradigm, subjects are instructed to fixate a central point, and then move their eyes towards a position in space in the opposite direction but equidistant to a peripheral, sudden onset target. Antisaccade errors occur when subjects are "distracted" by the target and make a saccade towards it. These errors are more common in patients with schizophrenia but the underlying cause remains unclear. To determine whether antisaccade errors simply reflect a general inability to maintain fixation or are the consequence of a more specific deficit in the strategic control of internally generated actions, patients with first-episode schizophrenia and healthy controls performed three saccadic paradigms which shared the core feature of requiring a prepotent saccade to be suppressed, but varied in their concurrent cognitive demands. We found that both groups showed an increase in errors as the cognitive demands increased across task. However, this increase was significantly steeper in the schizophrenic patients than in the controls. We also found that schizophrenic patients were as able as controls to inhibit prepotent saccades towards a target in a paradigm with no other cognitive demands. Possible explanations of these results include reduced working memory resources and impaired motor preparation in schizophrenia.


Subject(s)
Saccades , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Schizophrenic Psychology , Adult , Attention , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Schizophrenia/diagnosis , Task Performance and Analysis
16.
Theor Appl Genet ; 105(6-7): 1075-1086, 2002 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12582936

ABSTRACT

A comparative map of American wildrice ( Zizania palustris var. interior L.) was used to identify loci controlling seed shattering, plant height, maturity, tiller number, plant habit, panicle length seed length, and color traits. Two to six significant quantitative-trait-loci (QTLs, P < 0.05) were detected for each trait evaluated, representing the first trait-mapping in wildrice. The chosen population was designed to emphasize the mapping of loci controlling the shattering trait, which is the most important trait in the management of this newly domesticated species. Three loci were detected that controlled the discretely categorized variation between shattering and non-shattering plants. Seed-shattering loci were detected and validated among the F(2) and F(3) generations. A multiple regression model with these three loci described 49.6% of the additive genetic variation. A genetic model with the same three loci including dominance and locus interactions predicted the shattering versus non-shattering phenotype at a success rate of 87%. The comparative map was based on mapped RFLP markers used in white rice ( Oryza sativa L.) and other grass species. Anchor loci provided a reference point for the identification of potential orthologous genes on the basis of white rice mutant loci and consensus grass species QTLs. Candidate orthologous loci were identified among all traits evaluated. The study underscores the benefits of extending trait analysis through comparative mapping, as well as challenges of QTL analysis in a newly domesticated species.

17.
Neuropsychologia ; 40(4): 411-22, 2002.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11684174

ABSTRACT

We have taken a novel approach to the study of problem solving involving the detailed analysis of natural scanning eye movements during the 'one touch' Tower of London task. Control subjects and patients with idiopathic Parkinson's disease (PDs) viewed a series of pictures depicting two arrangements of coloured balls in pockets within the upper and lower halves of a computer display. The task was to plan (but not execute) the shortest movement sequence required to rearrange the balls in one half of the display (the Workspace) to match the arrangement in the opposite half (the Goalspace) and indicate the number of moves required for problem solution. As problem complexity increased, control subjects spent proportionally more time fixating the Workspace region. This pattern was found regardless of whether subjects were instructed to solve problems by rearranging balls in the lower or upper visual fields. The distribution of gaze within the Workspace was also found to be problem dependent, with gaze being selectively directed towards the problem critical balls. In contrast, PDs were found to make more errors in the task and failed to show any dissociation in the amount of time fixating the two halves of the display. This pattern suggests that the patients had difficulty in encoding and/or maintaining current goals during problem solving, consistent with a role for fronto-striatal circuits in mechanisms of working memory and attention.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Eye Movements , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Problem Solving , Aged , Attention , Case-Control Studies , Humans , Memory, Short-Term , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/psychology , Reaction Time , Severity of Illness Index
19.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 157(3): 284-91, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11605084

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: Smooth pursuit abnormalities have been observed in antipsychotic naive first-episode patients, suggesting that they are intrinsic to the illness. However, it is not clear whether these abnormalities are as severe as those observed in more chronic patients. In addition, although research suggests that there are no short-term effects of conventional antipsychotic medication, the effects of long-term antipsychotic medication on smooth pursuit eye movements are relatively unknown. OBJECTIVES: To determine the short and long term effects of antipsychotic medication on the smooth pursuit performance of first-episode and chronic patients with schizophrenia. METHODS: We compared the smooth pursuit performance of antipsychotic-treated and untreated first-episode and chronic schizophrenic patients with healthy controls using a comprehensive range of performance measures. This included velocity gain, the number, type and size of intrusive and corrective saccades, and the average time between the change in direction of the target and the change in direction of the eye movement, a measure of subjects' ability to predict target movement. RESULTS: Chronic schizophrenic patients had significantly reduced velocity gain, took longer to respond to the change in target direction and made more catch-up saccades than both first-episode schizophrenic patients and controls. First-episode patients were impaired relative to controls only on the measure of velocity gain. There were no differences between antipsychotic-naive and treated first-episode patients. Antipsychotic-free chronic patients were significantly less impaired on velocity gain than matched continuously treated chronic patients. These results were not influenced by group differences in age and symptom severity. CONCLUSIONS: These results show that: 1) the main index of smooth pursuit, velocity gain, is impaired early in the course of schizophrenia; 2) whereas velocity gain is unaffected by short-term (weeks) medication, it is worsened by chronic (years) treatment; 3) other indices of smooth pursuit, catch-up saccades and ability to predict target movement, are adversely influenced by illness chronicity rather than medication.


Subject(s)
Antipsychotic Agents/adverse effects , Pursuit, Smooth/drug effects , Schizophrenic Psychology , Adult , Antipsychotic Agents/therapeutic use , Chronic Disease , Female , Humans , Male , Prospective Studies , Schizophrenia/drug therapy , Time Factors
20.
Ann Neurol ; 50(3): 413-6, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11558800

ABSTRACT

Ocular flutter is a rare horizontal eye movement disorder characterized by rapid saccadic oscillations. It has been hypothesized that it is caused by loss of "pause" neuronal inhibition of "burst" neuron function in the paramedian pontine reticular formation (PPRF); however, there have been no imaging studies confirming such anatomical localization. We report the case of a woman with an acute attack of multiple sclerosis associated both with ocular flutter and a circumscribed pontine lesion, mainly involving the PPRF on magnetic resonance imaging. As she recovered from the attack, both the midline pontine lesion and the ocular flutter dramatically improved. This case is the first clear evidence that at least some cases of ocular flutter are due to lesions involving the PPRF.


Subject(s)
Multiple Sclerosis, Relapsing-Remitting/pathology , Ocular Motility Disorders/pathology , Pons/pathology , Reticular Formation/pathology , Adult , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Medulla Oblongata/pathology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...