Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 11 de 11
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 235(2): 507-515, 2017 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27787583

ABSTRACT

Hick's law states that increasing the number of response alternatives increases reaction time. Lawrence and colleagues report an exception to the law, whereby more alternatives lead to shorter saccadic reaction times (SRTs). Usher and McClelland (Psychol Rev 108(3):550-592. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.550 , 2001) predict such an anti-Hick's effect when accuracy is not prioritized in a task, which should result in higher error rates with more response alternatives, and in turn to a shorter right tail of the SRT distribution. In the current study, we aim to replicate the original controversial findings and we compare them to these predictions by examining error rates and SRT distributions. Two experiments were conducted where participants made rapid eye movements to one of few or many alternatives. In Experiment 1, the saccade target was an onset and participants started either with few or many possible target locations and then alternated between conditions. An anti-Hick's effect emerged only when participants had started with a small set-size block. In Experiment 2, placeholders were displayed at the possible target locations and independent groups were used. A reliable anti-Hick's effect in SRTs was observed. However, results did not meet the stated predictions: anticipations and false direction errors were never more frequent when the set size was larger and SRT differences between the two set-size conditions were not more pronounced at the slower end of the distributions. In line with Lawrence and colleagues, we speculate that initial motor preparation, and the subsequent inhibition to counteract a premature response, may induce the anti-Hick's effect.


Subject(s)
Reaction Time/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Young Adult
2.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 19(3): 456-61, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22454332

ABSTRACT

Participants in this study reached from central fixation to a lateral position that either contained or was opposite to the stimulus. Cognitive conflict was induced when the stimulus and response directions did not correspond. In the Simon task, the response direction was cued by the color of the lateral stimulus, and corresponding and noncorresponding trials varied randomly in the same block of trials, resulting in high uncertainty and long reaction times (RTs). In the stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) task, participants reached toward or away from the stimulus in separate blocks of trials, resulting in low uncertainty and short RTs. In the SRC task, cognitive conflict in noncorresponding trials slowed down RTs but hardly affected reach trajectories. In the Simon task, both RTs and reach trajectories were strongly influenced by stimulus-response correspondence. Despite the overall longer RTs in the Simon task, reaches were less direct and deviated toward the stimulus in noncorresponding trials. Thus, cognitive conflict was resolved before movement initiation in the SRC task, whereas it leaked into movement execution in the Simon task. Current theories of the Simon effect, such as the gating of response activation or response code decay, are inconsistent with our results. We propose that the SRC task was decomposed as approaching and avoiding the stimulus, which is sustained by stereotyped visuomotor routines. With complex stimulus-response relationships (Simon task), responses had to be coded as leftward and rightward, with more uncertainty about how to execute the action. This uncertainty permitted cognitive conflict to leak into the movement execution.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Conflict, Psychological , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Young Adult
3.
Nature ; 414(6859): 69-73, 2001 Nov 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11689944

ABSTRACT

Periodic bimanual movements are often the focus of studies of the basic organizational principles of human actions. In such movements there is a typical spontaneous tendency towards mirror symmetry. Even involuntary slips from asymmetrical movement patterns into symmetry occur, but not vice versa. Traditionally, this phenomenon has been interpreted as a tendency towards co-activation of homologous muscles, probably originating in motoric neuronal structures. Here we provide evidence contrary to this widespread assumption. We show for two prominent experimental models-bimanual finger oscillation and bimanual four-finger tapping-that the symmetry bias is actually towards spatial, perceptual symmetry, without regard to the muscles involved. We suggest that spontaneous coordination phenomena of this kind are purely perceptual in nature. In the case of a bimanual circling model, our findings reveal that highly complex, even 'impossible' movements can easily be performed with only simple visual feedback. A 'motoric' representation of the performed perceptual oscillation patterns is not necessary. Thus there is no need to translate such a 'motoric' into a 'perceptual' representation or vice versa, using 'internal models' (ref. 29). We suggest that voluntary movements are organized by way of a representation of the perceptual goals, whereas the corresponding motor activity, of sometimes high complexity, is spontaneously and flexibly tuned in.


Subject(s)
Motor Skills/physiology , Perception/physiology , Fingers , Hand , Humans , Models, Neurological , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Oscillometry
4.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 27(4): 829-40, 2001 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11518146

ABSTRACT

The judged final position of a moving stimulus has been suggested to be shifted in the direction of motion because of mental extrapolation (representational momentum). However, a perceptual explanation is possible: The eyes overshoot the final position of the target, and because of a foveal bias, the judged position is shifted in the direction of motion. To test this hypothesis, the authors replicated previous studies, but instead of having participants indicate where the target vanished, the authors probed participants' perceptual focus by presenting probe stimuli close to the vanishing point. Identification of probes in the direction of target motion was more accurate immediately after target offset than it was with a delay. Another experiment demonstrated that judgments of the final position of a moving target are affected by whether the eyes maintain fixation or follow the target. The results are more consistent with a perceptual explanation than with a memory account.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Humans , Random Allocation
5.
Percept Psychophys ; 63(5): 862-74, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11521852

ABSTRACT

It has been argued that two distinct maps of visual space are formed: a cognitive map that is susceptible to illusions, and a motor map that represents the physical world veridically. In the present study, subjects responded to a nonspatial attribute of a visual target stimulus by pressing a left or right key, while an illusory horizontal displacement of the target was induced. A Simon-type effect was obtained to the induced target motion or position shift-that is, responses were faster when the illusory target motion or location corresponded to the response position. Further experiments indicated that the observed effects cannot be accounted for by attentional shifts. These results suggest that the content of the cognitive map does not only influence perceptual judgments but is also responsible for the automatic activation of response codes. In other words, perception and action seem to be fed by a common, cognitively penetrable, spatial representation.


Subject(s)
Attention , Motion Perception , Optical Illusions , Orientation , Reaction Time , Adult , Functional Laterality , Humans , Psychomotor Performance
6.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 27(4): 1101-9, 2001 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11486921

ABSTRACT

The present study investigated whether and how visual memory and haptic perception are related. Participants were required to compare a visual reference velocity with a visual test velocity separated by a 4-s interval. During the retention interval, a fast or slow hand movement was performed. Although the hand movement was not visible, effects of the speed of the distracting body movement occurred. Slow movements resulted in a lowering of the represented visual velocity, whereas fast movements heightened the represented velocity. Subsequent experiments extended the effect to body movements that differed from the visual motion and ruled out the possibility that the effect was due to changes in visual perception or interference from semantic, verbal, and acoustic memory codes. Perhaps haptic velocity information and visual velocity information stored in short-term memory are blended.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term , Movement , Perceptual Masking , Visual Perception , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Female , Hand , Humans , Judgment , Male , Memory
7.
Percept Psychophys ; 63(7): 1153-70, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11766941

ABSTRACT

Current theories of arrival time have difficulty explaining performance in the common but neglected case of nonlinear approach. Global tau, a variable supposed to guide time-to-passage (TTP) judgments of objects approaching on linear trajectories, does not apply to circular movement. However, TTP judgments are surprisingly accurate in such cases. We simulated movement through a three-dimensional cloud of point-lights on various circular trajectories. Arrival-time judgments were found to be above chance when observers had to determine which of two expansionless targets would pass them first. Similar to the inside bias observed in heading studies on circular trajectories, observers showed a strong bias to select the target on the inside of their own curved motion path as passing by first. Analysis of the projected target motion revealed that targets on the inside had lower optical velocities and relatively high optical acceleration rates. Empirical TTP judgments agreed best with a strategy based on relative optical velocity changes.


Subject(s)
Judgment , Motion Perception , Time Perception , Adult , Computer Simulation , Distance Perception , Female , Humans , Male , Psychophysics , Signal Detection, Psychological
8.
Q J Exp Psychol A ; 53(4): 1163-85, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11131818

ABSTRACT

We investigated whether the representation of an observed causal movement is influenced by its observed effect. Subjects watched displays showing collisions between two objects. In this "launching event" (Michotte, 1946/1963), one of the two objects (Object A) started to move and set a second, initially stationary, object (Object B) into motion, which gave a strong impression of apparent causality. The apparent effectiveness of A's movement was manipulated by varying the velocities of A and B. When the velocity of B was higher than that of A, the effectiveness of the collision was high; when it was smaller it was low. Then, subjects were asked to reproduce the velocity of the causal movement. Reproduced velocity followed the velocity of both Object A and Object B, which supports the hypothesis that the effect of a movement is integrated with its apparent cause. However, when apparent causality was reduced by changing the direction of motion of B or by covering the point of collision, the influence of the effect on the representation of the cause persisted, suggesting that retroactive interference may account for the findings. The interference effect could not be reduced to temporal recency or spatial integration and was not obtained in the reverse temporal order (proactive interference). Rather, the two successive movements were blended in memory.


Subject(s)
Causality , Mental Recall , Motion Perception , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation , Probability Learning , Psychophysics
9.
Vision Res ; 40(27): 3703-15, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11090663

ABSTRACT

When observers are asked to localize the final position of a moving target, the judged position is usually displaced from the actual position in the direction of motion. The short-term time course of the displacement was investigated to test theories that attribute the localization error to spatial and temporal properties of human perception or to representational momentum. It was found that briefly after target offset, the judged position is already displaced in the direction of motion. It is argued that the shift results from eye movements after target offset that move the target's persisting image in the direction of motion.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Perceptual Distortion/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Memory/physiology
10.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 26(2): 634-47, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10811167

ABSTRACT

In speech perception, phonetic information can be acquired optically as well as acoustically. The motor theory of speech perception holds that motor control structures are involved in the processing of visible speech, whereas perceptual accounts do not make this assumption. Motor involvement in speech perception was examined by showing participants response-irrelevant movies of a mouth articulating /ba/ or /da/ and asking them to verbally respond with either the same or a different syllable. The letters "Ba" and "Da" appeared on the speaker's mouth to indicate which response was to be performed. A reliable interference effect was observed. In subsequent experiments, perceptual interference was ruled out by using response-unrelated imperative stimuli and by preexposing the relevant stimulus information. Further, it was demonstrated that simple directional features (opening and closing) do not account for the effect. Rather, the present study provides evidence for the view that visible speech is processed up to a late, response-related processing stage, as predicted by the motor theory of speech perception.


Subject(s)
Lipreading , Phonetics , Speech Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Speech Acoustics , Speech Intelligibility
11.
Z Exp Psychol ; 44(3): 394-430, 1997.
Article in German | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9522677

ABSTRACT

Pictures often do not appear distorted even when viewed at oblique angles. Three hypotheses have been put forth to explain this robustness of virtual space toward affine transformations. First, array specificity holds that the perception of depicted space is fully specified by the information available at the point of observation. Second, the notion of a compensatory mechanism involves an unconscious recreation of the scene according to the original viewpoint. Third, the indiscrimination hypothesis denies the ability of the visual system to resolve or detect affine transformations up to a certain degree. Three experiments were conducted to investigate these claims. Using a double projection technique devised by Cutting (1987), Experiment I showed that observers are able to discriminate and compensate, to some degree, for affine transformations if information about the projection surface is available. However, observers relied on relative image velocities rather than reconstructing the object. In Experiment 2 additional observer motion was simulated. In single and double projection trials that required more difficult judgments of object rotation, compensation was poor and observers seemed to rely on local cues. Finally, real and simulated rotation of the projection surface revealed that observers are able to compensate for only one primary projection surface slant. The results reject the indiscrimination hypothesis and support the notion of array specificity.


Subject(s)
Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Perceptual Distortion , Adult , Attention , Cues , Depth Perception , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Motion Perception , Optical Illusions , Psychophysics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...