Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 47
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Cereb Cortex Commun ; 4(2): tgad005, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37188067

ABSTRACT

The extrastriate body area (EBA) is a region in the lateral occipito-temporal cortex (LOTC), which is sensitive to perceived body parts. Neuroimaging studies suggested that EBA is related to body and tool processing, regardless of the sensory modalities. However, how essential this region is for visual tool processing and nonvisual object processing remains a matter of controversy. In this preregistered fMRI-guided repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) study, we examined the causal involvement of EBA in multisensory body and tool recognition. Participants used either vision or haptics to identify 3 object categories: hands, teapots (tools), and cars (control objects). Continuous theta-burst stimulation (cTBS) was applied over left EBA, right EBA, or vertex (control site). Performance for visually perceived hands and teapots (relative to cars) was more strongly disrupted by cTBS over left EBA than over the vertex, whereas no such object-specific effect was observed in haptics. The simulation of the induced electric fields confirmed that the cTBS affected regions including EBA. These results indicate that the LOTC is functionally relevant for visual hand and tool processing, whereas the rTMS over EBA may differently affect object recognition between the 2 sensory modalities.

2.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 15(3): 638-645, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35951577

ABSTRACT

We are adept at discriminating object properties such as softness and temperature using touch. Previous studies have investigated the nature of each object property, but the interactions between these properties are not fully understood. Tactile softness perception relies on multiple sensory cues such as the size of the contact area, indentation depth, and force exerted. In addition to these cues, the temperature of the stimulus may contribute to tactile softness perception by changing the sensitivity to changes in stimulus compliance. To test this hypothesis, we conducted two psychophysical experiments in which the subjects estimated the magnitude of perceived softness after touching deformable objects. We varied the compliance and temperature of the stimuli. The linear functions of compliance fit to the magnitude estimates under cold conditions (9-15°C) were steeper than the functions fit to the magnitude estimates under room temperature (21-25°C). These results indicate that temperature can sharpen our tactile softness perception of deformable surfaces by increasing the sensitivity to differences in compliance.


Subject(s)
Touch Perception , Cues , Humans , Temperature , Touch
3.
Front Psychol ; 13: 830306, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35369145

ABSTRACT

Contrary to the assumption of arbitrariness in modern linguistics, sound symbolism, which is the non-arbitrary relationship between sounds and meanings, exists. Sound symbolism, including the "Bouba-Kiki" effect, implies the universality of such relationships; individuals from different cultural and linguistic backgrounds can similarly relate sound-symbolic words to referents, although the extent of these similarities remains to be fully understood. Here, we examined if subjects from different countries could similarly infer the surface texture properties from words that sound-symbolically represent hardness in Japanese. We prepared Japanese sound-symbolic words of which novelty was manipulated by a genetic algorithm (GA). Japanese speakers in Japan and English speakers in both Singapore and the United States rated these words based on surface texture properties (hardness, warmness, and roughness), as well as familiarity. The results show that hardness-related words were rated as harder and rougher than softness-related words, regardless of novelty and countries. Multivariate analyses of the ratings classified the hardness-related words along the hardness-softness dimension at over 80% accuracy, regardless of country. Multiple regression analyses revealed that the number of speech sounds /g/ and /k/ predicted the ratings of the surface texture properties in non-Japanese countries, suggesting a systematic relationship between phonetic features of a word and perceptual quality represented by the word across culturally and linguistically diverse samples.

4.
Neurosci Res ; 180: 48-57, 2022 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35218859

ABSTRACT

Despite the multiple regions and neural networks associated with value-based decision-making, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is possible a particularly important one. Although the role of the OFC in reinforcer devaluation tasks, which assess the ability to represent identity, sensory qualities, and subjective values of the expected outcomes, has been established, the specific aspect represented in this area remains unclear. In this study, using functional magnetic resonance imaging, wherein participants rated the palatability of 128 food items using photographs, we investigated whether the human OFC represents object identity, sensory qualities, or value. Employing many items helped us dissociate object identity from sensory qualities and values; the inferred sensory qualities of identical items were manipulated by a change in metabolic state. Moreover, value differences between items were analytically controlled by employing a technique similar to age adjustment. The palatability ratings for food items significantly decreased after a meal. Using representational similarity analysis, we confirmed that the OFC represents value. Moreover, identical items were represented similarly in the lateral OFC in a given metabolic state; however, these representations were altered post-feeding. Importantly, this change was not explained by subjective value, suggesting that the OFC represents sensory quality and value, but not object identity.


Subject(s)
Prefrontal Cortex , Reward , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging
5.
J Agric Food Chem ; 70(1): 247-259, 2022 Jan 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34965128

ABSTRACT

Application of the sensomics concept on dried scallops, a Japanese specialty produced from the adductor muscle of scallops, revealed after activity-guided fractionation with subsequent (comparative) taste dilution analyses besides nucleotides, amino acids, organic acids, and inorganic ions, the presence of taste-modulating quaternary ammonium compounds and opines in highly taste-active fractions. In order to recreate the taste of dried scallops, two independent quantitation approaches were applied and compared. The first approach used multiple targeted UHPLC-MS/MS and high-performance ion chromatography methods. Besides already established quantitation methods for basic taste compounds, a new HILIC-UHPLC-MS/MSMRM method for the quantitation of chromatographically challenging opines, using synthesized stable isotope-labeled standards, was developed. Furthermore, a qHNMR approach was applied, enabling a direct identification and quantitation of organic taste compounds in a food extract without prior fractionation using a reference 1H NMR database. Both methods yielded similar quantitative results of taste-active compounds in dried scallop extracts and subsequent taste recombination experiments based on these data were able to recreate the taste of dried scallops.


Subject(s)
Pectinidae , Taste , Animals , Flavoring Agents/analysis , Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy , Tandem Mass Spectrometry
7.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 16510, 2021 08 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34389767

ABSTRACT

Touching an object can elicit affective sensations. Because these sensations are critical for social interaction, tactile preferences may be adapted to the characteristics of the human body. We have previously shown that compliance, a physical correlate of softness, increased the tactile pleasantness of a deformable surface. However, the extent to which object compliance similar to the human body elicits tactile pleasantness remains unknown. We addressed this question by using a wide range of compliances and by measuring the distribution of compliance of human body parts. The participants numerically estimated the perceived pleasantness or softness while pushing tactile stimuli with their right index fingers. The perceived softness monotonically increased with increasing compliance and then leveled off around the end of the stimulus range. By contrast, pleasantness showed an inverse U pattern as a function of compliance, reaching the maximum between 5 and 7 mm/N. This range of compliance was within that for both hand and arm. These results indicate that objects with similar compliance levels as those of human body parts yield the highest pleasantness when pushing them.


Subject(s)
Touch Perception/physiology , Touch/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Fingers , Hardness , Human Body , Humans , Individuality , Male , Pleasure , Stress, Mechanical , Young Adult
8.
Soc Neurosci ; 16(4): 448-465, 2021 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34133907

ABSTRACT

The extrastriate body area (EBA) in the lateral occipito-temporal cortex has an important role in reciprocal interaction, as it detects congruence between self and other's hand actions. However, it is unclear whether the EBA can detect congruence regardless of the type of action. In the present study, we examined the neural substrate underlying congruence detection of three types of actions: hand gestures, vocalizations, and facial expressions. A univariate analysis revealed a congruency effect, especially for imitating action, for all three types of actions in the EBA. A multi-voxel pattern analysis classifier in the EBA was able to distinguish between initiating interaction from responding to interaction in all experiments. Correspondingly, the congruency effect in the EBA revealed by univariate analysis was stronger for responding to than for initiating interaction. These findings suggest that the EBA might contribute to detect congruence regardless of the body part used (i.e. face or hand) and the type of action (i.e. gestural or vocal). Moreover, initiating and responding to interaction might be processed differently within the EBA. This study highlights the role of the EBA in comparing between self and other's actions beyond hand actions.Running head: Function of EBA in reciprocal imitation.


Subject(s)
Gestures , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Brain Mapping , Facial Expression , Humans , Imitative Behavior
9.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 7399, 2021 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33795716

ABSTRACT

Unlike the assumption of modern linguistics, there is non-arbitrary association between sound and meaning in sound symbolic words. Neuroimaging studies have suggested the unique contribution of the superior temporal sulcus to the processing of sound symbolism. However, because these findings are limited to the mapping between sound symbolism and visually presented objects, the processing of sound symbolic information may also involve the sensory-modality dependent mechanisms. Here, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment to test whether the brain regions engaged in the tactile processing of object properties are also involved in mapping sound symbolic information with tactually perceived object properties. Thirty-two healthy subjects conducted a matching task in which they judged the congruency between softness perceived by touch and softness associated with sound symbolic words. Congruency effect was observed in the orbitofrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, insula, medial superior frontal gyrus, cingulate gyrus, and cerebellum. This effect in the insula and medial superior frontal gyri was overlapped with softness-related activity that was separately measured in the same subjects in the tactile experiment. These results indicate that the insula and medial superior frontal gyrus play a role in processing sound symbolic information and relating it to the tactile softness information.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Brain/physiology , Nerve Net , Sound , Symbolism , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Data Analysis , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuroimaging , Young Adult
10.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 11189, 2020 07 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32636415

ABSTRACT

The sense of touch allows us to infer objects' physical properties, while the same input also produces affective sensations. These affective sensations are important for interpersonal relationships and personal well-being, which raises the possibility that tactile preferences are adapted to the characteristics of the skin. Previous studies examined how physical properties such as surface roughness and temperature influence affective sensations; however, little is known about the effect of compliance (physical correlate of softness) on pleasantness. Thus, we investigated the psychophysical link between softness and pleasantness. Pieces of human skin-like rubber with different compliances were pressed against participants' fingers. Two groups of participants numerically estimated the perceived magnitude of either pleasantness or softness. The perceived magnitude of pleasantness and softness both increased monotonically as a function of increasing object compliance, levelling off at around the end of the stimulus range. However, inter-subject variability was greater for pleasantness than for perceived softness, whereas the slope of the linear function fit to the magnitude estimates was steeper for softness than for pleasantness. These results indicate that object compliance is a critical physical determinant for pleasantness, whereas the effect of compliance on pleasantness was more variable among individuals than the effect on softness was.


Subject(s)
Pleasure , Touch Perception , Female , Fingers/physiology , Humans , Male , Touch , Young Adult
11.
Soc Neurosci ; 15(3): 311-323, 2020 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31944165

ABSTRACT

Gestural interaction, where a person initiates interaction (initiator) and another person responds to it (follower), changes during development. The neural network comprising the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), inferior parietal lobule (IPL), and the lateral occipito-temporal cortex (LOTC) is relevant to gestural interaction. The LOTC includes the extrastriate body area (EBA). Activation of these brain regions depends on the initiating/following role in adults. We conducted functional magnetic resonance imaging study on 18 children and 18 adults, to elucidate developmental changes of the neural mechanism underlying gestural interaction. We manipulated the initiating/following role (initiating/following) and congruency (congruent/incongruent) of executed and observed actions. After analyzing regional brain activity, we assessed psycho-physiological interaction to examine functional connectivity. Activation in the IFG and connectivity between the IFG and EBA in the Initiating rather than Following condition, which might be associated with evaluating social relevance, was stronger in adults than in children. The increase of the incongruency effect in the following condition (relative to the initiating condition) in the bilateral IPL was significantly attenuated in children compared with adults. These results suggest that the fronto-parieto-temporal network, involved in gestural interactions, undergoes developmental changes.


Subject(s)
Gestures , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Psychomotor Performance , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Brain Mapping , Child , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neural Pathways/physiology , Psychophysics , Young Adult
12.
Cereb Cortex Commun ; 1(1): tgaa007, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34296088

ABSTRACT

The lateral occipitotemporal cortex (LOTC) that responds to human bodies and body parts has been implicated in social development and neurodevelopmental disorders like autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Neuroimaging studies using a representational similarity analysis (RSA) revealed that body representation in the LOTC of typically developing (TD) adults is categorized into 3 clusters: action effector body parts, noneffector body parts, and face parts. However, its organization of younger people (i.e., children and adolescents) and its association with individual traits remain unclear. In this functional MRI study, TD adults and children/adolescents observed photographs of hands, feet, arms, legs, chests, waists, upper/lower faces, the whole body, and chairs. The univariate analysis showed that fewer child/adolescent participants showed left LOTC activation in response to whole-body images (relative to those of chairs) than adult participants. Contrastingly, the RSA on both age groups revealed a comparable body representation with 3 clusters of body parts in the bilateral LOTC. Hence, this result indicates that, although response to whole-body images can differ, LOTC body part representations for children/ adolescents and adults are highly similar. Furthermore, sensory atypicality is associated with spatial LOTC organization, suggesting the importance of this region for understanding individual difference, which is frequently observed in ASD.

13.
IEEE Trans Haptics ; 13(3): 571-577, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31725388

ABSTRACT

Movement of a grid of bars between the two hands creates the tactile illusion of a velvet-like material, namely, the velvet hand illusion (VHI). It was recently proposed that the VHI is caused by a masking effect; bar movement suppresses conscious perception of tactile inputs from the opposing hand. If this hypothesis sufficiently explains the VHI, the physical properties of the opposing hand should not affect the illusion. Another hypothesis suggests that the integration of inputs from the grid of bars and the hands plays a critical role in the VHI. To compare these two hypotheses, the VHI was elicited under two conditions; the grid of bars was between one hand and a soft texture or the grid of bars was between one hand and a hard texture. A hand was stimulated by moving bars while contacting the stationary texture held by the opposing hand. The grid of bars with the soft texture induced a stronger illusion and softer feeling than that with the hard texture. This result supports the integration hypothesis in which tactile inputs from both bars and textures attached to the opposing hand are integrated.


Subject(s)
Hand , Illusions , Touch Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Physical Stimulation , Young Adult
14.
J Neurosci Methods ; 327: 108393, 2019 11 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31415843

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Tactile stimulation used to induce emotional responses is often not well-controlled. Replicating the same tactile stimulations across studies is difficult, compared to replicating visual and auditory modalities, which have standardized stimulus sets. Standardizing a stimulation method by replicating stimuli across studies is necessary to further elucidate emotional responses in neuroscience research using tactile stimulation. THE NEW METHOD: We developed a tactile stimulation device. The device's ultrasonic motor and optical force sensor have the following criteria: (1) controls the physical property of stimuli, pressure, and stroking speed; (2) measures actual touch timing; (3) is safe to use in a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner; and (4) produces low noise in electroencephalography (EEG) and MRI. RESULTS: The noise level of the device's drive was sufficiently low. For the EEG experiment, we successfully used signal processing to diminish the commercial power supply noise. For functional MRI (fMRI) scans, we found <5% signal loss occurred during device rotation. COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHOD(S): We found no previous report about the noise level of a tactile stimulation device used to induce emotional responses during EEG and fMRI recordings. The signal loss rate was comparable with that of other robotic devices used in MRI scanners. Emotional feelings induced by this stimulation method were comparable with those elicited in other sensory modalities. CONCLUSIONS: The developed device could be used for cognitive-affective neuroscience research when conducting EEG and fMRI scans. The device should aid in standardizing affective tactile stimulation for research in psychology and cognitive neuroscience.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography/methods , Emotions/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Physical Stimulation/instrumentation , Female , Humans , Male , Physical Stimulation/methods , Touch Perception/physiology , Young Adult
15.
Front Psychol ; 10: 878, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31068868

ABSTRACT

Contexts of face perception are diverse. They range from the social environment to body postures, from the expresser's gaze direction to the tone of voice. In extending the research on contexts of face perception, we investigated people's perception of tears on a face. The act of shedding tears is often perceived as an expression of sad feelings aroused by experiencing loss, disappointment, or helplessness. Alternatively, tears may also represent the excessive intensity of any emotion, such as extreme fear during an unexpected encounter with a giant bear and extreme happiness when you win a competition. Investigating these competing interpretations of tears, we found that the addition of tears to different facial expressions made the expressions conceptually closer to sad expressions. In particular, the results of the similarity analysis showed that, after the addition of tears, patterns of ratings for anger, fear, disgust, and neutral facial expressions became more similar to those for sadness expressions. The effect of tears on the ratings of basic emotions and their patterns in facial expressions are discussed.

16.
Neuroimage ; 197: 156-166, 2019 08 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31029866

ABSTRACT

Humans are adept at perceiving physical properties of an object through touch. Tangible object properties can be categorized into two types: macro-spatial properties, including shape and orientation; and material properties, such as roughness, softness, and temperature. Previous neuroimaging studies have shown that roughness and temperature are extracted at nodes of a network, such as that involving the parietal operculum and insula, which is different from the network engaged in processing macro-spatial properties. However, it is unclear whether other perceptual dimensions pertaining to material properties engage the same regions. Here, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging study to test whether the parietal operculum and insula were involved in extracting tactually-perceived softness magnitude. Fifty-six healthy right-handed participants estimated perceived softness magnitude using their right middle finger. We presented three stimuli that had the same shape but different compliances. The force applied to the finger was manipulated at two levels. Classical mass-univariate analysis showed that activity in the parietal operculum, insula, and medial prefrontal cortex was positively associated with perceived softness magnitude, regardless of the applied force. Softness-related activity was stronger in the ventral striatum in the high-force condition than in the low-force condition. The multivariate voxel pattern analysis showed higher accuracy than chance levels and control regions in the parietal operculum/insula, postcentral gyrus, posterior parietal lobule, and middle occipital gyrus. These results indicate that a distributed set of the brain regions, including the parietal operculum and insula, is involved in representing perceived softness.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Touch Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neural Pathways/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Physical Stimulation , Young Adult
17.
Proc Biol Sci ; 286(1901): 20190467, 2019 04 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31014213

ABSTRACT

Many species use touching for reinforcing social structures, and particularly, non-human primates use social grooming for managing their social networks. However, it is still unclear how social touch contributes to the maintenance and reinforcement of human social networks. Human studies in Western cultures suggest that the body locations where touch is allowed are associated with the strength of the emotional bond between the person touched and the toucher. However, it is unknown to what extent this relationship is culturally universal and generalizes to non-Western cultures. Here, we compared relationship-specific, bodily touch allowance maps across one Western ( N = 386, UK) and one East Asian ( N = 255, Japan) country. In both cultures, the strength of the emotional bond was linearly associated with permissible touch area. However, Western participants experienced social touching as more pleasurable than Asian participants. These results indicate a similarity of emotional bonding via social touch between East Asian and Western cultures.


Subject(s)
Cross-Cultural Comparison , Touch , Adult , Female , Humans , Japan , Male , Middle Aged , Object Attachment , Social Behavior , United Kingdom
18.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 72(10): 2408-2422, 2019 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30895891

ABSTRACT

Social touch constitutes a critical component of human interactions. A gentle tap on the hand, for instance, can sometimes create emotional bonding and reduce interpersonal distance in social interactions. Evidence of tactile empathy suggests that touch can be experienced through both physical sensation and observation, yet vicarious perception of observed touch on an object as a function of the object's conceptual representation (e.g., Is this object identified as mine? Does this object feel like part of me?) remains less explored. Here we examined the affective judgement of social touch when the illusory sense of ownership over a dummy hand was manipulated through the rubber-hand illusion. When the same social touch was performed on either the real or the dummy hand, we found a similar sense of perceived pleasantness between the felt and observed touch, but only when the dummy hand was embodied; when it was not, the perceived pleasantness of the observed touch was lesser (an "embodiment effect"; Experiment 1). In addition, we found that the embodiment effect associated with the observed touch was insensitive to the way in which embodiment was manipulated (Experiment 2), and that this effect was specific to social but not neutral touch (Experiment 3). Taken together, our findings suggest a role of embodiment in the affective component of observed social touch and contribute to our understanding of tactile empathy for objects.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Empathy/physiology , Hand , Illusions/physiology , Interpersonal Relations , Social Perception , Touch Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Judgment/physiology , Male , Young Adult
19.
Cortex ; 108: 234-251, 2018 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30261368

ABSTRACT

Detecting relationships between our own actions and the subsequent actions of others is critical for our social behavior. Self-actions differ from those of others in terms of action kinematics, body identity, and feedback timing. Thus, the detection of social contingency between self-actions and those of others requires comparison and integration of these three dimensions. Neuroimaging studies have highlighted the role of the frontotemporal network in action representation, but the role of each node and their relationships are still controversial. Here, we conducted a functional MRI experiment to test the hypothesis that the lateral prefrontal cortex and lateral occipito-temporal cortex are critical for the integration processes for social contingency. Twenty-four adults performed right finger gestures and then observed them as feedback. We manipulated three parameters of visual feedback: action kinematics (same or different gestures), body identity (self or other), and feedback timing (simultaneous or delayed). Three-way interactions of these factors were observed in the left inferior and middle frontal gyrus (IFG/MFG). These areas were active when self-actions were directly fed back in real-time (i.e., the condition causing a sense of agency), and when participants observed gestures performed by others after a short delay (i.e., the condition causing social contingency). In contrast, the left extrastriate body area (EBA) was sensitive to the concordance of action kinematics regardless of body identity or feedback timing. Body identity × feedback timing interactions were observed in regions including the superior parietal lobule (SPL). An effective connectivity analysis supported the model wherein experimental parameters modulated connections from the occipital cortex to the IFG/MFG via the EBA and SPL. These results suggest that both social contingency and the sense of agency are achieved by hierarchical processing that begins with simple concordance coding in the left EBA, leading to the complex coding of social relevance in the left IFG/MFG.


Subject(s)
Imitative Behavior/physiology , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Brain Mapping , Cues , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Occipital Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Photic Stimulation , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Temporal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Young Adult
20.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 39(12): 4787-4801, 2018 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30096223

ABSTRACT

Humans are adept at perceiving textures through touch. Previous neuroimaging studies have identified a distributed network of brain regions involved in the tactile perception of texture. However, it remains unclear how nodes in this network contribute to the tactile awareness of texture. To examine the hypothesis that such awareness involves the interaction of the primary somatosensory cortex with higher order cortices, we conducted a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study utilizing the velvet hand illusion, in which an illusory velvet-like surface is perceived between the hands. Healthy participants were subjected to a strong illusion, a weak illusion, and tactile perception of real velvet. The strong illusion induced greater activation in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) than the weak illusion, and increases in such activation were positively correlated with the strength of the illusion. Furthermore, both actual and illusory perception of velvet induced common activation in S1. Psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis revealed that the strength of the illusion modulated the functional connectivity of S1 with each of the following regions: the parietal operculum, superior parietal lobule, precentral gyrus, insula, and cerebellum. The present results indicate that S1 is associated with the conscious tactile perception of textures, which may be achieved via interactions with higher order somatosensory areas.


Subject(s)
Cerebellum/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Connectome/methods , Illusions/physiology , Nerve Net/physiology , Somatosensory Cortex/physiology , Touch Perception/physiology , Adult , Cerebellum/diagnostic imaging , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Somatosensory Cortex/diagnostic imaging
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...