Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 14 de 14
Filter
1.
Psychol Med ; 40(9): 1541-8, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19951448

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) is a poorly understood disorder that involves a preoccupation with imagined or minor bodily defects. Only a few studies of neuropsychological function have been conducted. Two previous studies have indicated executive dysfunction in BDD. The current study sought to further define these executive deficits. METHOD: Fourteen DSM-IV BDD patients and 14 age- and sex-matched control participants took part. Because of the high incidence of co-morbidity in BDD, patients with co-morbid Axis I disorders were not excluded. Control participants had no history of psychiatric illness. All participants completed the following executive function (EF) tests: Spatial Span (SS), Spatial Working Memory (SWM) and the Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) task. They also completed the Pattern Recognition (PR) test, a test of visual memory (VM). RESULTS: BDD participants made significantly more between-search errors on the SWM task, an effect that increased with task difficulty. Between-search errors are an example of poor maintenance and manipulation of information. SOC results indicated slower subsequent thinking times (i.e. the time taken to plan) in BDD participants. There were no group differences in SS or PR scores. The severity of BDD, depressive or anxiety symptoms was not correlated with performance on any of the cognitive tasks. CONCLUSIONS: The results of this study indicate that BDD patients have EF deficits in on-line manipulation, planning and organization of information. By contrast, spatial memory capacity, motor speed and visual memory were intact. Considered with evidence from lesion and neuroimaging studies, these results suggest frontal lobe dysfunction in BDD.


Subject(s)
Body Dysmorphic Disorders/psychology , Executive Function , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Matched-Pair Analysis , Memory, Short-Term , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reaction Time
3.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry ; 35(2): 224-30, 2001 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11284905

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Increasingly, epidemiological studies are employing computerized and highly standardized interviews, such as the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI-Auto), to assess the prevalence of psychiatric illness. Recent studies conducted in specialist units have reported poor agreement between experienced clinicians' and CIDI-Auto diagnoses. In this study we investigated the concordance rate between clinicians and the CIDI-Auto for the diagnosis of six anxiety disorders and two mood disorders, whereby the CIDI-Auto was treated as the 'gold standard'. METHOD: Subjects were 262 patients who were assessed by a clinical psychologist or psychiatrist and completed the CIDI-Auto at a tertiary referral unit for anxiety and mood disorders. Agreement between the clinicians' diagnoses and the diagnoses generated by the CIDI-Auto according to both DSM-IV and ICD-10 codes, were examined by kappa statistics. Sensitivity and specificity values were also calculated. RESULTS: Agreement between clinicians and the CIDI-Auto (DSM-IV) ranged from poor for social phobia and posttraumatic stress disorder (kappa < 0.30) to moderate for obsessive- compulsive disorder (OCD; kappa = 0.52). Agreement between clinicians and the CIDI-Auto (ICD-10) ranged from poor for major depressive episode (kappa = 0.25) to moderate for OCD (kappa = 0.57). With the CIDI diagnosis treated as the gold standard, clinicians' diagnoses showed low sensitivity (kappa < 0.70) for all the disorders except for OCD (for ICD-10), but high specificity (kappa > 0.70) for all the disorders. CONCLUSION: Poor agreements between experienced clinicians and the CIDI-Auto were reported for anxiety and mood disorders in the current study. A major implication is that if diagnosis alone directed treatment, then patients could receive different treatments, depending on whether the computer, or a clinician, made the diagnosis.


Subject(s)
Anxiety Disorders/diagnosis , Interview, Psychological , Mood Disorders/diagnosis , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Adult , Decision Making , Female , Humans , Male , Sensitivity and Specificity
4.
Aust J Rural Health ; 9(2): 91-6, 2001 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11259963

ABSTRACT

Rural Australians have limited access to care for mental health problems. We describe a collaboration between the University of Melbourne Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry and a rural Area Mental Health Service to provide a specialist anxiety and depression treatment service in rural Victoria. The clinical service and the education and training approach are described.


Subject(s)
Academic Medical Centers/organization & administration , Anxiety Disorders/therapy , Community Mental Health Services/organization & administration , Depressive Disorder/therapy , Health Services Accessibility , Rural Health Services/organization & administration , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Ambulatory Care Facilities/organization & administration , Child , Cooperative Behavior , Humans , Interinstitutional Relations , Middle Aged , Organizational Case Studies , Victoria
5.
Psychol Med ; 29(5): 1227-33, 1999 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10576314

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Olfactory identification ability has been associated with processing in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), an area that has been implicated in the pathophysiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Although olfactory sensitivity is normal in patients with OCD, no study has investigated olfactory identification in this disorder. METHODS: A group of 20 subjects with OCD and 23 age- and education-matched controls performed a standardized test of olfactory identification. They also performed computerized tests of spatial memory span, spatial working memory and spatial recognition memory that have been shown previously to be sensitive to cognitive deficits in patients with OCD. RESULTS: Performance on the olfactory identification task, spatial recognition task and spatial span task was significantly worse in the OCD group than controls. CONCLUSIONS: While impairment in spatial cognition is consistent with previous studies of OCD, its significance for brain-behaviour models of OCD is unclear. However, the finding of abnormal olfactory identification in patients with OCD is consistent with the hypothesis that there is a disruption to processing at the level of the OFC in the disorder.


Subject(s)
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/complications , Olfaction Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Frontal Lobe/pathology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Psychological , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/psychology
6.
J Anxiety Disord ; 12(4): 271-92, 1998.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9699114

ABSTRACT

Slowed color naming of threat-related words on the modified Stroop task has been interpreted as indicative of selective processing biases, although alternative models have been proposed to explain inconsistent findings. The present study compared the performance of participants with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and normal controls on a computerized version of the modified Stroop, and investigated the relationship of Stroop effects to avoidant cognitive strategies. Contrary to predictions, control and OCD groups were notably faster at color naming OCD threat, general threat, and positive words compared to neutral words at the strategic level of awareness. This was not the case for OCD threat words at the automatic level of awareness where faster color naming was observed, although the expected differences between groups were not found. A thought replacement measure was generally predictive of OCD participants' automatic pattern of interference for negatively valenced words. Results indicate the complexity of the Stroop effect and the need for its ongoing review.


Subject(s)
Attention , Avoidance Learning , Awareness , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/psychology , Adult , Arousal , Color Perception , Defense Mechanisms , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/diagnosis , Personality Inventory , Problem Solving , Reaction Time , Semantics
7.
Arch Gen Psychiatry ; 55(5): 415-23, 1998 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9596044

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The neuropsychological dysfunction associated with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) has similarities to the deficits reported in other affective or anxiety disorders. We directly compared cognitive function in patients with OCD with that in matched patients with unipolar depression and panic disorder and healthy control subjects to establish the specific nature of neuropsychological deficits in OCD. METHODS: Thirty patients with OCD, 30 patients with panic disorder, 20 patients with unipolar depression, and 30 controls completed a computerized neuropsychological battery that assessed the accuracy and latency of executive, visual memory, and attentional functions. RESULTS: The groups did not differ according to age, years of education, or estimated IQ. However, we found group differences in cognitive performance. The patients with OCD were impaired on measures of spatial working memory, spatial recognition, and motor initiation and execution. In contrast, performance of these tasks by patients with panic disorder or depression did not differ from that of controls. There were no group differences for performance on the measures of planning, cognitive speed, pattern recognition, and delayed matching to sample, although patients with depression were impaired for attentional set shifting. CONCLUSIONS: Neuropsychological deficits were observed in patients with OCD that were not observed in matched patients with panic disorder or unipolar depression. As such, the cognitive dysfunction in OCD appears to be related to the specific illness processes associated with the disorder.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Neuropsychological Tests , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/diagnosis , Adult , Attention/physiology , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Depressive Disorder/diagnosis , Depressive Disorder/psychology , Diagnosis, Differential , Female , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology , Motor Skills/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/psychology , Panic Disorder/diagnosis , Panic Disorder/psychology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Set, Psychology , Space Perception/physiology
8.
Biol Psychiatry ; 43(5): 348-57, 1998 Mar 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9513750

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Although neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) have implicated the frontal cortex and subcortical structures in the pathophysiology of the disorder, few studies have examined cognitive function in patients with OCD on tasks validated in the assessment of frontal lobe and subcortical dysfunction. METHODS: The accuracy and latency of executive and visual memory function was assessed in 23 nondepressed OCD patients and 23 normal healthy controls matched for age, sex, education, and estimated IQ. RESULTS: The patients with OCD performed within the normal range on tasks of short-term memory capacity, delay dependent visual memory, pattern recognition, attentional shifting, and planning ability; however, specific cognitive deficits related to spatial working memory, spatial recognition, and motor initiation and execution were observed in the patient group. These deficits were not correlated with aspects of the patients' intellectual functioning or comorbid psychological symptoms, suggesting that the impairments were related to the specific clinical features of OCD. CONCLUSIONS: Patients with OCD showed specific cognitive deficits on tasks of executive and visual memory function. The pattern of impaired performance in these patients was qualitatively similar to the performance of patients with frontal lobe excisions and subcortical pathology on the same test battery, suggesting that the underlying pathophysiology of the disorder could best be conceptualized as reflecting dysfunction of frontal-striatal systems.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/psychology , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Neostriatum/physiopathology , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/physiopathology , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/psychology , Adult , Aged , Attention/physiology , Cognition Disorders/drug therapy , Female , Humans , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/drug therapy , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors/therapeutic use , Space Perception/physiology
9.
Psychol Med ; 27(6): 1277-85, 1997 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9403899

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: While neuropsychological studies have consistently reported impaired cognition in elderly patients with unipolar depression, studies of cognitive function in younger patients with depression have produced equivocal results. The aim of this study was to examine the presence and nature of cognitive deficits in young patients with depression. METHODS: Neuropsychological function was assessed in 20 young patients with unipolar depression, in comparison to 20 age-, education- and IQ- matched controls. Subtests from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) were employed, as this battery has proved sensitive to deficits in middle-aged and elderly patients with depression. RESULTS: The patients were not impaired for short-term memory capacity, spatial working memory, planning ability, cognitive speed, delayed matching to sample or recognition memory. Compared to controls, the patients showed impaired subsequent movement latencies on the Tower of London task, suggesting deficits in the ability to sustain motor responses in depression. The depression group were also impaired on the task of attentional set shifting, requiring more trials to criterion at the intradimensional stage of the task and being more likely to fail the task at the extradimensional shift stage than controls. Further analysis indicated that half of the depression group failed to complete all stages of the set shifting task. These patients were more likely to have required in-patient hospitalization at some time during their illness. CONCLUSIONS: These results indicate that there are specific cognitive deficits in young patients with depression and that their presence may be related to a history of hospitalization.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Depressive Disorder/diagnosis , Neuropsychological Tests , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Analysis of Variance , Attention , Depressive Disorder/psychology , Female , Form Perception , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Male , Memory, Short-Term , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Problem Solving , Psychomotor Performance , Severity of Illness Index , Space Perception
10.
Aust Fam Physician ; 25(10): 1569-73, 1996 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8936738

ABSTRACT

Post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) occurs after a person has been exposed to a traumatic event involving actual or threatened death, and has responded with intense fear or helplessness. The event is then persistently re-experienced. The person avoids stimuli associated with the trauma and experiences a numbing of general responsiveness. Symptoms of increased arousal can occur as well as depression and anxiety. PTSD causes clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning. The general practitioner is uniquely placed to identify PTSD and can have a key role in treatment. Cognitive behavioural treatment is a central therapeutic approach and can be carried out in general practice. The issues are to counteract the physiological components, expose the patient to the feared situation and help the patient to relearn that the stimuli are not necessarily associated with danger or threat. Repeated brief consultations over time can facilitate this process.


Subject(s)
Family Practice , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic , Antidepressive Agents/therapeutic use , Behavior Therapy/methods , Humans , Prognosis , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/complications , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/therapy
11.
Behav Res Ther ; 34(1): 85-95, 1996 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8561768

ABSTRACT

The Padua Inventory (PI) is a measure of obsessive-compulsive phenomena, which is used in clinical and research settings. One reason for the PI's growing acceptance is the emergence of a good deal of evidence supporting the adequacy of its reliability, convergent validity, and evidence supporting the stability of its factor structure. However, there have been some concerns about its discriminant validity. The use of the PI in Australia has been limited by the lack of local normative data and information about its psychometric properties. The cross-national validation of the PI has both theoretical and practical implications, and could add further evidence for its adequacy as a measure of obsessive-compulsive phenomena. Results from the present study found that the PI exhibited a similar but not identical factor structure compared to previous studies, consistently good reliability, adequate convergent validity, and differences in normative data compared with previous studies. Overall, while one may not necessarily assume the generalisability of normative data across different cultural contexts, there is a good deal of consistency in the psychometric properties of the PI. However, there is a need to further demonstrate the PI's discriminant validity, particularly for the "Urges and Worries" subscale.


Subject(s)
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/diagnosis , Psychometrics , Adolescent , Adult , Australia , Female , Humans , Male , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/psychology , Reproducibility of Results
12.
Pediatrics ; 85(4): 559-66, 1990 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2314969

ABSTRACT

We used the Toddler Temperament Scale with large representative samples of younger (mean age = 20.5 months; N = 1188) and older (mean age = 35.4 months; N = 1360) Australian toddlers. There were significant sex differences on 6 of the temperament dimensions for the young group, and on 5 of the 9 dimensions for the older group. Older boys were also more likely to be categorized clinically as having a "difficult" temperament and less likely to have an "easy" temperament. Each group was divided into quartiles according to socioeconomic status. For the younger toddlers there were significant differences in 3 of 9 temperament dimensions, and for the older group there were significant differences in 7 of 9 dimensions. Groups with higher socioeconomic status had temperament ratings which were more likely to make them easier to manage, and to be categorized clinically as having an easy temperament, but toddlers with low socioeconomic status were more likely to have a difficult temperament. There were significant differences in temperament dimension scores between Australian toddlers and those studied in an American setting. These results indicate that toddler temperament ratings differ according to age, sex, social class, and cultural context. Great caution needs to be taken in interpreting individual temperament profiles utilizing comparison data obtained from different sociocultural settings. Future temperament "norms" may need to specify characteristics of the group of children from which they were derived to allow more valid comparisons.


Subject(s)
Personality Development , Personality Tests , Personality , Temperament , Age Factors , Child, Preschool , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Sex Factors , Socioeconomic Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires
13.
Int J Psychol ; 24(1-5): 585-603, 1989 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23336797

ABSTRACT

Three infant cohorts from Greek cultural backgrounds were compared with an Anglo-Australian infant cohort across 9 dimensions of temperament using Australian and Greek versions of the revised Infant Temperament Questionnaire (Cazey and McDevitt 1978). Infants from a Greek cultural background were generally considered to be more 'difficult' than the Anglo-Australian infants on the temperament dimensions of Approach, Adaptability, Mood and Distractibility even after the effects of social class were controlled statistically. The reported cross-cultural differences were discussed with regard to the potential influence on temperament ratings of ethnicity and culture, social status, psychobiological factors, migrant status, social assimilation, and questionnaire characteristics.

14.
Aust Paediatr J ; 24(6): 343-5, 1988 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3242481

ABSTRACT

A revised validated version of the Toddler Temperament Scale (TTS) was used to develop normative data for temperament in Australian toddlers. Results confirm the accepted practice of dividing the group into younger and older toddlers, with normative data reported for those younger and older than 24 months. The TTS can be used above the normal cut-off age of 36 months. Care must be taken with interpretation of temperament scores, especially when comparing children of different backgrounds.


Subject(s)
Personality Development , Personality , Temperament , Australia , Child, Preschool , Humans , Infant , Personality Tests
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...