Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 16 de 16
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
PLoS One ; 16(12): e0260863, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34882734

ABSTRACT

Numerous empirical and modeling studies have been done to find a relationship between postural stability and the susceptibility to motion sickness (MS). However, while the demonstration of a causal relationship between postural stability and the susceptibility to MS is still lacking, recent studies suggest that motion sick individuals have genuine deficits in selecting and reweighting multimodal sensory information. Here we investigate how the adaptation to changing postural situations develops and how the dynamics in multisensory integration is modulated on an individual basis along with MS susceptibility. We used a postural task in which participants stood on a posturographic platform with either eyes open (EO) or eyes closed (EC) during three minutes. The platform was static during the first minute (baseline phase), oscillated harmonically during the second minute (perturbation phase) and returned to its steady state for the third minute (return phase). Principal component (PC) analysis was applied to the sequence of short-term power density spectra of the antero-posterior position of the center of pressure. Results showed that the less motion-sick a participant is, the more similar is his balance between high and low frequencies for EO and EC conditions (as calculated from the eigenvector of the first PC). By fitting exponential decay models to the first PC score in the return phase, we estimated, for each participant in each condition, the sluggishness to return to the baseline spectrum. We showed that the de-adaptation following platform oscillation depends on the susceptibility to MS. These results suggest that non motion-sick participants finely adjust their spectrum in the perturbation phase (i.e. reweighting) and therefore take longer to return to their initial postural control particularly with eyes closed. Thus, people have idiosyncratic ways of doing sensory reweighting for postural control, these processes being tied to MS susceptibility.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological , Feedback, Sensory/physiology , Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Postural Balance , Proprioception , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
2.
Motor Control ; 25(2): 182-197, 2021 Jan 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33421961

ABSTRACT

The aim of this experiment was to investigate the postural response to specific types of long-term memory (episodic vs. semantic) in young adults performing an unperturbed upright stance. Although a similar level of steadiness (mean distance) was observed, dual tasking induced a higher velocity, more energy in the higher frequency range (power spectral density), and less regularity (sample entropy) compared with a simple postural task. Moreover, mean velocity was always greater in the semantic than in the episodic task. The differences in postural control during dual tasking may result from the types of processes involved in the memory task. Findings suggest a spatial process sharing between posture and episodic memory.


Subject(s)
Memory, Long-Term/physiology , Postural Balance/physiology , Adult , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male , Semantics , Young Adult
3.
BMC Neurosci ; 22(1): 1, 2021 01 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33397293

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: A common problem in neurophysiological signal processing is the extraction of meaningful information from high dimension, low sample size data (HDLSS). We present RoLDSIS (regression on low-dimension spanned input space), a regression technique based on dimensionality reduction that constrains the solution to the subspace spanned by the available observations. This avoids regularization parameters in the regression procedure, as needed in shrinkage regression methods. RESULTS: We applied RoLDSIS to the EEG data collected in a phonemic identification experiment. In the experiment, morphed syllables in the continuum /da/-/ta/ were presented as acoustic stimuli to the participants and the event-related potentials (ERP) were recorded and then represented as a set of features in the time-frequency domain via the discrete wavelet transform. Each set of stimuli was chosen from a preliminary identification task executed by the participant. Physical and psychophysical attributes were associated to each stimulus. RoLDSIS was then used to infer the neurophysiological axes, in the feature space, associated with each attribute. We show that these axes can be reliably estimated and that their separation is correlated with the individual strength of phonemic categorization. The results provided by RoLDSIS are interpretable in the time-frequency domain and may be used to infer the neurophysiological correlates of phonemic categorization. A comparison with commonly used regularized regression techniques was carried out by cross-validation. CONCLUSION: The prediction errors obtained by RoLDSIS are comparable to those obtained with Ridge Regression and smaller than those obtained with LASSO and SPLS. However, RoLDSIS achieves this without the need for cross-validation, a procedure that requires the extraction of a large amount of observations from the data and, consequently, a decreased signal-to-noise ratio when averaging trials. We show that, even though RoLDSIS is a simple technique, it is suitable for the processing and interpretation of neurophysiological signals.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Models, Theoretical , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Humans , Sample Size
4.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 145(4): 2561, 2019 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31046350

ABSTRACT

Simulations of waveguide acoustics require a description of the boundary condition at the open end. For problems involving higher order transverse modes, it is often described by a multimodal radiation impedance matrix. Expressions for the computation of this matrix for an infinite flange condition are available only for circular and rectangular cross-sectional shapes. Thus, a general expression valid for arbitrary cross-sectional shapes is of interest. Such an expression is proposed, validated against known cases, and applied to an arbitrary cross-section shape. The solution is shown to be computationally efficient.

5.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27883290

ABSTRACT

The effect of normal aging on lexical production and semantic processing was evaluated in 72 healthy participants. Four tasks were used, picture naming (PN), picture categorization (PC), numerical judgment (NJ), and color judgment (CJ). The dependence of reaction time (RT) and correct responses with age was accounted by mixed-effects models. Participants underwent neuropsychological testing for verbal, executive, and memory functions. The RTs increase significantly with age for all tasks. After parceling out the non-specific cognitive decline, as reflected by the NJ task, the RT for the PN task decreases with age. Behavioral data were interpreted in relation with neuropsychological scores. Our results suggest that (a) naming becomes more automatic and semantic processing slightly more difficult with age, and (b) a non-specific general slowdown of cognitive processing occurs with age. Lexical production remained unaltered, based on compensatory automatic processes. This study also suggests a possible slowdown of semantic processing, even in normal aging.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Semantics , Speech , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Judgment , Linear Models , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Psycholinguistics , Reaction Time , Visual Perception
6.
PLoS One ; 12(10): e0186431, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29036180

ABSTRACT

The prediction of object stability on earth requires the establishment of a perceptual frame of reference based on the direction of gravity. Across three experiments, we measured the critical angle (CA) at which an object appeared equally likely to fall over or right itself. We investigated whether the internal representation of the gravity direction, biased by either simulated tilt (rotating visual surround) or real body tilt situations, influences in a similar fashion the judgment of stability. In the simulated tilt condition, the estimated CA and the perceived gravity are both deviated in the same direction. In the real tilt condition, the orientation of the body affects the perception of gravity direction but has no effect on the estimated CA. Results suggest that people differently weigh gravity direction information provided by visual motion and by visual polarity cues for estimating the stability of objects.


Subject(s)
Gravity Sensing , Visual Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Motion , Orientation , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
7.
Proc Biol Sci ; 284(1862)2017 Sep 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28878060

ABSTRACT

Human adults show an attentional bias towards fearful faces, an adaptive behaviour that relies on amygdala function. This attentional bias emerges in infancy between 5 and 7 months, but the underlying developmental mechanism is unknown. To examine possible precursors, we investigated whether 3.5-, 6- and 12-month-old infants show facilitated detection of fearful faces in noise, compared to happy faces. Happy or fearful faces, mixed with noise, were presented to infants (N = 192), paired with pure noise. We applied multivariate pattern analyses to several measures of infant looking behaviour to derive a criterion-free, continuous measure of face detection evidence in each trial. Analyses of the resulting psychometric curves supported the hypothesis of a detection advantage for fearful faces compared to happy faces, from 3.5 months of age and across all age groups. Overall, our data show a readiness to detect fearful faces (compared to happy faces) in younger infants that developmentally precedes the previously documented attentional bias to fearful faces in older infants and adults.


Subject(s)
Attention , Facial Expression , Fear , Happiness , Face , Humans , Infant
8.
PLoS One ; 10(12): e0144466, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26657203

ABSTRACT

Motion sickness (MS) usually occurs for a narrow band of frequencies of the imposed oscillation. It happens that this frequency band is close to that which are spontaneously produced by postural sway during natural stance. This study examined the relationship between reported susceptibility to motion sickness and postural control. The hypothesis is that the level of MS can be inferred from the shape of the Power Spectral Density (PSD) profile of spontaneous sway, as measured by the displacement of the center of mass during stationary, upright stance. In Experiment 1, postural fluctuations while standing quietly were related to MS history for inertial motion. In Experiment 2, postural stability measures registered before the onset of a visual roll movement were related to MS symptoms following the visual stimulation. Study of spectral characteristics in postural control showed differences in the distribution of energy along the power spectrum of the antero-posterior sway signal. Participants with MS history provoked by exposure to inertial motion showed a stronger contribution of the high frequency components of the sway signal. When MS was visually triggered, sick participants showed more postural sway in the low frequency range. The results suggest that subject-specific PSD details may be a predictor of the MS level. Furthermore, the analysis of the sway frequency spectrum provided insight into the intersubject differences in the use of postural control subsystems. The relationship observed between MS susceptibility and spontaneous posture is discussed in terms of postural sensory weighting and in relation to the nature of the provocative stimulus.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Motion Sickness/physiopathology , Postural Balance/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Adult , Cues , Disease Susceptibility , Female , Humans , Male , Motion , Photic Stimulation , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
9.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 137(2): 832-43, 2015 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25698017

ABSTRACT

In this paper, a multimodal theory accounting for higher order acoustical propagation modes is presented as an extension to the classical plane wave theory. This theoretical development is validated against experiments on vocal tract replicas, obtained using a 3D printer and finite element simulations. Simplified vocal tract geometries of increasing complexity are used to investigate the influence of some geometrical parameters on the acoustical properties of the vocal tract. It is shown that the higher order modes can produce additional resonances and anti-resonances and can also strongly affect the radiated sound. These effects appear to be dependent on the eccentricity and the cross-sectional shape of the geometries. Finally, the comparison between the simulations and the experiments points out the importance of taking visco-thermal losses into account to increase the accuracy of the resonance bandwidths prediction.

10.
Neuropsychologia ; 49(3): 498-506, 2011 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21126529

ABSTRACT

While the mechanisms of short-term adaptation to prism-altered apparent visual direction have been widely investigated, the processes underlying adaptation to prism-altered perceived distance are less well known. This study used a hand-pointing paradigm and exposure with base-out prisms to evaluate the relative contributions of sensory (visual and proprioceptive) and motor components of adaptation to perceived-distance alteration. A main experiment was designed to elicit adaptation at the sensory and motor levels, by giving subjects altered visual feedback. A control experiment without visual feedback allowed the effects of eye muscle potentiation (EMP) induced by sustained fixation through the prisms to be uncovered. In the main experiment, the aftereffects were partitioned into two-thirds visual and one-third motor, with no significant proprioceptive component. These results differ from the classical pattern of short-term adaptation to prism-altered apparent visual direction, which includes mainly proprioceptive/motor adaptive components, with a smaller visual component. This difference can be attributed to differences in accuracy between proprioception and vision for localization in depth or in lateral directions. In addition, a comparison of the visual aftereffects in the main and control experiments revealed two sub-components with equal contributions: a recalibration of the mapping between the vergence signal and perceived distance, and an EMP-related aftereffect. These findings indicate that "visual" adaptation actually involves a multiplicity of processes.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Perceptual Distortion/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Distance Perception/physiology , Female , Figural Aftereffect/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Oculomotor Muscles/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Proprioception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Young Adult
11.
Exp Brain Res ; 202(4): 825-36, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20198365

ABSTRACT

Telestereoscopic viewing provides a method to distort egocentric distance perception by artificially increasing the interpupillary distance. Adaptation to such a visual rearrangement is little understood. Two experiments were performed in order to dissociate the effects of a sustained increased vergence demand, from those of an active calibration of the vergence/distance mapping. Egocentric distances were assessed within reaching space through open-loop pointing to small targets in the dark. During the exposure condition of the first experiment, subjects were instructed to point to the targets without feedback, whereas in the second experiment, hand visual feedback was available, resulting in a modified relationship between vergence-specified distance and reach distance. The visual component of adaptation in the second experiment was assessed on the unexposed hand. In the post-tests of both experiments, subjects exhibited a constant distance overestimation across all targets, with a more than twice larger aftereffect in the second one. These findings suggest two different processes: (1) an alteration in the vergence effort following sustained increased vergence; (2) a calibration of the vergence/distance mapping uncovering the visual component of adaptation.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Hand , Motor Activity , Space Perception , Visual Perception , Adult , Body Image , Darkness , Feedback, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance , Psychophysics , Task Performance and Analysis , Young Adult
12.
J Neurophysiol ; 102(1): 523-31, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19420122

ABSTRACT

Speech production involves some of the most precise and finely timed patterns of human movement. Here, in the context of jaw movement in speech, we show that spatial precision in speech production is systematically associated with the regulation of impedance and in particular, with jaw stiffness--a measure of resistance to displacement. We estimated stiffness and also variability during movement using a robotic device to apply brief force pulses to the jaw. Estimates of stiffness were obtained using the perturbed position and force trajectory and an estimate of what the trajectory would be in the absence of load. We estimated this "reference trajectory" using a new technique based on Fourier analysis. A moving-average (MA) procedure was used to estimate stiffness by modeling restoring force as the moving average of previous jaw displacements. The stiffness matrix was obtained from the steady state of the MA model. We applied this technique to data from 31 subjects whose jaw movements were perturbed during speech utterances and kinematically matched nonspeech movements. We observed systematic differences in stiffness over the course of jaw-lowering and jaw-raising movements that were correlated with measures of kinematic variability. Jaw stiffness was high and variability was low early and late in the movement when the jaw was elevated. Stiffness was low and variability was high in the middle of movement when the jaw was lowered. Similar patterns were observed for speech and nonspeech conditions. The systematic relationship between stiffness and variability points to the idea that stiffness regulation is integral to the control of orofacial movement variability.


Subject(s)
Jaw/physiology , Mouth/physiology , Movement , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Male , Models, Biological , Robotics/methods , Speech , Young Adult
13.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 51(6): 1507-21, 2008 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18695015

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: This article investigates jaw-finger coordination in a task involving pointing to a target while naming it with a CVCV (e.g., /papa/) versus CVCV (e.g., /papa/) word. According to the authors' working hypothesis, the pointing apex (gesture extremum) would be synchronized with the apex of the jaw-opening gesture corresponding to the stressed syllable. METHOD: Jaw and finger motions were recorded using Optotrak (Northern Digital, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada). The effects of stress position on jaw-finger coordination were tested across different target positions (near vs. far) and different consonants in the target word (/t/ vs. /p/). Twenty native Portuguese Brazilian speakers participated in the experiment (all conditions). RESULTS: Jaw response starts earlier, and finger-target alignment period is longer for CVCV words than for CVCV ones. The apex of the jaw-opening gesture for the stressed syllable appears synchronized with the onset of the finger-target alignment period (corresponding to the pointing apex) for CVCV words and with the offset of that period for CVCV words. CONCLUSIONS: For both stress conditions, the stressed syllable occurs within the finger-target alignment period because of tight finger-jaw coordination. This result is interpreted as evidence for an anchoring of the speech deictic site (part of speech that shows) in the pointing gesture.


Subject(s)
Fingers/physiology , Jaw/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Speech/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Phonetics , Time Factors
14.
Soc Neurosci ; 3(1): 26-36, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18633844

ABSTRACT

Ideomotor movements may arise in observers while they watch other people's actions. Previous studies have shown that ideomotor movements are guided by both perceptual and intentional characteristics of the actions being observed (perceptual induction and intentional induction, respectively; cf. Knuf, Aschersleben, & Prinz, 2001; de Maeght & Prinz, 2004). In the present study we explore the functional basis of intentional induction. More specifically we raise the issue of whose intentions count for intentional induction: observers' own intentions or observees' (implied) intentions? We studied ideomotor movements in a cooperative and a competitive task setting. In the cooperative setting observers' and observees' intentions were identical, but in the competitive setting they were different. Results indicate that ideomotor movements are guided by the observers' own intentions, not the observees' implied intentions. Our findings suggest that, though observers understand the intentions of others, their ideomotor movements are guided by their own intentions, expressing what they themselves wish to see the other is doing.


Subject(s)
Competitive Behavior/physiology , Cooperative Behavior , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods
15.
Biol Cybern ; 91(1): 23-36, 2004 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15309545

ABSTRACT

Opening a door, turning a steering wheel, and rotating a coffee mill are typical examples of human movements that are constrained by the physical environment. The constraints decrease the mobility of the human arm and lead to redundancy in the distribution of actuator forces (either joint torques or muscle forces). Due to this actuator redundancy, there is an infinite number of ways to form a specific arm trajectory. However, humans form trajectories in a unique way. How do humans resolve the redundancy of the constrained motions and specify the hand trajectory? To investigate this problem, we examine human arm movements in a crank-rotation task. To explain the trajectory formation in constrained point-to-point motions, we propose a combined criterion minimizing the hand contact force change and the actuating force change over the course of movement. Our experiments show a close matching between predicted and experimental data.


Subject(s)
Arm/physiology , Models, Neurological , Motor Activity/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Electromyography , Humans , Male , Sensitivity and Specificity , Torque
16.
J Neurophysiol ; 88(5): 2329-40, 2002 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12424274

ABSTRACT

Humans produce speech by controlling a complex biomechanical apparatus to achieve desired speech sounds. We show here that kinematic variability in speech may be influenced by patterns of jaw stiffness. A robotic device was used to deliver mechanical perturbations to the jaw to quantify its stiffness in the mid-sagittal plane. Measured jaw stiffness was anisotropic. Stiffness was greatest along a protrusion-retraction axis and least in the direction of jaw raising and lowering. Consistent with the idea that speech movements reflect directional asymmetries in jaw stiffness, kinematic variability during speech production was found to be high in directions in which stiffness is low and vice versa. In addition, for higher jaw elevations, stiffness was greater and kinematic variability was less. The observed patterns of kinematic variability were not specific to speech-similar patterns appeared in speech and nonspeech movements. The empirical patterns of stiffness were replicated by using a physiologically based model of the jaw. The simulation studies support the idea that the pattern of jaw stiffness is affected by musculo-skeletal geometry and muscle-force-generating abilities with jaw geometry being the primary determinant of the orientation of the stiffness ellipse.


Subject(s)
Jaw/physiology , Speech/physiology , Adult , Algorithms , Anisotropy , Biomechanical Phenomena , Computer Simulation , Feedback/physiology , Female , Humans , Jaw/anatomy & histology , Linear Models , Male , Models, Neurological , Muscle, Skeletal/innervation , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Myography , Robotics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...