Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 10 de 10
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Biol Psychol ; 176: 108468, 2023 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36481265

ABSTRACT

Previous research has shown greater risk aversion when people make choices about lives than cash. We tested the hypothesis that compared to placebo, exogenous testosterone administration would lead to riskier choices about cash than lives, given testosterone's association with financial risk-taking and reward sensitivity. A double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized trial was conducted to test this hypothesis (Clinical Trials Registry: NCT02734238, www.clinicaltrials.gov). We collected functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from 50 non-obese males before and shortly after 28 days of severe exercise-and-diet-induced energy deficit, during which testosterone (200 mg testosterone enanthate per week in sesame oil) or placebo (sesame seed oil only) was administered. Because we expected circulating testosterone levels to be reduced due to severe energy deficit, testosterone administration served a restorative function to mitigate the impact of energy deficit on testosterone levels. The fMRI task involved making choices under uncertainty for lives and cash. We also manipulated whether the outcomes were presented as gains or losses. Consistent with prospect theory, we observed the reflection effect such that participants were more risk averse when outcomes were presented as gains than losses. Brain activation in the thalamus covaried with individual differences in exhibiting the reflection effect. Testosterone did not impact choice, but it increased sensitivity to negative feedback following risky choices. These results suggest that exogenous testosterone administration in the context of energy deficit can impact some aspects of risky choice, and that individual differences in the reflection effect engage a brain structure involved in processing emotion, reward and risk.


Subject(s)
Gambling , Risk-Taking , Male , Humans , Testosterone , Gambling/psychology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Brain , Reward , Decision Making/physiology
2.
Neurorehabil Neural Repair ; 37(1): 66-75, 2023 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36575955

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Several measures of upper limb (UL) motor tasks have been developed to characterize recovery. However, UL performance and movement quality measures in isolation may not provide a true profile of functional recovery. OBJECTIVE: To investigate the measurement properties of a new trunk-based Index of Performance (IPt) of the UL combining endpoint performance (accuracy and speed) and movement quality (trunk displacement) in stroke. METHODS: Participants with stroke (n = 25, mean time since stroke: 18.7 ± 17.2 months) performed a reaching task over 3 evaluation sessions. The IPt was computed based on Fitts' Law that incorporated endpoint accuracy and speed corrected by the amount of trunk displacement. Test-retest reliability was analyzed using intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) and Bland-Altman plots. Standard error of measurement (SEM) and Minimal Detectable Change (MDC) were determined. Validity was investigated through the relationship between IPt, Fugl-Meyer Assessment (FMA-UE), and Action Research Arm Test (ARAT), as well as the ability of IPt to distinguish between levels of UL motor impairment severity. RESULTS: Test-retest reliability was excellent (ICC = .908, 95% CI: 0.807-0.96). Bland-Altman did not show systematic differences. SEM and MDC95 were 14% and 39%, respectively. Construct validity was satisfactory. The IPt showed low-to-moderate relationships with FMA-UE (R2 ranged from .236 to .428) and ARAT (R2 ranged from .277 to .306). IPt scores distinguished between different levels of UL severity. CONCLUSIONS: The IPt showed evidence of good reliability, and initial validity. The IPt may be a promising tool for research and clinical settings. Further research is warranted to investigate its validity with additional comparator instruments.


Subject(s)
Stroke Rehabilitation , Stroke , Humans , Psychometrics , Recovery of Function , Reproducibility of Results , Stroke/diagnosis , Upper Extremity , Torso
3.
Neuropsychologia ; 174: 108350, 2022 09 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35988804

ABSTRACT

Multitasking is a common requirement in many occupations. Considerable research has demonstrated that performance declines as a result of multitasking, and that it engages multiple brain regions. Despite growing evidence suggesting that brain regions operate as networks, minimal research has investigated the cognitive brain networks implicated in multitasking. The Multi-Attribute Task Battery II (MATB) is a common method for assessing multitasking ability that simulates a pilot's operational environment inside an aircraft cockpit. The aim of the present study was to examine multitasking performance on the MATB, and the associated neural patterns underlying performance with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Twenty-four participants completed the MATB in the fMRI scanner. Participants completed four runs of the MATB in a 2 (Task: multitasking vs. single tasking) × 2 (Difficulty: hard vs. easy) design. MATB performance was measured as a function of accuracy. We analyzed the fMRI brain scans using both static and dynamic functional connectivity to determine whether there were differences in the connectivity patterns associated with each of the four conditions. A significant interaction between Task and Difficulty was observed such that multitasking performance accuracy, which was derived from the average across tasks, was lower than single tasking in the hard, but not easy, condition. The fMRI data revealed that static and dynamic functional connectivity between the default mode and dorsal attention networks was stronger during multitasking relative to single tasking. The static functional connectivity between the default mode and left frontoparietal networks, along with the dynamic functional connectivity between the dorsal attention and left frontoparietal networks, were both more anti-correlated during multitasking relative to single tasking. Taken together, the static and dynamic functional connectivity analyses provide complementary information to reveal the interactions among cognitive networks that support multitasking performance. Targeting these networks may offer a path to enhance multitasking ability through the application of neurostimulation and neuroenhancement techniques.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Brain , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Nerve Net/physiology , Neural Networks, Computer , Neural Pathways/physiology
5.
Cogn Neuropsychol ; 38(6): 365-386, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35274593

ABSTRACT

Traditionally, it has been assumed that logical thinking requires deliberation. However, people can also make logical responses quickly, exhibiting logical intuitions. We examined the neural correlates of logical intuitions by administering base rate problems during fMRI scanning using a two-response paradigm where participants first responded quickly and then reflectively to problems that did or did not pit a normative response against an intuitively-cued stereotypical response (i.e., conflict vs. non-conflict problems). As predicted, participants were less likely to make judgments in accordance with base rates on conflict problems. Critically, in only 4% of cases did longer deliberation change an initially biased response to a normatively correct response. The fMRI data revealed that intuitively-made initial biased judgments nevertheless activate regions typically involved in cognitive control, executive functions and attention, including anterior, inferior, middle and superior frontal cortex, suggesting that even when errors are made, there might be very early awareness of conflict.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Thinking , Cues , Frontal Lobe , Humans , Thinking/physiology
6.
Neuroimage ; 223: 117325, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32882380

ABSTRACT

Divergent thinking tests have been used extensively in neuroscientific studies of creativity. However, output from tests of divergent thinking can be scored in different ways, and those scores can influence assessments of divergent thinking performance and its relationship with brain activation. Here we sought to investigate the relationship between various methods of scoring the Alternate Uses Task (AUT)-a well-known test of divergent thinking-and regional grey matter volume (GMV) using voxel-based morphometry (VBM). We assessed AUT performance based on (a) traditional approaches that involve scoring participants' output on fluency, flexibility, originality, and elaboration, (b) a subjective approach that involves scoring output directly on "snapshot" creativity, and (c) the definitional approach that involves scoring output separately on novelty and usefulness-the two criteria deemed necessary and jointly sufficient to categorize an idea as creative. Correcting for age, sex, intracranial volume, verbal IQ and working memory capacity, we found negative correlations between regional GMV in the left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG) and novelty and usefulness scores, but no correlation involving other scoring approaches. As part of the brain's core semantic system, this region is involved in concept retrieval and integration. We discuss the implications of these findings for our understanding of the neural bases of divergent thinking, and how ITG could be related to the generation of novel and useful responses.


Subject(s)
Brain/anatomy & histology , Neuropsychological Tests , Thinking/physiology , Adult , Creativity , Female , Gray Matter/anatomy & histology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
7.
Can J Neurol Sci ; 47(6): 775-784, 2020 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32493533

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Damage to the corticospinal tract (CST) from stroke leads to motor deficits. The damage can be quantified as the amount of overlap between the stroke lesion and CST (CST Injury). Previous literature has shown that the degree of motor deficits post-stroke is related to the amount of CST Injury. These studies delineate the stroke lesion from structural T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scans, often acquired for research. In Canada, computed tomography (CT) is the most common imaging modality used in routine acute stroke care. In this proof-of-principle study, we determine whether CST Injury, using lesions delineated from CT scans, significantly explains the variability in motor impairment in individuals with stroke. METHODS: Thirty-seven participants with stroke were included in this study. These individuals had a CT scan within the acute stage (7 days) of their stroke and underwent motor assessments. Brain images from CT scans were registered to MRI space. We performed a stepwise regression analysis to determine the contribution of CST injury and demographic variables in explaining motor impairment variability. RESULTS: Using clinically available CT scans, we found modest evidence that CST Injury explains variability in motor impairment (R2adj = 0.12, p = 0.02). None of the participant demographic variables entered the model. CONCLUSION: We show for the first time a relationship between CST Injury and motor impairment using CT scans. Further work is required to evaluate the utility of data derived from clinical CT scans as a biomarker of stroke motor recovery.


Subject(s)
Pyramidal Tracts , Stroke , Brain , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Pyramidal Tracts/diagnostic imaging , Stroke/complications , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
8.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 9480, 2018 06 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29930399

ABSTRACT

Biomarkers that represent the structural and functional integrity of the motor system enable us to better assess motor outcome post-stroke. The degree of overlap between the stroke lesion and corticospinal tract (CST Injury) is a measure of the structural integrity of the motor system, whereas the left-to-right motor cortex resting state connectivity (LM1-RM1 rs-connectivity) is a measure of its functional integrity. CST Injury and LM1-RM1 rs-connectivity each individually correlate with motor outcome post-stroke, but less is understood about the relationship between these biomarkers. Thus, this study investigates the relationship between CST Injury and LM1-RM1 rs-connectivity, individually and together, with motor outcome. Twenty-seven participants with upper limb motor deficits post-stroke completed motor assessments and underwent MRI at one time point. CST Injury and LM1-RM1 rs-connectivity were derived from T1-weighted and resting state functional MRI scans, respectively. We performed hierarchical multiple regression analyses to determine the contribution of each biomarker in explaining motor outcome. The interaction between CST Injury and LM1-RM1 rs-connectivity does not significantly contribute to the variability in motor outcome. However, inclusion of both CST Injury and LM1-RM1 rs-connectivity explains more variability in motor outcome, than either alone. We suggest both biomarkers provide distinct information about an individual's motor outcome.


Subject(s)
Motor Cortex/physiopathology , Motor Skills , Pyramidal Tracts/physiopathology , Stroke/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Connectome , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Pyramidal Tracts/diagnostic imaging , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Upper Extremity/innervation , Upper Extremity/physiopathology
9.
J Neurol Sci ; 384: 21-29, 2018 Jan 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29249372

ABSTRACT

Movement is traditionally viewed as a process that involves motor brain regions. However, movement also implicates non-motor regions such as prefrontal and parietal cortex, regions whose integrity may thus be important for motor recovery after stroke. Importantly, focal brain damage can affect neural functioning within and between distinct brain networks implicated in the damage. The aim of this study is to investigate how resting state connectivity (rs-connectivity) within and between motor and frontoparietal networks are affected post-stroke in correlation with motor outcome. Twenty-seven participants with chronic stroke with unilateral upper limb deficits underwent motor assessments and magnetic resonance imaging. Participants completed the Chedoke-McMaster Stroke Assessment as a measure of arm (CMSA-Arm) and hand (CMSA-Hand) impairment and the Action Research Arm Test (ARAT) as a measure of motor function. We used a seed-based rs-connectivity approach defining the motor (seed=contralesional primary motor cortex (M1)) and frontoparietal (seed=contralesional dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)) networks. We analyzed the rs-connectivity within each network (intra-network connectivity) and between both networks (inter-network connectivity), and performed correlations between: a) intra-network connectivity and motor assessment scores; b) inter-network connectivity and motor assessment scores. We found: a) Participants with high rs-connectivity within the motor network (between M1 and supplementary motor area) have higher CMSA-Hand stage (z=3.62, p=0.003) and higher ARAT score (z=3.41, p=0.02). Rs-connectivity within the motor network was not significantly correlated with CMSA-Arm stage (z=1.83, p>0.05); b) Participants with high rs-connectivity within the frontoparietal network (between DLPFC and mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex) have higher CMSA-Hand stage (z=3.64, p=0.01). Rs-connectivity within the frontoparietal network was not significantly correlated with CMSA-Arm stage (z=0.93, p=0.03) or ARAT score (z=2.53, p=0.05); and c) Participants with high rs-connectivity between motor and frontoparietal networks have higher CMSA-Hand stage (rs=0.54, p=0.01) and higher ARAT score (rs=0.54, p=0.009). Rs-connectivity between the motor and frontoparietal networks was not significantly correlated with CMSA-Arm stage (rs=0.34, p=0.13). Taken together, the connectivity within and between the motor and frontoparietal networks correlate with motor outcome post-stroke. The integrity of these regions may be important for an individual's motor outcome. Motor-frontoparietal connectivity may be a potential biomarker of motor recovery post-stroke.


Subject(s)
Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Hand/physiopathology , Motor Cortex/physiopathology , Motor Skills , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Stroke/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Brain Mapping , Chronic Disease , Cross-Sectional Studies , Disability Evaluation , Female , Frontal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Motor Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Motor Skills/physiology , Neural Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Proof of Concept Study , Recovery of Function/physiology , Rest , Stroke/diagnostic imaging
10.
PLoS One ; 10(9): e0137704, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26368276

ABSTRACT

Embodied cognition holds that abstract concepts are grounded in perceptual-motor simulations. If a given embodied metaphor maps onto a spatial representation, then thinking of that concept should bias the allocation of attention. In this study, we used positive and negative self-esteem words to examine two properties of conceptual cueing. First, we tested the orientation-specificity hypothesis, which predicts that conceptual cues should selectively activate certain spatial axes (in this case, valenced self-esteem concepts should activate vertical space), instead of any spatial continuum. Second, we tested whether conceptual cueing requires semantic processing, or if it can be achieved with shallow visual processing of the cue words. Participants viewed centrally presented words consisting of high or low self-esteem traits (e.g., brave, timid) before detecting a target above or below the cue in the vertical condition, or on the left or right of the word in the horizontal condition. Participants were faster to detect targets when their location was compatible with the valence of the word cues, but only in the vertical condition. Moreover, this effect was observed when participants processed the semantics of the word, but not when processing its orthography. The results show that conceptual cueing by spatial metaphors is orientation-specific, and that an explicit consideration of the word cues' semantics is required for conceptual cueing to occur.


Subject(s)
Self Concept , Semantics , Shame , Speech Perception , Visual Perception , Adult , Humans , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...