Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 12 de 12
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
2.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 55(4): 277-84, 2001 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11768852

ABSTRACT

English-speaking (n = 32) and Chinese-speaking adults (n = 32) solved single-digit multiplication problems. In one condition, problems were presented as visual digits (e.g., 8 x 9). In the other condition, problems were presented as auditory number words in the participant's first language (e.g., /eit/ /taimz/ /nain/). Chinese-speaking adults made proportionately more operand-intrusion errors (e.g., 4 x 8 = 24) than English-speaking adults. Both groups made more operand-intrusion errors with auditory than with visual presentation. These findings are similar to those found when participants solve problems presented as visual number words (e.g., eight x nine), suggesting that in both cases the activation of phonological codes interferes with processing.


Subject(s)
Cross-Cultural Comparison , Language , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Problem Solving , Speech Perception , Adult , Attention , Female , Humans , Male
4.
Mem Cognit ; 27(5): 803-12, 1999 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10540809

ABSTRACT

Adults (N = 32) solved simple multiplication (e.g., 8 x 7) and corresponding division problems (e.g., 56/8). Self-reports of solution processes were given by half of the participants. Latency patterns and error rates were closely related across operations and were similar in self-report and no-report conditions. Solution of division problems, however, facilitated solution of multiplication problems more than the reverse. On large division problems, participants reported that they "recast" problems as multiplication (e.g., 56/8 as 8 x = 56). These results support the hypothesis that multiplication and division are stored in separate mental representations but that solution of difficult division problems sometimes involves access to multiplication.


Subject(s)
Association Learning , Cognition , Mathematics , Memory , Problem Solving , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Refractory Period, Psychological , Regression Analysis , Self Disclosure
5.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 50(4): 393-6, 1996 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9025329

ABSTRACT

Using multiplication facts, this experiment demonstrated an interference effect in the number-matching task. Here, subjects verified the presence of a target number (e.g., 8) in a previously presented cue (e.g., 5 x 8) that was masked after 60 ms. The SOAs between cue and target were 100, 120, 220, and 350 ms. Subjects were slower to reject targets that were the product of the cue (e.g., 40) than to reject unrelated targets (e.g., 42), but this was true only at the 100- and 120-ms SOAs (i.e., the interference effect). This pattern is consistent with the interference effect found by LeFevre and colleagues using addition facts. Furthermore, the present result supports the interpretation that the interference effect previously found with addition facts was due to obligatory activation and not to automatic counting.


Subject(s)
Discrimination Learning , Mental Recall , Problem Solving , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time
6.
Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord ; 18(8): 542-6, 1994 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7951474

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to investigate the relationships between adiposity and skeletal maturity, relative skeletal maturity and percentage of predicted adult stature. A representative sample of 6,029 Flemish girls aged 6-16 years of age was investigated. Age specific correlations between adiposity and biological maturity indicators were calculated and in each age group the fattest 5% were compared with the leanest 5%. Adiposity was estimated from the sum of five skinfolds (biceps, triceps, subscapular, suprailiac and medial calf). Skeletal age was assessed according to the Tanner-Whitehouse technique. Relative skeletal age was calculated as the difference between skeletal age and chronological age and percentage of predicted adult stature was calculated according to the Tanner-Whitehouse Mark II regression technique. Correlations between adiposity and maturity indicators are positive, but vary between r = 0.00 and r = 0.39. When stature is statistically controlled, correlations are reduced slightly. The 5% fattest girls are equally advanced (0.2 to 1.2 years) as the 5% leanest girls are delayed (0.0 to 0.9 years) in skeletal maturation. Attained statures are consistent with the maturity data and indicate that size differences between fat and lean girls are primarily due to maturity differences. It was concluded that during childhood and adolescence, fatness is associated with advanced and leanness with delayed biological maturity status. This association seems to have long term effects that merit further study.


Subject(s)
Adipose Tissue , Body Composition , Adolescent , Age Determination by Skeleton , Aging , Belgium , Body Height , Child , Female , Humans , Menarche , Skinfold Thickness
7.
Mem Cognit ; 22(2): 188-200, 1994 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8035695

ABSTRACT

In two experiments, we found evidence for individual differences in the obligatory activation of addition facts. Subjects were required to verify the presence of a target digit (e.g., 4) in a previously presented pair (e.g., 5+4). Subjects rejected targets that formed the sum of the initial pair (e.g., 5+4 and 9) more slowly than they rejected unrelated targets (e.g., 5+4 and 7). This interference of the sum was largest for subjects who were relatively skilled at multi-digit arithmetic. Less skilled subjects did not show statistically significant effects of obligatory activation. In comparison with less skilled subjects, skilled subjects showed differential interference on plus-one (e.g., 3+1) and standard (e.g., 2+3) problems when the plus sign was presented, and on ties (e.g., 2 2) when the plus sign was omitted. These results suggest that network models of arithmetic fact retrieval are appropriate for skilled subjects, but that alternative models need to be considered for less skilled individuals.


Subject(s)
Individuality , Mathematics , Memory , Problem Solving , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Neural Networks, Computer , Reaction Time , Task Performance and Analysis
8.
Physiol Behav ; 52(3): 603-8, 1992 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1409927

ABSTRACT

During aging, the lordosis reflex of female rats changes from a state of cyclic lordosis, when the lordosis reflex can be elicited by manual palpation only on proestrus, to a state of constant lordosis (CL), when it can be elicited daily. Social signals from other females altered this pattern of behavioral senescence. Group living decreased the lordosis reflex intensity in both old and young rats and delayed the onset of CL. Long-term group living delayed CL until late in the life span, while temporary group living delayed it only until rats returned to isolation. Long-term group living also changed the temporal relationship between CL and the acyclic, anovulatory state of estrus. Finally, the lordosis reflex intensity, measured by manual palpation, predicted the old rats' receptivity to a male, indicating that constant lordosis is a biomarker for other aspects of behavioral reproductive senescence.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Sexual Behavior, Animal/physiology , Social Behavior , Animals , Estrus/physiology , Female , Male , Posture/physiology , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Social Isolation/psychology
9.
Endocrinology ; 127(5): 2601-3, 1990 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2226335

ABSTRACT

Cultured murine lymphoid cells release a PRL-like immunoreactive (IR) protein which may be important in immunity, as anti-PRL antisera inhibit lymphocyte proliferation in vitro. We examined culture supernatants (SNs) and cell lysates from concanavalin A (Con A) activated murine thymocytes to identify these proteins. Western blot analysis of cell lysates revealed three specifically-stained PRL-IRs. A doublet of bands at 35.6 and 33.6 kDa was associated with the particulate fraction of the cell. These PRL-IRs were present in lymphocytes independently of mitogen stimulation. In contrast, a 22 kDa PRL-IR was only produced in mitogen stimulated cells, and was specifically immunoprecipitated with anti-PRL antiserum. In addition, all three PRL-like IRs incorporated 35S-methionine in vitro, indicating that they are synthesized by these cells. Only the 22 kDa PRL-like protein was present in culture medium from stimulated cells, suggesting that this may be the PRL bioactivity previously demonstrated in SNs from murine lymphocytes.


Subject(s)
Lymphocytes/metabolism , Prolactin/biosynthesis , Animals , Blotting, Western , Cells, Cultured , Mice , Precipitin Tests , Reference Values , Spleen/cytology , Spleen/metabolism , Thymus Gland/cytology , Thymus Gland/metabolism
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...