Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 20
Filter
1.
J Psychopathol Clin Sci ; 133(1): 76-89, 2024 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38059936

ABSTRACT

Dissociative symptoms, such as depersonalization and derealization, are experienced by about half of individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Theoretical models propose that acute dissociation is accompanied by specific behavioral, physiological, and experiential alterations and contributes to unfavorable PTSD symptom course. Yet, empirical evidence is scarce. Here, we explored associations between dissociative and behavioral, physiological, and experiential threat responses as well as effects of dissociative responding on PTSD symptom course. Individuals with PTSD (N = 71) participated in a preregistered script-driven imagery study including exposure to standardized, detail-enriched trauma, and neutral scripts. Stabilometry, eye-tracking, facial electromyography, autonomic psychophysiology, and self-report data were collected. Moreover, PTSD symptoms were assessed before and 3 months after testing. Analyses did not link acute dissociation to bodily and facial immobility or staring in response to trauma scripts. However, dissociation displayed an inverted U-shaped relationship with heart rate and was linked to higher nonspecific skin conductance fluctuation and higher high-frequency heart rate variability in response to trauma scripts. Moreover, acute dissociation was linked to higher self-reported negative affect responses to trauma scripts and displayed a U-shaped relationship with unfavorable PTSD symptom course. While results did not confirm hypothesized behavioral markers of dissociation, they do support defense-cascade model assumptions of an inverted U-shaped relationship between dissociation and psychophysiological arousal resulting from a progression of parasympathetic versus sympathetic dominance with increasing dissociation. On an experiential level, results did not confirm posttraumatic dissociation-induced emotional numbing, questioning theoretical notions. The observed nonlinear associations may help explain the heterogeneity of prior findings and might inform an updated conceptualization of posttraumatic dissociation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic , Humans , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Emotions , Dissociative Disorders/diagnosis , Dissociative Disorders/psychology , Autonomic Nervous System , Self Report
2.
Eur J Psychotraumatol ; 14(2): 2238492, 2023.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37593980

ABSTRACT

Background: The fifth version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) introduced the dissociative subtype of posttraumatic stress disorder (D-PTSD). To assess this subtype, the Dissociative Subtype of PTSD Scale (DSPS), a 15-item self-report measure to identify lifetime and current dissociative symptoms of D-PTSD, was developed. However, so far, the scale has only been validated in war veterans. Moreover, criterion validity and diagnostic utility have not been examined yet.Objective: We aimed to validate the DSPS in two samples of civilian trauma-exposed German-speaking participants.Methods: In Study 1, a pre-registered online study, participants with and without PTSD symptoms (N = 558) answered questionnaires about traumatic experiences, dissociation, PTSD, depression, generalized anxiety disorder, somatic symptom disorder, alcohol use disorder, absorption, and dissociative responding to trauma-related questionnaires. In Study 2, which used secondary data of a pre-registered clinical study, participants with a PTSD diagnosis (N = 71) answered questionnaires about traumatic experiences, dissociation, PTSD, depression, generalized anxiety disorder, somatic symptom disorder, and dissociative responding to standardized trauma exposure. Moreover, PTSD, D-PTSD, and other diagnoses were assessed with structured clinical interviews.Results: Analyses confirmed a three-factor structure as well as high internal consistency, and high convergent, discriminant, and criterion validity of the DSPS. Moreover, the scale was able to identify a latent D-PTSD group and individuals with D-PTSD diagnosis.Conclusions: The DSPS constitutes a reliable and valid tool to assess D-PTSD symptoms in clinical practice and research and thereby may contribute to a better understanding of these debilitating symptoms.


Many individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) suffer from dissociative symptoms which can be assessed with the Dissociative Subtype of PTSD Scale (DSPS; Wolf et al., 2017).The DSPS demonstrated good psychometric properties in two German-speaking trauma-exposed samples and hence might be used to assess D-PTSD symptoms in research and clinical practice.Complementing the original English version, a German version of the DSPS is provided in the Supplements.


Subject(s)
Medically Unexplained Symptoms , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic , Humans , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Psychometrics , Dissociative Disorders/diagnosis , Anxiety Disorders
3.
Behav Res Ther ; 164: 104289, 2023 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36934622

ABSTRACT

While research has elucidated processes underlying dissociative symptoms in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder, little is known about the circumstances under which trauma-related dissociation initially arises. To experimentally investigate causes and concomitants of peritraumatic dissociation, we subjected sixty-nine healthy women to aversive-audiovisual and painful-electrical stimulation in a 2(aversive/neutral film) x 2(pain/no pain) within-subject design while recording psychophysiological and fMRI-BOLD responses. Afterwards, participants rated negative-affect, pain, and dissociation for each condition. Using Bayesian multilevel regression models, we examined (1) whether aversive-audiovisual and painful-electrical stimulation elicit higher dissociation-levels than control conditions and (2) whether stronger negative-affect and pain responses (operationalized via self-report, psychophysiological, and neural markers) correlate with higher dissociation-levels. Several key findings emerged: Both aversive-audiovisual and painful-electrical stimulation elicited dissociation. Dissociation was linked to higher self-reported negative-affect, but we did not find enough evidence linking it to psychophysiological and neural negative-affect markers. However, dissociation was associated with higher levels of self-reported pain, a skin-conductance-response-based pain marker, and the fMRI-BOLD-based Neurologic-Pain-Signature. Results indicate that both aversive-audiovisual and painful stimuli can independently cause dissociation. Critically, pain responses captured via self-report, psychophysiological, and neural markers were consistently linked to higher dissociation-levels suggesting a specific, evolutionary meaningful, contribution of pain to the rise of dissociation.


Subject(s)
Pain , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic , Humans , Female , Bayes Theorem , Psychophysiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Affect , Dissociative Disorders
4.
5.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 143: 105819, 2022 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35724562

ABSTRACT

Intrusions, a key symptom of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), can occur in the form of images but also as pain sensations. Similar to audiovisual intrusions, the frequency and persistence of pain intrusions varies greatly between individuals. In the current study, we examined whether peritraumatic circulating 17ß-estradiol (E2) levels are a biologic factor associated with subsequent audiovisual (i.e., film) and pain intrusion development, and whether peritraumatic stress levels modulate this relationship. Forty-one free-cycling women participated in an ecologically informed trauma-pain-conditioning (TPC) paradigm, using trauma-films and pain as unconditioned stimuli. Independent variables were salivary peritraumatic E2 levels and stress indexed by salivary cortisol and self-reported state-anxiety during TPC. Outcomes were film- and pain-intrusions occurring during daily-life in the week following TPC and a Memory-Triggering-Task in response to conditioned stimuli 24 h after TPC. In the week after analogue-trauma, higher peritraumatic E2 levels were associated with a greater probability of experiencing film-intrusions in the beginning of the week, which switched to a lower probability toward the end of the week. This time-dependent relationship between E2 and film-intrusions only held for higher state-anxious women. In contrast, results indicated a consistent inverse relationship between peritraumatic E2 levels and pain-intrusions during daily-life and Memory-Triggering-Task. Together, these data suggest that higher peritraumatic E2 levels could be associated with lower long-term visual trauma intrusions, as well as lower pain-intrusions, and thereby possibly constitute a protective biologic factor for PTSD and potentially also for chronic pain.


Subject(s)
Estradiol , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic , Biological Factors , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Female , Humans , Pain , Protective Factors
6.
Pain ; 163(11): 2118-2137, 2022 11 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35239544

ABSTRACT

ABSTRACT: Psychological trauma is typically accompanied by physical pain, and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) often cooccurs with chronic pain. Clinical reports suggest that pain after trauma may be part of re-experiencing symptomatology. Classical conditioning can underlie visual re-experiencing because intrusions can occur as conditioned responses (CRs) to trauma-related cues. If individuals also experience pain to cues previously paired with, but not inflicting nociceptive stimulation anymore (ie, conditioned stimuli, CS), conditioning could also explain re-experiencing of pain. Sixty-five participants underwent classical conditioning, where painful electrocutaneous stimulation and aversive film clips served as unconditioned stimuli (US) in a 2 (pain/no pain) × 2 (aversive/neutral film) design. Conditioned stimuli were neutral pictures depicting contextual details from the films. One day later, participants were re-exposed to CS during a memory-triggering task (MTT). We assessed pain-CRs by self-report and an fMRI-based marker of nociceptive pain, the neurological pain signature (NPS), and recorded spontaneous daily-life pain intrusions with an e-diary. During conditioning, pain-signaling CS elicited more self-reported pain and NPS responses than no-pain-signaling CS. Possibly because the aversive film masked differences in participants' responses to pain-signaling CS vs no pain-signaling CS, pain-CRs during acquisition were most evident within the neutral film condition. When participants were re-exposed to CS during MTT, self-reported pain-CRs during the neutral film condition and, although more uncertain, NPS-CRs during the aversive film condition persisted. Of importance, participants with stronger pain-CRs showed a greater probability and severity of experiencing spontaneous pain intrusions during daily life. Our data support that spatiotemporally associating innocuous cues with pain (CS) endows these cues to elicit conditioned pain responses in the absence of noxious stimulation. In this way pain can emerge as a CR with emotional and sensory components. Classical conditioning presents a possible mechanism explaining pain intrusions and, more broadly, pain experienced without a nociceptive input.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Classical , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Cues , Fear/psychology , Humans , Pain/etiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnostic imaging , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology
7.
Brain Sci ; 12(3)2022 Mar 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35326331

ABSTRACT

The current study investigated heart rate (HR) and heart rate variability (HRV) across day and night in patients with disorders of consciousness (DOC). We recorded 24-h electrocardiography in 26 patients with DOC (i.e., unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (UWS; n = 16) and (exit) minimally conscious state ((E)MCS; n = 10)). To examine diurnal variations, HR and HRV indices in the time, frequency, and entropy domains were computed for periods of clear day- (forenoon: 8 a.m.-2 p.m.; afternoon: 2 p.m.-8 p.m.) and nighttime (11 p.m.-5 a.m.). The results indicate that patients' interbeat intervals (IBIs) were larger during the night than during the day, indicating HR slowing. The patients in UWS showed larger IBIs compared to the patients in (E)MCS, and the patients with non-traumatic brain injury showed lower HRV entropy than the patients with traumatic brain injury. Additionally, higher HRV entropy was associated with higher EEG entropy during the night. Thus, cardiac activity varies with a diurnal pattern in patients with DOC and can differentiate between patients' diagnoses and etiologies. Moreover, the interaction of heart and brain appears to follow a diurnal rhythm. Thus, HR and HRV seem to mirror the integrity of brain functioning and, consequently, might serve as supplementary measures for improving the validity of assessments in patients with DOC.

8.
Eur J Psychotraumatol ; 12(1): 1991609, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34868483

ABSTRACT

Background: Peritraumatic dissociation is purported to emerge together with attenuated autonomic arousal, immobility, and staring. However, empirical evidence is scarce and heterogeneous. Moreover, it is still a matter of debate whether these responses predict intrusion formation. Objective: The present trauma-analogue study examined associations between peritraumatic dissociation, autonomic activation, facial movements, staring, and intrusion formation. Method: Seventy-one healthy women watched a highly aversive film, while autonomic activation (heart rate, respiratory sinus arrhythmia, skin conductance level), facial movements (temporal variations in corrugator electromyography), and staring (fixation duration, tracklength) were assessed. Afterwards, participants rated the intensity of dissociation during film viewing and reported intrusions and associated distress in a smartphone application for 24 hours. Results: Peritraumatic dissociation was linked to higher autonomic arousal (higher heart rate and, on a trend-level, lower respiratory sinus arrhythmia), increased facial movements, and staring (lower tracklength). Peritraumatic dissociation, higher autonomic arousal (higher heart rate and lower respiratory sinus arrhythmia), staring (higher fixation duration), and, on a trend-level, more facial movements were linked to higher intrusion load (number x distress of intrusions) and together explained 59% of variance. Skin conductance level was neither linked to peritraumatic dissociation nor intrusion load. Conclusions: Our results suggest that, at low-dissociation-levels observed in trauma-analogue studies, peritraumatic dissociation may occur together with heightened autonomic arousal and facial movements, indexing increased negative affect. Staring might, irrespectively of dissociation-levels, serve as objective marker for dissociation. Together, peritraumatic dissociation and its psychophysiological correlates might set the stage for later intrusion formation.


Antecedentes: Se supone que la disociación peritraumática surge junto con la activación autonómica atenuada, la inmovilidad y la mirada fija. Sin embargo, la evidencia empírica es escasa y heterogénea. Además, sigue siendo objeto de debate si estas respuestas predicen la formación de intrusiones.Objetivo: El presente estudio análogo al trauma examinó las asociaciones entre la disociación peritraumática, la activación autonómica, los movimientos faciales, la mirada fija y la formación de intrusiones.Método: Setenta y una mujeres sanas vieron una película altamente aversiva mientras se evaluaba la activación autonómica (frecuencia cardíaca, arritmia sinusal respiratoria, nivel de conductancia de la piel), los movimientos faciales (variaciones temporales en la electromiografía del corrugador) y la mirada fija (duración de la fijación, longitud del seguimiento). Posteriormente, las participantes calificaron la intensidad de la disociación durante la visualización de la película e informaron sobre las intrusiones y la angustia asociada en una aplicación para teléfonos inteligentes durante 24 horas.Resultados: La disociación peritraumática se relacionó con una mayor activación autonómica (mayor frecuencia cardíaca y, a nivel de tendencia, menor arritmia sinusal respiratoria), mayores movimientos faciales y mirada fija (menor duración del seguimiento). La disociación peritraumática, la mayor activación autonómica (mayor frecuencia cardíaca y menor arritmia sinusal respiratoria), la mirada fija (mayor duración de la fijación) y, en un nivel de tendencia, más movimientos faciales estaban vinculados a una mayor carga de intrusiones (número x angustia de intrusiones) y juntos explicaban el 59% de la varianza. El nivel de conductancia de la piel no se relacionó con la disociación peritraumática ni con la carga de intrusión.Conclusiones: Nuestros resultados sugieren que, a niveles bajos de disociación observados en estudios de trauma análogos, la disociación peritraumática puede ocurrir junto con una mayor activación autonómica y movimientos faciales, lo que indica un aumento del afecto negativo. La mirada fija, independientemente de los niveles de disociación, podría servir como marcador objetivo de disociación. En conjunto, la disociación peritraumática y sus correlatos psicofisiológicos podrían sentar las bases para la formación posterior de intrusiones.


Subject(s)
Autonomic Nervous System/physiopathology , Dissociative Disorders/physiopathology , Psychological Trauma/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Arousal/physiology , Eye Movement Measurements , Facial Muscles/physiology , Female , Galvanic Skin Response , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia/physiology , Young Adult
9.
Behav Res Ther ; 135: 103761, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33186828

ABSTRACT

Maladaptive avoidance behaviour, a key symptom of anxiety-related disorders, prevents extinction learning and maintains anxiety. Individual personality traits likely influence avoidance propensity: high sensation-seeking may decrease avoidance, thereby increasing extinction, and neuroticism may have the reverse effect. However, research on this is scarce. Using a naturalistic conditioned avoidance paradigm, 163 women underwent differential fear acquisition to a conditioned stimulus (CSplus). Next, during extinction, participants could either choose a risky shortcut, anticipating shock signalled by CSplus, or a time-consuming avoidance option (lengthy detour). Across participants, increased skin conductance (SCR) acquisition learning predicted subsequent instrumental avoidance. Avoidance, in turn, predicted elevated post-extinction SCR and shock-expectancy, i.e., 'protection-from-extinction'. Mediation analyses revealed that sensation seeking decreased protection-from-extinction-both for shock-expectancy and SCR-via attenuating avoidance. Neither sensation seeking nor neuroticism were related to acquisition learning and neuroticism was neither related to avoidance nor extinction. Transcranial direct currentstimulation administered before extinction did not influence present results. Results highlight the important role of elevated avoidance propensity in fear maintenance. Results moreover provide evidence for reduced sensation-seeking and increased acquisition learning to be avoidance-driving mechanisms. Since approach-avoidance conflicts are faced by anxiety patients on a daily basis, strengthening sensation-seeking-congruent attitudes and approach behaviours may optimize individualized treatment.


Subject(s)
Avoidance Learning , Conditioning, Classical , Extinction, Psychological , Fear , Neuroticism , Female , Galvanic Skin Response , Humans , Young Adult
10.
Biol Psychol ; 151: 107845, 2020 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31958549

ABSTRACT

Emotions involve response synchronization across experiential, physiological, and behavioral systems, referred to as concordance or coherence. Women are thought to be more emotionally aware and expressive than men and may therefore display stronger response concordance; however, research on this topic is scant. Using a random-order film-average design, we assessed concordance among experiential (arousal, valence), autonomic (electrodermal activity, heart rate, preejection period, respiratory sinus arrhythmia), respiratory (respiratory rate), and behavioral (corrugator and zygomatic electromyography) responses to 15 two-minute films varying in valence and arousal. We then calculated for each participant and pair of measures a within-subject correlation index using averages from the 15 films. Pronounced individual concordance of up to 0.9 was observed. Arousal-physiology and valence-behavior concordances were particularly pronounced. Women displayed higher concordance than men for almost all pairs of measures. Findings indicate stronger psychophysiological response coupling in women than men and provide novel insights into affective differences between the sexes.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Arousal/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Sex Factors , Adult , Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Electromyography , Female , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Motion Pictures , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysiology , Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia
11.
Neurobiol Aging ; 87: 26-34, 2020 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31843256

ABSTRACT

Impaired learning and memory functioning are prime markers for Alzheimer's disease (AD). Although initial evidence points to impaired fear acquisition in later AD, no study has investigated fear conditioning in early stages and amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), a condition often preceding AD. The present study examined if fear conditioning gradually decays from healthy elderly to patients with aMCI, to patients with AD. Patients with AD (n = 43), patients with aMCI (n = 43), and matched healthy controls (n = 40) underwent a classical fear conditioning paradigm. During acquisition, a neutral face (conditioned stimulus, CS+) was paired with an electrical stimulus, whereas another face (unconditioned stimulus, CS-) was unpaired. Conditioned responses were measured by unconditioned stimulus expectancy, valence, and skin conductance. Compared to healthy controls, both patient groups showed less differential (CS+ vs. CS-) fear acquisition across all measures. Patients further displayed slowed extinction indexed by higher unconditioned stimulus expectancy and reduced positive valence for CS+, declining from aMCI to AD. Groups did not differ in responses during a preconditioning habituation phase and in unconditioned responding. Diminished differential fear acquisition and slowed extinction could represent prognostic markers for AD onset.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Conditioning, Psychological , Extinction, Psychological , Fear , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Animals , Antibodies, Monoclonal, Humanized , Electric Stimulation , Female , HIV Antibodies , Humans , Male , Memory , Mice
12.
Sensors (Basel) ; 19(20)2019 Oct 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31615054

ABSTRACT

Wearable sensors are increasingly used in research, as well as for personal and private purposes. A variety of scientific studies are based on physiological measurements from such rather low-cost wearables. That said, how accurate are such measurements compared to measurements from well-calibrated, high-quality laboratory equipment used in psychological and medical research? The answer to this question, undoubtedly impacts the reliability of a study's results. In this paper, we demonstrate an approach to quantify the accuracy of low-cost wearables in comparison to high-quality laboratory sensors. We therefore developed a benchmark framework for physiological sensors that covers the entire workflow from sensor data acquisition to the computation and interpretation of diverse correlation and similarity metrics. We evaluated this framework based on a study with 18 participants. Each participant was equipped with one high-quality laboratory sensor and two wearables. These three sensors simultaneously measured the physiological parameters such as heart rate and galvanic skin response, while the participant was cycling on an ergometer following a predefined routine. The results of our benchmarking show that cardiovascular parameters (heart rate, inter-beat interval, heart rate variability) yield very high correlations and similarities. Measurement of galvanic skin response, which is a more delicate undertaking, resulted in lower, but still reasonable correlations and similarities. We conclude that the benchmarked wearables provide physiological measurements such as heart rate and inter-beat interval with an accuracy close to that of the professional high-end sensor, but the accuracy varies more for other parameters, such as galvanic skin response.


Subject(s)
Benchmarking , Wearable Electronic Devices , Adult , Algorithms , Female , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Young Adult
13.
Sensors (Basel) ; 19(17)2019 Sep 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31484366

ABSTRACT

There is a rich repertoire of methods for stress detection using various physiological signals and algorithms. However, there is still a gap in research efforts moving from laboratory studies to real-world settings. A small number of research has verified when a physiological response is a reaction to an extrinsic stimulus of the participant's environment in real-world settings. Typically, physiological signals are correlated with the spatial characteristics of the physical environment, supported by video records or interviews. The present research aims to bridge the gap between laboratory settings and real-world field studies by introducing a new algorithm that leverages the capabilities of wearable physiological sensors to detect moments of stress (MOS). We propose a rule-based algorithm based on galvanic skin response and skin temperature, combing empirical findings with expert knowledge to ensure transferability between laboratory settings and real-world field studies. To verify our algorithm, we carried out a laboratory experiment to create a "gold standard" of physiological responses to stressors. We validated the algorithm in real-world field studies using a mixed-method approach by spatially correlating the participant's perceived stress, geo-located questionnaires, and the corresponding real-world situation from the video. Results show that the algorithm detects MOS with 84% accuracy, showing high correlations between measured (by wearable sensors), reported (by questionnaires and eDiary entries), and recorded (by video) stress events. The urban stressors that were identified in the real-world studies originate from traffic congestion, dangerous driving situations, and crowded areas such as tourist attractions. The presented research can enhance stress detection in real life and may thus foster a better understanding of circumstances that bring about physiological stress in humans.


Subject(s)
Wearable Electronic Devices , Algorithms , Humans , Stress, Physiological/physiology
14.
Cognit Ther Res ; 43(1): 174-184, 2019 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30880849

ABSTRACT

Intrusive thoughts, images, and their appraisal remain difficult to study despite their clinical relevance. Clinical studies typically used time-based (frequency and distress per observation period), while analogue studies mainly used event-based (report upon occurrence) assessment. A comparison of intrusion frequency, distress appraisal, compliance, and reactivity across different assessments is mostly lacking, particularly with regard to analogue research. Here, intrusions were induced via aversive films and assessed by a smart phone application for 4 days. Three sampling modes were compared by randomizing participants to one of three conditions: either one, or five time-based daily prompts, or event-based assessment. At the end of the study, all participants reported intrusions once again in a retrospective summary assessment. Results indicate that intrusions and their distress decayed over a few days. The three assessments did not differ in intrusion frequency, distress appraisal, compliance (generally high), reactivity (generally low), or retrospective summary assessment. Across groups, the more aversive and arousing participants rated the film clips and the more reactivity to the electronic-diary assessment they reported, the more intrusive memories they had; assessment modes did not differ on this. Thus, no general differences were found between electronic-diary assessment modes for analogue intrusions, giving researchers flexibility for tailoring ecological momentary assessment to specific study aims.

15.
Behav Res Ther ; 116: 140-148, 2019 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30921745

ABSTRACT

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is marked by persistent fear of being scrutinized by others. This and most diagnostic symptoms relate to some form of fear of negative evaluation (FNE). More recent accounts of SAD, such as the Bivalent Fear of Evaluation Model, however, complement FNE with fear of positive evaluation (FPE), described as distress and avoidance of positive feedback. An explicit test of the incremental validity of FPE in discriminating SAD patients from controls - over and on top of the explanatory power of FPE - is currently missing and generally, well controlled laboratory experiments with positive and negative social stimuli in this patient group are rare. To fill this gap, we exposed 35 patients with SAD and healthy controls (HCs) to short social-evaluative video clips with actors expressing negative and positive as well as neutral statements while recording reactivity on experiential measures (valence, arousal, and approval ratings) as well as on facial electromyography and electrocardiography. In addition, participants completed questionnaire measures of FNE and FPE. Results revealed that FPE questionnaire scores as well as experiential (valence and appreciation) and electromyographical reactivity measures to positive videos improved prediction of group membership beyond the predictive power of FNE questionnaires scores and reactivity to negative videos. Results document the importance of FPE to more fully characterize and understand social anxiety and SAD. Implications include amendments to future diagnostic criteria, theoretical models, and treatment approaches for SAD.


Subject(s)
Fear/physiology , Fear/psychology , Phobia, Social/physiopathology , Phobia, Social/psychology , Social Perception , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Electrocardiography , Electromyography , Face/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
16.
Biol Psychol ; 131: 54-62, 2018 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27654506

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Stress and emotions alter eating behavior in several ways: While experiencing negative or positive emotions typically leads to increased food intake, stress may result in either over- or undereating. Several participant characteristics, like gender, BMI and restrained, emotional, or external eating styles seem to influence these relationships. Thus far, most research relied on experimental laboratory studies, thereby reducing the complexity of real-life eating episodes. The aim of the present study was to delineate the effects of stress, negative and positive emotions on two key facets of eating behavior, namely taste- and hunger-based eating, in daily life using ecological momentary assessment (EMA). Furthermore, the already mentioned individual differences as well as time pressure during eating, an important but unstudied construct in EMA studies, were examined. METHODS: Fifty-nine participants completed 10days of signal-contingent sampling and data were analyzed using multilevel modeling. RESULTS: Results revealed that higher stress led to decreased taste-eating which is in line with physiological stress-models. Time pressure during eating resulted in less taste- and more hunger-eating. In line with previous research, stronger positive emotions went along with increased taste-eating. Emotional eating style moderated the relationship between negative emotions and taste-eating as well as hunger-eating. BMI moderated the relationship between negative as well as positive emotions and hunger-eating. CONCLUSIONS: These findings emphasize the importance of individual differences for understanding eating behavior in daily life. Experienced time pressure may be an important aspect for future EMA eating studies.


Subject(s)
Eating/psychology , Emotions , Feeding Behavior/psychology , Stress, Psychological/psychology , Taste Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Ecological Momentary Assessment , Female , Humans , Hunger , Male , Middle Aged , Time Factors , Young Adult
17.
Biol Psychol ; 130: 30-40, 2017 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29054817

ABSTRACT

Sex differences in emotional reactivity have been studied primarily for negative but less so for positive stimuli; likewise, sex differences in the psychophysiological response-patterning during such stimuli are poorly understood. Thus, the present study examined sex differences in response to negative/positive and high/low arousing films (classified as threat-, loss-, achievement-, and recreation-related, vs. neutral films), while measuring 18 muscular, autonomic, and respiratory parameters. Sex differences emerged for all films, but were most prominent for threat-related films: Despite equivalent valence and arousal ratings, women displayed more facial-muscular and respiratory responding than men and pronounced sympathetic activation (preejection period, other cardiovascular and electrodermal measures), while men showed coactivated sympathetic/parasympathetic responding (including increased respiratory sinus arrhythmia). This indicates a prototypical threat-related defense response in women, while men showed a pattern of sustained orienting, which can be understood as a shift toward less threat proximity in the defense cascade model. Clinical implications are discussed within a socio-evolutionary framework.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Arousal/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Motion Pictures , Sex Factors , Adult , Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Female , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Psychophysiology , Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia/physiology , Social Behavior , Young Adult
18.
Behav Res Methods ; 48(4): 1528-1545, 2016 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26511369

ABSTRACT

Psychophysiological science employs a large variety of signals from the human body that index the activity of the peripheral nervous system. This allows for studying interactions of psychological and physiological processes that are relevant for understanding cognition, emotion, and psychopathology. The multidimensional nature of the data and the interactions between different physiological signals represent a methodological and computational challenge. Analysis software in this domain is often limited in its coverage of the signals from different physiological systems, and therefore only partially addresses these challenges. ANSLAB (short for Autonomic Nervous System Laboratory) is an integrated software suite that supports data visualization, artifact detection, data reduction, automated processing, and statistical analysis for a large range of autonomic, respiratory, and muscular measures. Analysis modules for cardiovascular (e.g., electrocardiogram, heart rate variability, blood pressure wave, pulse wave, and impedance cardiography), electrodermal (skin conductance level and responses), respiratory (respiratory pattern, timing, and volume variables, as well as capnography), and muscular (eye-blink startle, facial and bodily electromyography) systems are complemented by specialized modules (e.g., body temperature and accelerometry, cross-spectral analysis of respiratory and cardiac measures, signal averaging, and statistical analysis) and productivity-enhancing features (batched processing, fully automatized analyses, and data management). ANSLAB also facilitates the analysis of long-term recordings from ambulatory assessment studies. The present article reviews several analysis modules included in ANSLAB and describes how these address some of the current needs and methodological challenges of psychophysiological science.


Subject(s)
Psychophysiology/methods , Software , Autonomic Nervous System/physiology , Data Display , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Humans
19.
IEEE Rev Biomed Eng ; 4: 73-88, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22273792

ABSTRACT

Today, medical endoscopy is a widely used procedure to inspect the inner cavities of the human body. The advent of endoscopic imaging techniques-allowing the acquisition of images or videos-created the possibility for the development of the whole new branch of computer-aided decision support systems. Such systems aim at helping physicians to identify possibly malignant abnormalities more accurately. At the beginning of this paper, we give a brief introduction to the history of endoscopy, followed by introducing the main types of endoscopes which emerged so far (flexible endoscope, wireless capsule endoscope, and confocal laser endomicroscope). We then give a brief introduction to computer-aided decision support systems specifically targeted at endoscopy in the gastrointestinal tract. Then we present general facts and figures concerning computer-aided decision support systems and summarize work specifically targeted at computer-aided decision support in the gastrointestinal tract. This summary is followed by a discussion of some common issues concerning the approaches reviewed and suggestions of possible ways to resolve them.


Subject(s)
Computers , Decision Support Systems, Clinical , Endoscopy, Gastrointestinal/methods , Capsule Endoscopes , Capsule Endoscopy , Endoscopes, Gastrointestinal , Gastrointestinal Diseases/diagnosis , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Microscopy, Confocal/methods , Software
20.
Comput Methods Programs Biomed ; 95(2 Suppl): S68-78, 2009 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19356823

ABSTRACT

Feature extraction techniques based on selection of highly discriminant Fourier filters have been developed for an automated classification of magnifying endoscope images with respect to pit patterns of colon lesions. These are applied to duodenal imagery for diagnosis of celiac disease. Features are extracted from the Fourier domain by selecting the most discriminant features using an evolutionary algorithm. Subsequent classification is performed with various standard algorithms (KNN, SVM, Bayes classifier) and combination of several Fourier filters and classifiers which is called multiclassifier. The obtained results are promising, due to a high specificity for the detection of mucosal damage typical of untreated celiac disease.


Subject(s)
Automation , Celiac Disease/diagnosis , Duodenum/pathology , Fourier Analysis , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...