Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 33
Filter
1.
J Interpers Violence ; 38(1-2): NP772-NP795, 2023 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35343282

ABSTRACT

Status and affection are both goals related to social needs. The imbalanced needs theory of aggression proposes that although aggression can be used to realize status, this strategy is detrimental for realizing affection in the same social context. Thus, to the degree that the social circles overlap in which status and affection needs are realized, it becomes more costly (in terms of affection) to achieve status via aggression. This theory was tested for different forms of aggression, in different contexts, in a sample of adults from the general population (N = 253, M age = 29.95, SD = 2.60, 78% female). Participants reported on social needs with the Interpersonal Goals Inventory and reported on general measures of physical and social aggression, as well as rule breaking, and aggression at the workplace and in intimate partner relationships. As hypothesized, status needs were associated with physical aggression when affection needs were weak. This interaction, though to a lesser degree, also extended to social forms of aggression and rule breaking. At the workplace, aggression was only related to weak affection needs, whereas aggression in intimate partner relationships was, as expected, unrelated to both social needs. Together, these findings support the results of an earlier test of the imbalanced needs theory of aggression in adolescence, and encourage more research into the link between aggression and the satisfaction of social needs.


Subject(s)
Aggression , Interpersonal Relations , Adult , Adolescent , Humans , Female , Male , Sexual Partners , Social Environment
2.
J Youth Adolesc ; 49(7): 1481-1491, 2020 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31732836

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have shown that status goals motivate direct forms of interpersonal aggression. However, status goals have been studied mostly in isolation from affection goals. It is theorized that the means by which status and affection goals are satisfied change during adolescence, which can affect aggression. This is tested in a pooled sample of (pre)adolescents (N = 1536; 49% girls; ages 10-15), by examining associations between status goals and direct aggression and the moderating role of affection goals. As hypothesized, with increasing age, status goals were more strongly associated with direct aggression. Moreover, for older adolescents, status goals were only associated with aggression when affection goals were weak. These findings support the changing relationship between status goals and direct aggression during adolescence.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Aggression/psychology , Goals , Self Concept , Adolescent , Child , Crime Victims/psychology , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Peer Group , Social Desirability
3.
Sci Data ; 6(1): 32, 2019 04 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30996323

ABSTRACT

In the Human Penguin Project (N = 1755), 15 research groups from 12 countries collected body temperature, demographic variables, social network indices, seven widely-used psychological scales and two newly developed questionnaires (the Social Thermoregulation and Risk Avoidance Questionnaire (STRAQ-1) and the Kama Muta Frequency Scale (KAMF)). They were collected to investigate the relationship between environmental factors (e.g., geographical, climate etc.) and human behaviors, which is a long-standing inquiry in the scientific community. More specifically, the present project was designed to test principles surrounding the idea of social thermoregulation, which posits that social networks help people to regulate their core body temperature. The results showed that all scales in the current project have sufficient to good psychometrical properties. Unlike previous crowdsourced projects, this dataset includes not only the cleaned raw data but also all the validation of questionnaires in 9 different languages, thus providing a valuable resource for psychological scientists who are interested in cross-national, environment-human interaction studies.


Subject(s)
Body Temperature Regulation , Social Environment , Body Temperature , Body Temperature Regulation/physiology , Climate , Demography , Humans , Surveys and Questionnaires
4.
Behav Brain Sci ; 40: e86, 2017 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29342545

ABSTRACT

We comment on the proposition "that lower temperatures and especially greater seasonal variation in temperature call for individuals and societies to adopt … a greater degree of self-control" (Van Lange et al., sect. 3, para. 4) for which we cannot find empirical support in a large data set with data-driven analyses. After providing greater nuance in our theoretical review, we suggest that Van Lange et al. revisit their model with an eye toward the social determinants of self-control.


Subject(s)
Aggression , Self-Control , Animals , Climate , Humans , Spheniscidae , Violence
5.
J Adolesc ; 45: 1-10, 2015 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26349449

ABSTRACT

This study investigates the types and behavioral associations of peer status in school-bound young adults in the Netherlands. We argue that adolescent peer popularity and its link with aggressive and norm-breaking behavior result from adolescents' desire to create an image of maturity among their peers. We expect that in young adults who are approaching working life, peer status is defined by affective measures of status and prosociality rather than adverse behaviors. Analyses revealed a three cluster solution of (1) liked, (2) liked-popular and (3) neutral members of the peer group, showing that status is primarily defined by being well-liked, though popularity remains relevant. Status was primarily associated with prosocial behavior, especially for females. Peer status in young males remained associated with overt aggressive behavior.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Interpersonal Relations , Peer Group , Adolescent , Aggression/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Netherlands , Surveys and Questionnaires
6.
Front Psychol ; 6: 464, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25954223

ABSTRACT

Beyond breathing, the regulation of body temperature-thermoregulation-is one of the most pressing concerns for many animals. A dysregulated body temperature has dire consequences for survival and development. Despite the high frequency of social thermoregulation occurring across many species, little is known about the role of social thermoregulation in human (social) psychological functioning. We outline a theory of social thermoregulation and reconsider earlier research on people's expectations of their social world (i.e., attachment) and their prediction of the social world. We provide support and outline a research agenda that includes consequences for individual variation in self-regulatory strategies and capabilities. In our paper, we discuss physiological, neural, and social processes surrounding thermoregulation. Emphasizing social thermoregulation in particular, we appeal to the economy of action principle and the hierarchical organization of human thermoregulatory systems. We close with future directions of a crucial aspect of human functioning: the social regulation of body temperature.

7.
Behav Brain Sci ; 38: e104, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26786163

ABSTRACT

In their target article, Kalisch et al. explicate an appraisal-based model to explain how people bounce back from stress. We posit that for their model, it is crucial to understand the begin-state φ (the "self") - a state that is shaped by early social thermoregulation and through the social network.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Social Environment , Humans , Social Support
8.
PLoS One ; 9(4): e95421, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24759901

ABSTRACT

Rejection can convey that one is seen as inferior and not worth bothering with. Is it possible for people to feel vicariously rejected in this sense and have reactions that are similar to those following personal rejection, such as feeling humiliated, powerless, and angry? A study on personal rejection was followed by two main studies on vicarious group-based rejection. It was found that merely observing rejection of ingroup members can trigger feelings of humiliation that are equally intense as those experienced in response to personal rejection. Moreover, given that the rejection is explicit, vicariously experienced feelings of humiliation can be accompanied by powerlessness and anger. Potentially, this combination of emotions could be an important source of offensive action against rejecters.


Subject(s)
Anger/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Rejection, Psychology , Adolescent , Dangerous Behavior , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
9.
PLoS One ; 8(6): e65137, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23755182

ABSTRACT

Contrary to what is often assumed, order is not the strongest context for encouraging normative behavior. The strongest context effect on normative behavior comes from cues that clearly convey other people's respect for norms. Ironically, this show of respect necessitates some contrasting disrespect that is being restored. Using civic virtues (such as helping behavior) as a prototype of normative behavior, the three field experiments described in this paper reveal the impact of normative cues on civic virtues. Results show that the strongest effect on making people follow prosocial norms in public places emanates from seeing order being restored, rather than just order being present. The robust and surprisingly large effects show that observing other people's respect for one particular norm (as evidenced in their restoring physical order) makes it more likely that the onlooker follows other norms as well. This implies that prosocial behavior has the highest chance of spreading when people observe order being restored. There are clear policy implications: create low cost "normative respect cues" wherever it is desirable to increase conformity to norms.


Subject(s)
Helping Behavior , Social Conformity , Behavior Control , Female , Humans , Male , Mass Behavior , Motivation
10.
PLoS One ; 8(2): e54821, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23457454

ABSTRACT

In adolescence, being socially successful depends to a large extent on being popular with peers. Even though some youths have what it takes to be popular, they are not, whereas others seem to have a secret ingredient that just makes the difference. In this study the G-allele of a functional polymorphism in the promotor region of the 5HT2A serotonin receptor gene (-G1438A) was identified as a secret ingredient for popularity among peers. These findings build on and extend previous work by Burt (2008, 2009). Tackling limitations from previous research, the role of the 5HT2A serotonin receptor gene was examined in adolescent males (N = 285; average age 13) using a unique sample of the TRAILS study. Carrying the G-allele enhanced the relation between aggression and popularity, particularly for those boys who have many female friends. This seems to be an "enhancer" effect of the G-allele whereby popularity relevant characteristics are made more noticeable. There is no "popularity gene", as the G-allele by itself had no effect on popularity.


Subject(s)
Polymorphism, Genetic , Receptor, Serotonin, 5-HT2A/genetics , Achievement , Adolescent , Aggression , Alleles , Female , Friends , Genotype , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Peer Group
11.
J Adolesc Health ; 52(1): 102-7, 2013 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23260842

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Low heart rate (HR) has been linked to antisocial behavior (ASB). However, the effect of low HR may be mediated by affiliation with bullies. We hypothesized that individuals with low HR are more likely to affiliate with bullies and in turn are influenced by these peers. METHODS: Data come from two waves of a subsample of the TRAILS study (N = 809; 44.0% boys; mean age of 11.0 years at T1 and 13.5 years at T2). ASB was measured using the Antisocial Behavior Questionnaire at both waves. HR was assessed during rest at T1. Affiliation with bullies was assessed via peer nominations at T1. Possible gender differences were taken into account, and all analyses were adjusted for family context (i.e., family breakup and socioeconomic status). RESULTS: Regression analyses showed that lower HR was only associated with ASB in (pre)adolescents who affiliated with bullies. Moreover, the effect of lower HR on boys' ASB was partly mediated by affiliation with bullies. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings show that (pre)adolescents, and in particular boys, seem to be in environments that match their biological disposition and in turn are shaped by this environment.


Subject(s)
Antisocial Personality Disorder/physiopathology , Bullying/physiology , Heart Rate , Adolescent , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Negotiating , Peer Group , Regression Analysis , Surveys and Questionnaires
12.
J Adolesc Health ; 50(4): 371-6, 2012 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22443841

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To examine the relative contribution of weapon carrying of peers, aggression, and victimization to weapon carrying of male and female adolescents over time. METHODS: Data were derived from a population-based sample of male (N = 224) and female (N = 244) adolescents followed from grade 10 (M age = 15.5) to grade 11 (M age = 16.5). Peer networks were derived from best friend nominations. Self-reports were used to assess weapon carrying. Aggression and victimization were assessed using both self- and peer-reports. Use of dynamic social network modeling (SIENA) allowed prediction of weapon carrying in grade 11 as a function of weapon carrying of befriended peers, aggression, and victimization in grade 10, while selection processes and structural network effects (reciprocity and transitivity) were controlled for. RESULTS: Peer influence processes accounted for changes in weapon carrying over time. Self-reported victimization decreased weapon carrying 1 year later. Peer-reported victimization increased the likelihood of weapon carrying, particularly for highly aggressive adolescents. Boys were more likely to carry weapons than girls, but the processes associated with weapon carrying did not differ for boys and girls. CONCLUSIONS: These findings revealed that, in this population-based sample, weapon carrying of best friends, as well as aggression, contributed to the proliferation of weapons in friendship networks, suggesting processes of peer contagion as well as individual vulnerability to weapon carrying.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Crime Victims/psychology , Peer Group , Weapons , Adolescent , Female , Humans , Male , Social Support , United States , Weapons/statistics & numerical data
13.
Aggress Behav ; 37(5): 417-29, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21688275

ABSTRACT

The role of physical and relational aggression in adolescents' friendship selection was examined in a longitudinal sample of 274 Chilean students from 5th and 6th grade followed over 1 year. Longitudinal social network modeling (SIENA) was used to study selection processes for aggression while influence processes were controlled for. Furthermore, the effects of network characteristics (i.e., reciprocity and transitivity), gender, and social status on friendship selection were examined. The starting assumption of this study was that selection effects based on aggression might have been overestimated in previous research as a result of failing to consider influence processes and alternative characteristics that steer friendship formation. The results show that selection effects of both physical and relational aggression disappeared when network effects, gender, and social status were taken into account. Particularly gender and perceived popularity appeared to be far more important determinants of friendship selection over time than aggression. Moreover, a peer influence effect was only found for relational aggression, and not for physical aggression. These findings suggest that similarity in aggression among befriended adolescents can be considered to be mainly a by-product rather than a leading dimension in friendship selection.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Friends/psychology , Interpersonal Relations , Sex Characteristics , Social Support , Adolescent , Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Child , Chile , Female , Humans , Male , Personality , Surveys and Questionnaires
14.
Science ; 332(6026): 251-3, 2011 Apr 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21474762

ABSTRACT

Being the victim of discrimination can have serious negative health- and quality-of-life-related consequences. Yet, could being discriminated against depend on such seemingly trivial matters as garbage on the streets? In this study, we show, in two field experiments, that disordered contexts (such as litter or a broken-up sidewalk and an abandoned bicycle) indeed promote stereotyping and discrimination in real-world situations and, in three lab experiments, that it is a heightened need for structure that mediates these effects (number of subjects: between 40 and 70 per experiment). These findings considerably advance our knowledge of the impact of the physical environment on stereotyping and discrimination and have clear policy implications: Diagnose environmental disorder early and intervene immediately.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Environment , Prejudice , Stereotyping , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
15.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 49(5): 493-502, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20431469

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Why is low resting heart rate (HR) associated with antisocial behavior (ASB), i.e., aggression and rule breaking, in adolescence? Theory suggests that personality traits mediate this relationship but differently with age. In the present study this age-effect hypothesis is tested; we expected that the relationship between HR and aggression would be mediated in preadolescence by the personality trait behavioral inhibition, but not by sensation seeking. However, the relationship between HR and rule breaking in adolescence was predicted to be mediated by sensation seeking, but not by behavioral inhibition. Hypotheses were tested separately for boys and girls. METHOD: HR in supine position was assessed in repondents to the TRacking Adolescents' Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS) (N = 1,752; 48.5% boys) at age 11 years. Rule breaking and aggression at age 16 were assessed with two subscales from the Youth Self Report (YSR) questionnaire. Personality (i.e., sensation seeking and behavioral inhibition) was measured at ages 11, 13.5, and 16 with the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire-Revised (EATQ-R), Behavioral Inhibition System/ Behavioral Activation System (BIS/BAS) scales, or NEO Personality-Index Revised (NEO-PI-R). RESULTS: In boys, lower HR was associated with aggression and rule breaking in adolescence. The association between HR and rule breaking was mediated by sensation seeking in adolescence but not in preadolescence. Girls' HR was not associated with ASB, and no mediating effects were found. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings support the age-effect hypothesis in boys' rule breaking behavior. This shows that the association between HR and ASB depends on age, gender, and subtype of ASB.


Subject(s)
Antisocial Personality Disorder/physiopathology , Heart Rate , Adolescent , Age Factors , Aggression , Antisocial Personality Disorder/etiology , Antisocial Personality Disorder/psychology , Child , Electrocardiography , Female , Humans , Male , Personality , Personality Inventory , Sex Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires , Temperament
16.
Child Dev ; 81(2): 480-6, 2010.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20438454

ABSTRACT

To understand the complex nature of bullies' acceptance and rejection, this article considered goal-framing effects of status and affection as they relate to the gender of the bully (male vs. female bullies), the target (male vs. female victims), and the evaluator (acceptance and rejection from male vs. female classmates). The hypotheses were tested with data from a social network questionnaire conducted in 26 elementary school classes (N = 481 children; M(age) = 10.5 years). The findings revealed that bullies were only rejected by those for whom they were a potential threat and that bullies generally chose their victims so as to minimize loss of affection by choosing victims who were not likely to be defended by significant others.


Subject(s)
Crime Victims/psychology , Gender Identity , Rejection, Psychology , Social Behavior , Social Identification , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Peer Group , Psychometrics , Social Desirability , Sociometric Techniques , Surveys and Questionnaires
17.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 38(6): 803-13, 2010 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20336362

ABSTRACT

In this study a homophily selection hypothesis was tested against a default selection hypothesis, to answer whether preferred and realized friendships of highly aggressive boys differed. In a large peer-nomination sample, we assessed who highly overt aggressive, low prosocial boys (n = 181) nominated as friends (preferred friendships) and who among the nominated friends reciprocated the friendship (realized friendships). These preferred and realized friendships were compared with those of less aggressive (n = 1,268) and highly aggressive but also prosocial boys (bi-strategics; n = 55). Results showed that less aggressive boys preferred peers low on aggression, whereas highly aggressive and bi-strategic boys preferred peers not particular high or low on aggression. In line with default selection, highly aggressive boys ended up with aggressive peers even though that was not their preference. In general, received support proved an important determinant of highly aggressive, bi-strategic, and less aggressive boys' preferred and realized friendships. Especially highly aggressive boys preferred emotionally supportive friends, but ended up with the least supportive peers. In sum, for friendships of highly overt aggressive boys, the evidence favors default selection over homophily selection.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Friends/psychology , Peer Group , Adolescent , Chi-Square Distribution , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Logistic Models , Male , Social Adjustment
18.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 38(1): 119-30, 2010 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19707865

ABSTRACT

In a large sample of early adolescents (T2: n = 1023; M age = 13.51; 55.5% girls) it was investigated whether the effects of parental and peer acceptance and rejection on psychopathology (externalizing and internalizing problems) remain when taking into account both contexts simultaneously. Moreover, we examined whether acceptance in one context can buffer rejection in the other. It was found that when analyzing peer and parent effects simultaneously (1) the protective effect of parental acceptance and the risk effect of peer rejection were diminished; (2) the protective effect of peer acceptance and the risk-effect of parental rejection remained strong; and (3) peer acceptance buffered parental rejection but parental acceptance did not buffer peer rejection. The results imply that the parent and peer contexts are interdependent. Implications and directions for future research are given.


Subject(s)
Conduct Disorder/diagnosis , Conduct Disorder/psychology , Interpersonal Relations , Parent-Child Relations , Peer Group , Rejection, Psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Aggression , Child , Conduct Disorder/epidemiology , Female , Humans , Male , Motivation , Object Attachment , Prospective Studies , Risk Factors , Social Environment , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
19.
Br J Soc Psychol ; 49(Pt 1): 175-87, 2010 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19397843

ABSTRACT

In two studies we show that people make environments norm-relevant and this increases the likelihood that environments influence norm-relevant judgments. When people see environments without having people on their mind, this effect does not occur. Specifically, when exposed to an environment (a library), people's perceived importance of environment-relevant norms (be silent in libraries) increases, when the concept of 'people' is primed compared to when this is not the case. The impact on normative judgments of priming significant others (Study 1) is stronger than priming people in general (Study 2). Additional effects on conformism and public self-consciousness are discussed, as well as implications for future studies.


Subject(s)
Interpersonal Relations , Judgment , Self Concept , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Female , Humans , Male , Social Environment , Young Adult
20.
Dev Psychol ; 45(2): 419-30, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19271828

ABSTRACT

This study examined the possible risk-buffering and risk-enhancing role of family characteristics on the association between temperament and early adolescent externalizing and internalizing problems, adjusted for familial vulnerability for psychopathology and early childhood problem behavior. Furthermore, it explored whether these effects were specific or conditional for either internalizing or externalizing problems or more generic for psychopathology. Data on temperament (frustration and fearfulness) and family characteristics (overprotection, rejection, emotional warmth, and socioeconomic status) came from a large longitudinal Dutch population sample of early adolescents (n = 2,149; M age = 13.55 years; 51.2% girls). Hypotheses on the direction and the specificity of the effects were derived from a goal-framing approach. The findings indicate that family characteristics can either buffer or enhance the temperamental risk in the development of psychopathology. Analyses on the direction of these effects resulted in a descriptive classification of domain-specific, conditional, and generic factors that promote or protect the development of psychopathology. Implications of the results are discussed, and directions for future research are given.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Internal-External Control , Parenting/psychology , Personality Development , Social Environment , Temperament , Adolescent , Child , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Netherlands , Prospective Studies , Psychopathology , Risk Factors , Socialization , Socioeconomic Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...