Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 31
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Vision Res ; 43(9): 983-92, 2003 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12676242

ABSTRACT

Studies indicate dichromats detect large, long duration spectral increments presented on bright white backgrounds with a blue-yellow colour opponent mechanism. Since opponent processes signal colour, we hypothesized that under these viewing conditions dichromats should perceive spectral increments as coloured at detection threshold. Psychophysical detection and colour discrimination thresholds were determined for normal and dichromatic humans. Test stimuli were 2 degrees, 200 ms increments presented upon a white, 1000 td, spatially coincident background. As expected, normal observers were able to discriminate between white and spectral flashes at intensities near detection threshold intensities. Dichromatic observers required suprathreshold ( approximately 0.30 log units) stimulus intensities to discriminate between the white and spectral flashes. The results do not support our hypothesis and alternative explanations for the elevated colour discrimination thresholds in dichromats are discussed.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Color Vision Defects/psychology , Sensory Thresholds , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychophysics
2.
Vis Neurosci ; 17(1): 119-25, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10750833

ABSTRACT

Psychophysical (behavioral) detection thresholds and color-discrimination thresholds were determined in a macaque using a two-alternative forced-choice procedure. On a white background, detection thresholds were determined for a white increment and three spectral increments: 618, 516, and 456 nm. Intermixed with detection threshold determinations, color-discrimination thresholds were determined by presenting the white increment, and one of the spectral increments, at 1.0 log units above their respective detection thresholds and dimming both until discrimination performance fell to threshold. The monkey could discriminate the color of the increments at detection threshold because the average color-discrimination threshold was 0.98+/-0.14 log attenuation. Because the monkey was much more sensitive to the spectral increments than the white increment, we performed an unconventional experiment. We determined the monkey's detection threshold for the white increment alone, and with broadband color filters in the white light path without adjusting the light's intensity. Insertion of several color filters in the light path lowered detection thresholds of both the macaque and six human trichromats. We believe that this improvement in detection thresholds produced by simply inserting color filters in a white light path is a threshold manifestation of the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect and suggests that one of color vision's important evolutionary advantages may be improved detection sensitivity.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Animals , Color Perception Tests , Female , Humans , Macaca mulatta , Male , Middle Aged , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
3.
Vision Res ; 34(22): 2997-3003, 1994 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7975334

ABSTRACT

Psychophysical increment thresholds were compared for periods of phenomenological dominance or suppression produced by different stimulation of the two eyes. Three experimental procedures were used; binocular rivalry, permanent suppression and flash suppression. The amount of suppression produced by each procedure was evaluated under conditions intended to accentuate color or luminance system contribution to the detection of a spectral flash. All three procedures resulted in a different pattern of color and luminance suppression. Binocular rivalry suppressed color sensitivity more than luminance and within color, blue (439 nm) sensitivity was more suppressed than red (613 nm). Permanent suppression resulted in a similar pattern of suppression but only blue color sensitivity was reliably more suppressed than luminance sensitivity. Flash suppression produced distinctly different results such that blue color sensitivity was reliably less suppressed than luminance or red color sensitivity, which were not different from each other. Taken together these results provide clues as to where and when the physiological processes mediating visual suppression may be found in the nervous system.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Light , Vision Disparity/physiology , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds
4.
Vis Neurosci ; 9(6): 617-22, 1992 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1450113

ABSTRACT

The reports of rod-dominated psychophysical spectral sensitivity from the deprived eye of monocularly lid-sutured (MD) monkeys are intriguing but difficult to reconcile with the absence of any reported deprivation effects in retina. As most studies of MD retina have been from cat, we have examined psychophysically the increment threshold spectral sensitivity of MD cats using both reaction time and simultaneous two-choice behavioral procedures. Although the deprived eyes exhibited an absolute increment threshold sensitivity deficit, both rod and cone spectral sensitivity functions were obtained on large white backgrounds. This normal transition from rod to cone vision, as background luminance increased, was also found in threshold vs. intensity functions. Using their deprived eye, some cats exhibited a rod spectral sensitivity function when a smaller, normally photopic, background was used providing some support for a hypothesis that the rod-dominated spectral sensitivity observed in monkey may represent detection of scattered stimulus light. Alternatively monocular deprivation may reveal a rod-dominated mechanism which exists in monkey but not in cat.


Subject(s)
Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Vision, Monocular/physiology , Animals , Cats , Photic Stimulation , Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds
5.
Vision Res ; 31(9): 1633-7, 1991.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1949632

ABSTRACT

We measured increment thresholds, using a reaction time or two-choice behavioral technique, in three cats monocularly deprived of normal vision for 14-16 months. Luminance increment thresholds could be obtained as early as nine days after lid opening and improved by 0.5-2.0 log units over the next few weeks. Also, visual reaction times decreased during the sensitivity improvement. When compared to the data for grating discrimination, these results suggest that visual recovery from monocular deprivation may proceed at different rates for different psychophysical discriminations.


Subject(s)
Light , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Animals , Cats , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Reinforcement, Psychology , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Time Factors
6.
Vis Neurosci ; 5(6): 557-64, 1990 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2085471

ABSTRACT

If physiological mechanisms similar to cat Y and X cells explain faster detection of low spatial frequencies by humans, then cats should show the same effect. We have tested this prediction by determining the visual reaction time of cats over a range of spatial frequencies and contrasts by training them to respond quickly when a vertical sine-wave grating was presented. At 50% contrast, the cat's visual reaction time increased monotonically from 0.25-2.0 cpd (cycle/deg). At every spatial frequency tested, the cat's reaction time increased monotonically as contrast decreased. By determining contrast threshold (70% detection) at each spatial frequency, it was possible to determine reaction times for different spatial frequencies at equal physical contrasts and equal "threshold equivalent" contrasts. Some of the cat's faster detection of low spatial frequencies was due to sensitivity differences and some was not. To determine if faster detection of low spatial frequencies was based upon Y cells, we took advantage of the fact that Y cells show a strong peripheral effect while X cells do not. Low and high spatial frequencies were detected in the presence of a flickering (7 Hz) or steady (70 Hz) surround. Surround frequency had no effect upon reaction times to 2.0 cpd but the flickering surround increased reaction times to 0.25 cpd. These results indicate that, in cats, rapid detection of low spatial frequencies is by Y cells and slower detection of high spatial frequencies is by X cells.


Subject(s)
Reaction Time , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Animals , Cats , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Retinal Ganglion Cells/physiology , Sensory Thresholds , Spatial Behavior
7.
Vision Res ; 30(7): 1107-10, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2392839

ABSTRACT

Human observers have different thresholds for flicker detection and color detection of a rapidly flickering spectral stimulus presented on a steady white background. A flickering surround, which did not overlap the stimulus or background, reduced flicker sensitivity but not color sensitivity for both monocular and binocular viewing. However, a flickering surround presented to one eye had no influence upon either color or flicker thresholds of the other eye.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Flicker Fusion/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Macula Lutea/physiology , Adult , Humans , Male , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Spectrophotometry , Vision, Binocular/physiology , Vision, Monocular/physiology
8.
J Physiol ; 382: 537-53, 1987 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3625560

ABSTRACT

1. The psychophysical spectral sensitivity of cats was assessed using a two-choice visual discrimination task by determining increment thresholds and critical flicker frequency on white and chromatic backgrounds. 2. For large increments, on 0.0, 0.3 and 3.0 cd/m2 white backgrounds, the cats were most sensitive to 497 nm indicating that these backgrounds are scotopic. On 30 and 300 cd/m2 white backgrounds, the cats were most sensitive to about 454 and 561 nm indicating that these backgrounds are photopic. Sensitivity to intermediate wave-lengths indicated independent action of 'blue' and 'green' cones. 3. For large increments, thresholds on photopic yellow and magenta backgrounds indicated the additive influence of 'blue' and 'green' cones. 4. Spectral sensitivity functions obtained with a critical flicker frequency criterion of 10 Hz on a 30 cd/m2 white background reflected only the activity of the 'green' cone while at 20 Hz the function reflected an additive contribution of both 'blue' and 'green' cones. 5. For small increments, on a 30 cd/m2 white or 96 cd/m2 orange background, sensitivity reflected only the activity of the 'green' cone. 6. The cat's photopic spectral sensitivity is influenced by the psychophysical test upon which it is based in a manner that is similar to what has been found for other vertebrates. No evidence was found for a 500 nm mechanism active at photopic levels.


Subject(s)
Cats/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Adaptation, Ocular , Animals , Female , Flicker Fusion , Light , Male , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
10.
Behav Brain Res ; 9(2): 143-50, 1983 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6882521

ABSTRACT

Increment thresholds on a white background were determined for normal and binocularly deprived (BD) cats over a wide luminance range. Threshold vs intensity curves had a slope of unity for both groups but the increment threshold (delta I/I) for normal cats was 0.09 while increment threshold for BD cats was 0.60. Absolute threshold was reliably better in normal cats. the deficit of BD cats is considered a potential result of their abnormal cortical physiology and/or abnormal lateral geniculate physiology.


Subject(s)
Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Retina/physiology , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Animals , Cats , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Female , Male , Sensory Thresholds , Visual Pathways/physiology
12.
Behav Brain Res ; 6(2): 185-94, 1982 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7138645

ABSTRACT

Critical flicker frequency (CFF) was determined for both eyes of long-term monocularly deprived (MD) cats over a wide luminance range. Although MD cats could discriminate flicker before and after lid opening, CFF of the deprived eye (30 Hz) was much lower than CFF of the non-deprived eye (40 Hz) and the CFF of the non-deprived eye was lower than a normal cat's monocular CFF (58 Hz). The CFF deficit of the deprived eye became less pronounced at low luminance levels. The observation (and magnitude) of a CFF deficit for the deprived eye is compatible with the reports of a Y-cell loss in LGNd. The CFF deficit of the non-deprived eye has no obvious explanation.


Subject(s)
Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Flicker Fusion/physiology , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Animals , Cats , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Female , Geniculate Bodies/physiology , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Retinal Ganglion Cells/physiology
14.
Vision Res ; 22(4): 445-7, 1982.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7112944

ABSTRACT

Reaction time distributions were determined in response to near-threshold intensity increments predicted to isolate either the opponent-color or luminance system. The reaction time histograms show a clear distinction between when the chromatic and achromatic systems detect the stimulus. Our results are consistent with previous reports suggesting the achromatic system is more sensitive to higher temporal frequencies than the chromatic system.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Light , Photic Stimulation
15.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 22(1): 98-102, 1982 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7056628

ABSTRACT

Temporal contrast sensitivity functions were determined for the normal (20/20) and amblyopic eyes of five strabismic and/or anisometropic amblyopes and for both eyes of two nonamblyopes. In deep amblyopia (20/100+), contrast sensitivity was reduced at all temporal frequencies in the amblyopic eye, whereas no deficit was observed for subjects whose acuity was 20/40 or better. This result indicates that reduced temporal sensitivity is a significant component of strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/physiopathology , Optometry/instrumentation , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Adult , Amblyopia/diagnosis , Humans , Optometry/methods , Photic Stimulation/instrumentation , Photic Stimulation/methods , Strabismus/diagnosis , Strabismus/physiopathology
16.
Behav Brain Res ; 2(3): 323-34, 1981 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7225221

ABSTRACT

Visual acuity was determined for both eyes of long-term monocular-deprived cats over a wide luminance range. The influence of luminance on the rate of pattern vision recovery was also examined. Unlike strabismic humans and cats, the acuity deficit of monocularly deprived cats is not luminance dependent. This acuity is much worse at all luminance levels in the deprived eye. Likewise, initial testing of the deprived eye at low luminance levels does not facilitate recovery of pattern vision. Therefore, monocular lid fusion results in a visual deficit that differs from the effects of strabismus but resembles the effects of anisometropia in man.


Subject(s)
Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Retina/physiology , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Visual Acuity , Animals , Cats , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Female , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Psychophysics , Size Perception/physiology
17.
Exp Brain Res ; 43(1): 65-8, 1981.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7250258

ABSTRACT

Critical flicker fusion was determined over a wide luminance range for six Siamese and four normal cats. The Siamese cats had the lowest CFF at all luminance levels when compared to normal and binocularly deprived (BD) cats. The Siamese cat's inferior temporal resolution is most likely due to their profound y-cell loss because (1) CFF is proportional to y-cell population across normal, BD, and Siamese cats; and (2) unlike BD cats, the visual cortex of Siamese cats is comparatively normal. The implication of this finding for the CFF of dark-reared cats and normal cats viewing a stimulus which does not stimulate y-cell is discussed.


Subject(s)
Flicker Fusion/physiology , Retina/cytology , Animals , Cats , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Optic Nerve/physiology , Sensory Deprivation/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology
18.
Brain Res ; 183(1): 1-11, 1980 Feb 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7357396

ABSTRACT

Threshold visual acuity for three cats which were reared from birth to 4--12 months of age with bilateral lid closure was measured and compared to visual acuity in three cats which had the use of a non-deprived eye. The results indicate that binocular deprivation (BD) results in significant deficits in visual acuity which are proportional to the duration of deprivation. Threshold visual acuities were 3.7 cycles/deg. following 4 months of BD, 3.25 cycles/deg. following 7 months of BD and 2.55 cycles/deg. following 12 months of BD compared to acuities of 6.0, 6.5 and 6.8 cycles/deg9 for cats using a non-deprived eye. All BD cats had recovered from the initial visuomotor deficits, seen in these cats and reported in the literature, following lid-parting. The implication of such deficits in visual acuity on visual discrimination learning in BD cats is discussed.


Subject(s)
Sensory Deprivation , Visual Acuity , Animals , Cats , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Motor Skills/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Retina/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...