Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 36
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Memory ; 31(7): 1011-1018, 2023 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37160683

ABSTRACT

ABSTRACTCan exposure to a doctored photograph of a plausible yet fictitious childhood event create false memories in adults? Twenty years ago, (Wade, K. A., Garry, M., Don Read, J., & Lindsay, D. S. (2002). A picture is worth a thousand lies: Using false photographs to create false childhood memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 597-603) found that half of the participants reported false beliefs or memories after multiple interview sessions about a doctored photograph of themselves as children on a fictitious hot air balloon ride. In this replication, which rigorously recreated the method and procedure of Wade et al. (2002), participants were interviewed over three interview sessions using free recall and imagery techniques about three true and one fictitious childhood event photos. The balloon ride was modified to a culturally appropriate target event - a Viking ship ride - to ensure that the doctored photograph was functionally equivalent. The results showed almost identical patterns in the two studies: 40% (n = 8) of the participants reported partial or clear false beliefs or memories compared with 50% (n = 10) in the original study. The participants who reported false memories reported detailed and coherent memory narratives of the Viking ship ride not depicted in the doctored photograph. Our study successfully replicating the results of Wade et al. (2002), suggest that memories can relatively easily be implanted, regardless of cultural setting.


Subject(s)
Memory , Repression, Psychology , Adult , Child , Humans , Mental Recall , Narration
2.
Front Psychol ; 13: 835285, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35478762

ABSTRACT

There is agreement among researchers that no simple verbal cues to deception detectable by humans have been demonstrated. This paper examines the evidence for the most prominent current methods, critically considers the prevailing research strategy, proposes a taxonomy of lie detection methods and concludes that two common types of approach are unlikely to succeed. An approach to lie detection is advocated that derives both from psychological science and common sense: When an interviewee produces a statement that contradicts either a previous statement by the same person or other information the authorities have, it will in many cases be obvious to interviewer and interviewee that at least one of the statements is a lie and at the very least the credibility of the witness is reduced. The literature on Strategic Use of Evidence shows that features of interviews that foster such revelatory and self-trapping situations have been established to be a free account and the introduction of independent information late and gradually into the proceedings, and tactics based on these characteristics constitute the best current general advice for practitioners. If any other approach 1 day challenges this status quo, it is likely to be highly efficient automated systems.

3.
Psychiatr Psychol Law ; 28(5): 665-682, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35571598

ABSTRACT

Eyewitness evidence often plays a critical role in decisions made in the criminal justice system. To evaluate eyewitness testimony accurately, judges must be aware of factors that can contaminate this type of evidence. In 2008, a survey of judges in Norway revealed a lack of awareness of several factors that affect eyewitness testimony. In the current study, a survey was administered to Norwegian judges (N=98) to evaluate their knowledge of factors that affect eyewitness testimony. Results showed that judges' overall knowledge scores were similar to those reported in 2008, but substantial increases and decreases in knowledge were observed for specific factors. Additional analyses indicated that increased uncertainty regarding some eyewitness factors led to a decline in accuracy when compared to responses observed in 2008. The current study provides an updated assessment of judges' knowledge of eyewitness factors and highlights the need for more comprehensive training for judges regarding these factors.

4.
6.
Behav Sci Law ; 34(1): 113-25, 2016 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26990221

ABSTRACT

The emotional witness effect - the phenomenon whereby people are affected by the emotional manner in which a witness presents testimony - constitutes a possible source of wrongful decisions in legal contexts. One stereotypical view of abused children is that they should be sad when talking about their experiences of maltreatment, whereas children may in fact express a variety of emotional expressions when talking about abusive events. This raises the question as to whether there is an optimal mode in which to present child victim testimony that could reduce the possible influence of displayed emotions. In the present study, mock police interviews were carried out with female child actors, role-playing the victims of physical abuse by their stepfather, telling the same story with four emotional expressions (neutral, sad, angry, or positive). Laypersons (N = 465) were presented with the interviews as transcripts with the emotional reactions of the child witness noted, audio recordings, or videotaped recordings. Participants then rated the credibility of the victim witness. Replicating previous results, the "sad" expression elicited the highest credibility ratings across all modes of presentations. Presentation mode affected ratings of credibility, with the transcript versions resulting in the highest ratings. Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse, Sexual/psychology , Crime Victims/psychology , Criminal Law/methods , Emotions , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Criminal Law/standards , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Social Perception , Surveys and Questionnaires
7.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 143: 102-10, 2016 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26637948

ABSTRACT

Maltreated (n=26) and non-maltreated (n=31) 7- to 12-year-old children were tested on the Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM) false memory task using emotional and neutral word lists. True recall was significantly better for non-maltreated than maltreated children regardless of list valence. The proportion of false recall for neutral lists was comparable regardless of maltreatment status. However, maltreated children showed a significantly higher false recall rate for the emotional lists than non-maltreated children. Together, these results provide new evidence that maltreated children could be more prone to false memory illusions for negatively valenced information than their non-maltreated counterparts.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse/psychology , Emotions/physiology , Memory/physiology , Repression, Psychology , Child , Child Abuse/statistics & numerical data , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall/physiology
8.
Neuropsychologia ; 66: 1-9, 2015 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25445778

ABSTRACT

Retention of features in visual short-term memory (VSTM) involves maintenance of sensory traces in early visual cortex. However, the mechanism through which this is accomplished is not known. Here, we formulate specific hypotheses derived from studies on feature-based attention to test the prediction that visual cortex is recruited by attentional mechanisms during VSTM of low-level features. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) of human visual areas revealed that neural populations coding for task-irrelevant feature information are suppressed during maintenance of detailed spatial frequency memory representations. The narrow spectral extent of this suppression agrees well with known effects of feature-based attention. Additionally, analyses of effective connectivity during maintenance between retinotopic areas in visual cortex show that the observed highlighting of task-relevant parts of the feature spectrum originates in V4, a visual area strongly connected with higher-level control regions and known to convey top-down influence to earlier visual areas during attentional tasks. In line with this property of V4 during attentional operations, we demonstrate that modulations of earlier visual areas during memory maintenance have behavioral consequences, and that these modulations are a result of influences from V4.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Brain Mapping , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation
9.
J Vis ; 14(13): 16, 2014 Nov 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25406161

ABSTRACT

A frequently asked question concerns what a newborn infant can actually see. The contrast sensitivity function of newborn infants is well known, but its implications for the ability of newborns to perceive faces of adults remain unclear. We filtered gray scale animations of facial expressions in terms of both spatial frequency and contrast to correspond to the properties of newborn infants' acuity and showed them to adult participants. We reasoned that if adults were unable to identify the depicted facial expressions, then it would also seem unlikely that newborn infants could identify the same expressions. We found that for the simulated acuity the different expressions could be rather well identified at a distance of 30 cm, but when the distance was increased to 120 cm their discriminability was much degraded. This shows that although the perception of faces and facial expressions can function at the low visual resolution of the newborn infant, it is insufficient for distinguishing faces and facial expressions at moderate distances.


Subject(s)
Computer Simulation , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Facial Expression , Infant, Newborn/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
10.
Front Psychiatry ; 5: 102, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25165459

ABSTRACT

Eyewitness error is one of the leading causes of wrongful convictions. In fact, the American Psychological Association estimates that one in three eyewitnesses make an erroneous identification. In this review, we look briefly at some of the causes of eyewitness error. We examine what jurors, judges, attorneys, law officers, and experts from various countries know about eyewitness testimony and memory, and if they have the requisite knowledge and skills to accurately assess eyewitness testimony. We evaluate whether legal safeguards such as voir dire, motion-to-suppress an identification, cross-examination, jury instructions, and eyewitness expert testimony are effective in identifying eyewitness errors. Lastly, we discuss solutions to eyewitness error.

11.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 126: 357-68, 2014 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24997291

ABSTRACT

The accuracy and consistency of children's memories of their removals from their biological families by the Child Protective Services (CPS) was investigated. A researcher was present during the removals and documented what happened. A total of 37 maltreated children, aged 3 to 12 years, were interviewed 1 week and 3 months after the removals. The accuracy of the memory reports was high at both time points, but their consistency was fairly low; in all age groups (3-6, 7-10, and 11-12 years), a high percentage of new accurate information was reported during the second interview and a high percentage of the accurate information reported in the first interview was omitted in the second interview. Older children were significantly more consistent in their memory reports than younger children. The results show that low consistency in memory does not imply memory inaccuracy and has implications for the interpretation of successive interviews of children in forensic contexts.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse/psychology , Life Change Events , Mental Recall , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Interview, Psychological , Male , Memory, Episodic
12.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 25(11): 1944-56, 2013 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23691986

ABSTRACT

Visual STM of simple features is achieved through interactions between retinotopic visual cortex and a set of frontal and parietal regions. In the present fMRI study, we investigated effective connectivity between central nodes in this network during the different task epochs of a modified delayed orientation discrimination task. Our univariate analyses demonstrate that the inferior frontal junction (IFJ) is preferentially involved in memory encoding, whereas activity in the putative FEFs and anterior intraparietal sulcus (aIPS) remains elevated throughout periods of memory maintenance. We have earlier reported, using the same task, that areas in visual cortex sustain information about task-relevant stimulus properties during delay intervals [Sneve, M. H., Alnæs, D., Endestad, T., Greenlee, M. W., & Magnussen, S. Visual short-term memory: Activity supporting encoding and maintenance in retinotopic visual cortex. Neuroimage, 63, 166-178, 2012]. To elucidate the temporal dynamics of the IFJ-FEF-aIPS-visual cortex network during memory operations, we estimated Granger causality effects between these regions with fMRI data representing memory encoding/maintenance as well as during memory retrieval. We also investigated a set of control conditions involving active processing of stimuli not associated with a memory task and passive viewing. In line with the developing understanding of IFJ as a region critical for control processes with a possible initiating role in visual STM operations, we observed influence from IFJ to FEF and aIPS during memory encoding. Furthermore, FEF predicted activity in a set of higher-order visual areas during memory retrieval, a finding consistent with its suggested role in top-down biasing of sensory cortex.


Subject(s)
Frontal Lobe/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Mental Recall/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Young Adult
13.
Front Psychiatry ; 4: 28, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23720639

ABSTRACT

We surveyed 100 Italian defense attorneys about their knowledge and beliefs about factors affecting eyewitness accuracy. The results of similar surveys show that U.S. defense attorneys were significantly more knowledgeable than other legal professionals, including U.S. prosecutors and U.S. and European judges. The present survey of Italian defense attorneys produced similar results. However, the results suggest that the defense attorney's superior performance may be due at least in part to their skepticism of eyewitness testimony rather than their greater knowledge of eyewitness factors.

14.
Neuroimage ; 63(1): 166-78, 2012 Oct 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22776452

ABSTRACT

Recent studies have demonstrated that retinotopic cortex maintains information about visual stimuli during retention intervals. However, the process by which transient stimulus-evoked sensory responses are transformed into enduring memory representations is unknown. Here, using fMRI and short-term visual memory tasks optimized for univariate and multivariate analysis approaches, we report differential involvement of human retinotopic areas during memory encoding of the low-level visual feature orientation. All visual areas show weaker responses when memory encoding processes are interrupted, possibly due to effects in orientation-sensitive primary visual cortex (V1) propagating across extrastriate areas. Furthermore, intermediate areas in both dorsal (V3a/b) and ventral (LO1/2) streams are significantly more active during memory encoding compared with non-memory (active and passive) processing of the same stimulus material. These effects in intermediate visual cortex are also observed during memory encoding of a different stimulus feature (spatial frequency), suggesting that these areas are involved in encoding processes on a higher level of representation. Using pattern-classification techniques to probe the representational content in visual cortex during delay periods, we further demonstrate that simply initiating memory encoding is not sufficient to produce long-lasting memory traces. Rather, active maintenance appears to underlie the observed memory-specific patterns of information in retinotopic cortex.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Nerve Net/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
15.
Soc Neurosci ; 7(2): 202-16, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21787246

ABSTRACT

People are generally better at remembering faces of their own race than faces of a different race, and this effect is known as the own-race bias (ORB) effect. We used eye-tracking and pupillometry to investigate whether Caucasian and Asian face stimuli elicited different-looking patterns in Caucasian participants in a face-memory task. Consistent with the ORB effect, we found better recognition performance for own-race faces than other-race faces, and shorter response times. In addition, at encoding, eye movements and pupillary responses to Asian faces (i.e., the other race) were different from those to Caucasian faces (i.e., the own race). Processing of own-race faces was characterized by more active scanning, with a larger number of shorter fixations, and more frequent saccades. Moreover, pupillary diameters were larger when viewing other-race than own-race faces, suggesting a greater cognitive effort when encoding other-race faces.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Face , Prejudice , Pupil/physiology , Racial Groups , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adult , Asian People , Calibration , Databases, Factual , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Nose/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , White People , Young Adult
16.
PLoS One ; 6(4): e18651, 2011 Apr 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21525988

ABSTRACT

Neurons in the primary visual cortex, V1, are specialized for the processing of elemental features of the visual stimulus, such as orientation and spatial frequency. Recent fMRI evidence suggest that V1 neurons are also recruited in visual perceptual memory; a number of studies using multi-voxel pattern analysis have successfully decoded stimulus-specific information from V1 activity patterns during the delay phase in memory tasks. However, consistent fMRI signal modulations reflecting the memory process have not yet been demonstrated. Here, we report evidence, from three subjects, that the low V1 BOLD activity during retention of low-level visual features is caused by competing interactions between neural populations coding for different values along the spectrum of the dimension remembered. We applied a memory masking paradigm in which the memory representation of a masker stimulus interferes with a delayed spatial frequency discrimination task when its frequency differs from the discriminanda with ±1 octave and found that impaired behavioral performance due to masking is reflected in weaker V1 BOLD signals. This cross-channel inhibition in V1 only occurs with retinotopic overlap between the masker and the sample stimulus of the discrimination task. The results suggest that memory for spatial frequency is a local process in the retinotopically organized visual cortex.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Behavior/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging
17.
Brain Struct Funct ; 214(4): 355-9, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20237799

ABSTRACT

Previous literature suggest that processing of visually presented shapes and textures starts in the early visual areas, but subsequently follow different pathways. The purpose of this experiment was to further investigate differential activation for shapes and textures in order elucidate the pathways involved in visual shape and texture matching. In the present study, brain areas involved in discrimination of shapes and textures are mapped, using the same set of stimuli for shape and texture decisions. Texture matching activates more prefrontal regions than shape matching, particularly regions in the left middle frontal gyrus and bilateral inferior frontal gyrus. Shape specific activation includes an occipital/temporal region which is associated with multimodal object matching. The pattern of results suggests that recognition of textures may be based upon different ordering conditions in memory, which involve a prefrontal network and require a great deal more workload than the holistic representation of shape.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Visual Cortex/blood supply , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxygen/blood , Photic Stimulation/methods , Young Adult
18.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 105(3): 156-77, 2010 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19969304

ABSTRACT

A critical issue for developmental psychology is how to obtain accurate and complete eyewitness memory reports from preschoolers without offering suggestions that might result in false allegations. We examined effects of two interviewing strategies (police/verbal interviews and clinician/prop-assisted interviews) on young children's reports about a medical examination. A total of 58 4-year-olds participated in the study, which conformed to a 2 (Interview Type)x2 (Number of Interviews) factorial design. Analyses revealed that interviewers spent less time off topic and asked more free recall questions in the police/verbal interviews than in the clinician/prop-assisted interviews. Compared with police/verbal interviews, clinician/prop-assisted interviews resulted in significantly more correct rejections and commission errors in children's memory reports. However, on a final free recall test, error rates were comparable across conditions. Higher child verbal intelligence predicted memory accuracy in police/verbal interviews, and greater parental attachment anxiety predicted children being asked a higher number of misleading questions. The study provides new insights into interview techniques that promote preschoolers' accurate eyewitness reports.


Subject(s)
Forensic Psychiatry/methods , Interview, Psychological , Memory , Professional-Patient Relations , Repression, Psychology , Child , Child, Preschool , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male , Suggestion
19.
Scand J Psychol ; 50(6): 535-42, 2009 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19930252

ABSTRACT

Psychophysical studies of the short-term memory for attributes or dimensions of the visual stimulus that are important in online perceptual processing of visual images - spatial frequency, orientation, contrast, motion - identify a low-level perceptual memory mechanism. This mechanism is located early in the visual processing stream, prior to the structural description system, but beyond the primary visual cortex (V1); it is composed of a series of parallel, special-purpose perceptual mechanisms with independent but limited processing resources. Each mechanism is dedicated to the analysis of a single attribute or dimension of the visual stimulus, and is coupled to a memory store. The evidence for this model from psychophysical and recent functional brain imaging studies is reviewed.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Brain Mapping , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Humans , Models, Neurological
20.
Exp Brain Res ; 188(3): 363-9, 2008 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18425507

ABSTRACT

The concept of perceptual memory refers to the neural and cognitive processes underlying the storage of specific stimulus features such as spatial frequency, orientation, shape, contrast, and color. Psychophysical studies of perceptual memory indicate that observers can retain visual information about the spatial frequency of Gabor patterns independent of the orientation with which they are presented. Compared to discrimination of gratings with the same orientation, reaction times to orthogonally oriented gratings, however, increase suggesting additional processing. Using event-related fMRI we examined the pattern of neural activation evoked when subjects discriminated the spatial frequency of Gabors presented with the same or orthogonal orientation. Blood-oxygen level dependent BOLD fMRI revealed significantly elevated bilateral activity in visual areas (V1, V2) when the gratings to be compared had an orthogonal orientation, compared to when they had the same orientation. These findings suggest that a change in an irrelevant stimulus dimension requires additional processing in primary and secondary visual areas. The finding that the task-irrelevant stimulus property (orientation) had no significant effect on the prefrontal and intraparietal cortex supports a model of working memory in which discrimination and retention of basic stimulus dimensions is based on low-level perceptual memory stores that are located at an early stage in the visual process. Our findings suggest that accessing different stores requires time and has higher metabolic costs.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Functional Laterality , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Memory/physiology , Orientation , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Fingers/physiology , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Reference Values , Vision, Ocular
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...