Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Mem Cognit ; 2024 Jul 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39023698

ABSTRACT

Research is lacking regarding adults' ability to determine whether children's drawings are based on an experience or not. Drawings are useful in professional settings to alleviate linguistic demands, facilitate memory, and have been used as evidence. Determining the accuracy of veracity assessments of children's drawings would inform professionals regarding their use as evidence of experiences. Twenty-eight children (14 younger, Mage = 7.53 years, SDage = 1.19; 14 older, Mage = 11.67 years, SD = 1.27) produced drawings of two events: one staged experienced, and one narrative-based not experienced event. Fifty (Study 1, Mage = 23.72 years, SDage = 9.70) and 63 (Study 2, Mage = 25.92, SDage = 12.79) adults indicated whether each drawing was based on experience and their confidence in each assessment. In Study 2, additional drawing quality assessments were collected. Results indicated that adults were more accurate at distinguishing experienced than not experienced drawings for older artists. An inverse relationship was observed between confidence and accuracy-participants were more confident when they were inaccurate, especially for younger artists. Drawing quality improved with age and for drawings of experienced events. Adults tended to rate drawings of higher quality as resulting from experience leading to the highest accuracy for drawings from older artists that were based on experience. Overall, results suggest that there may be some features of drawings that allow for above chance levels of accuracy (up to 75%). However, rates are not high enough across assessments (M = 53.93%, range: 39%-75%) to reliably use them as indicators of experience.

2.
J Pediatr Psychol ; 41(2): 220-31, 2016 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26666267

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Given that forgetting negative experiences can help children cope with these experiences, we examined their ability to forget negative aspects of painful events. METHODS: 86 children aged 7-15 years participated in a retrieval-induced forgetting task whereby they repeatedly retrieved positive details of a physically painful experience, and an experimental pain task (cold-pressor task). RESULTS: Repeatedly retrieving positive details of a prior pain experience produced forgetting of the negative aspects of that experience. Pain-related self-efficacy predicted retrieval-induced forgetting; children with a poorer belief in their ability to cope with pain experienced less forgetting. Children who had a more difficult time forgetting prior negative experiences were more anxious about the pain task and reported higher pain thresholds. CONCLUSIONS: Understanding children's memory for painful experiences may help improve their pain management and coping ability.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Emotions , Inhibition, Psychological , Mental Recall , Pain/psychology , Retention, Psychology , Adolescent , Association Learning , Attention , Attitude , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Pain Threshold
3.
Mem Cognit ; 40(6): 902-17, 2012 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22371165

ABSTRACT

Although high levels of phantom recollection (illusory vivid experience of the prior "presentation" of unpresented items) have been found for false recognition, little is known about phantom recollection in recall. We examined this issue with Deese/Roediger-McDermott lists using two paradigms: repeated recall and conjoint recall. High levels of phantom recollection were observed with both standard behavioral measures and the parameters of fuzzy-trace theory's dual-recall model. In addition, phantom recollection and the true recollection that accompanies presented items appear to involve different retrieval processes, because they were dissociated by manipulations such as number of recall tests and list strength.


Subject(s)
Illusions/psychology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adult , Humans , Models, Theoretical , Psycholinguistics/methods , Psychological Tests , Psychological Theory , Young Adult
4.
Pain Res Manag ; 14(3): 233-7, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19547763

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To examine the effects of temperament and trait anxiety on memory for pain. METHODS: Three dimensions of temperament, as well as trait anxiety, were assessed in 36 children (five to 12 years of age) undergoing dental procedures; after the procedure, the children provided pain ratings. Following a six- to eight-week delay, the children reported how much pain they remembered. RESULTS: Most children (85%) accurately recalled their pain. Temperament had no significant effect, but trait-anxious children showed a greater likelihood of recalling more pain than they initially reported, suggesting that they may negatively distort recollections of painful experiences. CONCLUSIONS: When treating children, in particular trait-anxious children, clinicians should consider what children remember as part of pain management intervention.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/physiopathology , Anxiety/psychology , Mental Recall , Pain/psychology , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Pain Measurement , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Statistics as Topic , Temperament
5.
Memory ; 17(5): 518-27, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19468958

ABSTRACT

Past research has demonstrated that cognitive triage (weak-strong-weak recall pattern) is a robust effect that optimises children's recall. The aim of the current research was to determine whether adults' free recall also exhibits triage and whether cognitive triage is less marked with older than younger adults' recall. Younger and older adults memorized 16 unrelated words until all items were recalled perfectly. The triage pattern existed for both the younger and older adults' recall and there was evidence for age differences in triage. Our results are consistent with claims of greater verbatim forgetting and increased susceptibility to output interference with age in adulthood. Further research is needed to determine whether fuzzy-trace theory adequately explains the ageing of triage and what factors play a role in the development of this pattern of recall in adulthood.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Retention, Psychology/physiology , Adolescent , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Language , Male , Models, Psychological , Statistics as Topic , Young Adult
6.
Memory ; 14(1): 17-26, 2006 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16423738

ABSTRACT

Participants who witness an event and later receive post-event information that omits a critical scene are less likely to recall and to recognise that scene than are participants who receive no post-event information (Wright, Loftus, & Hall, 2001). The present study used the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm, in which participants study lists of semantic associates (e.g., hot, snow, warm, winter) that commonly elicit false memories of critical non-presented words (e.g., cold), to determine whether omitting information from a second presentation decreases memory for both presented and non-presented information. Participants were presented with a list of the semantic associates of six non-presented words. For half the participants, this list was presented a second time with the semantic associates of one of the non-presented words omitted. As expected, participants were less likely to recall and to recognise the presented words when they had been omitted from the second presentation. Omission also decreased the rate at which non-presented words were recalled, although false recognition of these words was not reduced. These results suggest that false recognition may be particularly difficult to attenuate and that post-event omission may be more detrimental to memory accuracy than previously thought.


Subject(s)
Mental Recall , Reinforcement, Psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Association , Cognition , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Memory, Short-Term , Models, Psychological , Psychological Tests , Recognition, Psychology , Semantics , Thinking
7.
Memory ; 13(7): 749-58, 2005 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16261695

ABSTRACT

Directed forgetting may reduce DRM false memory illusion by interfering with meaning processing. Participants were presented with a list composed of six 10-word semantically associated sub-lists, and they were either (a) asked to remember all list items of (b) asked to remember all associates from sub-lists and to forget all associates from other sub-lists. All participants were requested to recall and recognise list items. Although directed forgetting effects have been previously reported only for true responses in the DRM paradigm with the list method, we also found directed forgetting for false responses with the item method. Such forgetting instructions reduced both verbatim and meaning processing, decreasing both the intrusion and the false alarm rate. These results are consistent with two-process explanations of DRM false memories, such as fuzzy-trace theory, and add to our understanding of false memory editing.


Subject(s)
Repression, Psychology , Adult , Humans , Illusions , Psychological Tests , Recognition, Psychology
8.
J Pain ; 5(5): 241-9, 2004 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15219255

ABSTRACT

UNLABELLED: Children's memories of painful experiences can have long-term consequences for their reaction to later painful events and their acceptance of later health care interventions. This review surveys research on children's memory for pain, emphasizing implications for clinical practice. Topics reviewed include consequences of children's memories of pain; the development of memory; differences between explicit (declarative, verbal, autobiographic) memory and implicit (nondeclarative, nonverbal) memory; and individual differences, situational, and methodologic factors affecting memories of pain. Methods to prevent the adverse consequences of remembered pain are addressed with reference to current research on editing or reframing memories. PERSPECTIVE: This review covers topics of value to clinicians providing care to children undergoing painful procedures. Specific recommendations are offered regarding the importance of acknowledging and assessing children's previous memories of painful experiences, the type of information that benefits children before and after procedures, and the most appropriate questioning strategies. It might be possible to prevent or reduce the adverse effects of memories of pain.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Pain/physiopathology , Pain/psychology , Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Child , Humans , Individuality , Pain/prevention & control
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...