Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol ; 62(7): 2045-2062, 2018 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29237316

ABSTRACT

Is the relationship between criminal thinking and recidivism the same for criminal justice-involved individuals from varying demographic backgrounds? Relying on two independent samples of offenders and two measures of criminal thinking, the current studies examined whether four demographic factors-gender, race, age, and education-moderated the relationship between criminal thinking and recidivism. Study 1 consisted of 226 drug-involved probationers enrolled in a randomized clinical trial. Study 2 consisted of 346 jail inmates from a longitudinal study. Logistic regression models suggested that the strength of the relationship between criminal thinking and subsequent recidivism did not vary based on participant demographics, regardless of justice system setting or measure of criminal thinking. Criminal thinking predicts recidivism similarly for people who are male, female, Black, White, older, younger, and more or less educated.


Subject(s)
Criminals/psychology , Recidivism , Adult , Age Factors , Educational Status , Female , Humans , Male , Race Factors , Sex Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires
2.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 40(12): 1659-67, 2014 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25324328

ABSTRACT

We describe and appraise a theoretical model in which individual differences in perspective-taking eventuate in crime reduction. Specifically, it is hypothesized that perspective-taking propensities influence the tendency to feel empathic-concern, thereby heightening proneness for guilt, which ultimately inhibits criminal behavior (perspective-taking → empathic-concern → guilt-proneness → crime desistance). Data from two sources were analyzed: (a) a cross-sectional college sample and (b) a longitudinal sample of jail inmates. Overall, results lend credence to this theoretical model: Perspective-taking propensities ultimately "put the brakes" on criminal behavior-via an emotional pathway of empathic-concern and then guilt-proneness. Discussion focuses on the nature of perspective-taking, its generative role for moral emotion and behavior, as well as potential applications for crime reduction.


Subject(s)
Crime/prevention & control , Crime/psychology , Empathy , Guilt , Self Concept , Adult , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Individuality , Male , Morals , Prisoners/psychology , Psychometrics , Students/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
3.
Psychol Sci ; 25(3): 799-805, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24395738

ABSTRACT

Psychological research using mostly cross-sectional methods calls into question the presumed function of shame as an inhibitor of immoral or illegal behavior. In a longitudinal study of 476 jail inmates, we assessed shame proneness, guilt proneness, and externalization of blame shortly after incarceration. We interviewed participants (N = 332) 1 year after release into the community, and we accessed official arrest records (N = 446). Guilt proneness negatively and directly predicted reoffense in the 1st year after release; shame proneness did not. Further mediational modeling showed that shame proneness positively predicted recidivism via its robust link to externalization of blame. There remained a direct effect of shame on recidivism: Unimpeded by defensive externalization of blame, shame inhibited recidivism. Items assessing a motivation to hide were primarily responsible for this pattern. Overall, our results suggest that the pain of shame may have two faces-one with destructive potential and the other with constructive potential.


Subject(s)
Crime/psychology , Prisoners/psychology , Self Concept , Shame , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Antisocial Personality Disorder/psychology , Crime/statistics & numerical data , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Guilt , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Personality Inventory , Prisoners/statistics & numerical data , Psychometrics , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
4.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 103(6): 949-962, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22889070

ABSTRACT

Chaotic conditions are a prevalent and threatening feature of social life. Five studies examined whether social class underlies divergent responses to perceptions of chaos in one's social environments and outcomes. The authors hypothesized that when coping with perceptions of chaos, lower class individuals tend to prioritize community, relative to upper class individuals, who instead tend to prioritize material wealth. Consistent with these predictions, when personally confronting chaos, lower class individuals were more communally oriented (Study 1), more connected with their community (Study 2), and more likely to volunteer for a community-building project (Study 3), compared to upper class individuals. In contrast, perceptions of chaos caused upper class individuals to express greater reliance on wealth (Study 4) and prefer financial gain over membership in a close-knit community (Study 5), relative to lower class individuals. These findings suggest that social class shapes how people respond to perceptions of chaos and cope with its threatening consequences.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological/classification , Social Class , Social Perception , Social Support , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Income/classification , Male , Middle Aged , Nonlinear Dynamics , Psychological Tests , Residence Characteristics , Socioeconomic Factors , Stress, Psychological/psychology , Young Adult
5.
Cogn Emot ; 26(4): 634-49, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21827331

ABSTRACT

People can experience great distress when a group to which they belong (in-group) is perceived to have committed an immoral act. We hypothesised that people would direct hostility toward a transgressing in-group whose actions threaten their self-image and evoke collective shame. Consistent with this theorising, three studies found that reminders of in-group transgression provoked several expressions of in-group-directed hostility, including in-group-directed hostile emotion (Studies 1 and 2), in-group-directed derogation (Study 2), and in-group-directed punishment (Study 3). Across studies, collective shame-but not the related group-based emotion collective guilt-mediated the relationship between in-group transgression and in-group-directed hostility. Implications for group-based emotion, social identity, and group behaviour are discussed.


Subject(s)
Complicity , Hostility , Shame , Social Behavior , Social Identification , Adult , Female , Guilt , Humans , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...