Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 11 de 11
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 203: 107798, 2023 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37422207

ABSTRACT

Social buffering is a phenomenon in which the stress response of an individual can be reduced by the presence of another individual. However, little is known about the effect of social buffering on aversive after memory extinction, especially when animals are tested alone afterwards. The aim of this study was to verify the social buffering effect in rats during the extinction session of the contextual fear conditioning model and the fear response when animals are tested alone in the following day. Animals were divided into subjects and associates, with the subjects undergoing the fear conditioning protocol and the associates paired with the subjects during the fear extinction session. Across five different experiments, we tested moderate and high intensity contextual fear conditioning protocols, as well four variations of pairs: (i) two conditioned subjects, (ii) a conditioned subject and a non-conditioned associate, (iii) a conditioned subject and an associate who observed the conditioning of the partner and (iv) two conditioned subjects, with one treated with diazepam. The social buffering effect was found efficient to reduce the fear memory expression during the fear extinction session. In the moderate intensity protocol, the reduction in freezing time occurred only in subjects accompanied by non-conditioned associates and observer associates. In the high intensity protocol, the social buffering effect occurred in subjects accompanied by either conditioned or non-conditioned associates, although the effect was more evident in the presence of non-conditioned subjects. Treatment of the conditioned associates with diazepam did not improve the social buffering effect. Moreover, social buffering effects were not correlated with self-grooming or prosocial behaviors, which indicates that the presence of another animal might decrease freezing by promotion of exploratory activity. Finally, the social buffering effect was not observed in the extinction test, either because the extinction was too effective in the moderate intensity protocol or because the extinction was equally ineffective in the high intensity protocol. Our results suggest that social buffering does not improve fear extinction consolidation.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Classical , Conditioning, Psychological , Rats , Animals , Rats, Wistar , Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Extinction, Psychological/physiology , Social Behavior , Fear/physiology
2.
Neurobiol Learn Mem ; 190: 107602, 2022 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35202815

ABSTRACT

Trait anxiety is a form of chronic state anxiety that can be part of the individual's personality and one of the individual factors that increase the risk of developing PTSD. To be able to distinguish between trait and state anxiety in animal models might be challenging, since the tests themselves are often anxiogenic. One possible approach is the use of the free exploratory paradigm (FEP), a model that consists of providing the animal the choice to explore both a familiar and a novel environment. High trait anxiety animals choose to explore more the familiar environment. Thus, the objective of this study was to evaluate if differences in trait anxiety could lead to impairments in acquisition and extinction of fear memory in a contextual fear conditioning protocol. First, Swiss mice were divided into high trait anxiety (HTA) or low trait anxiety (LTA) based on their exploration of the novel environment (%TNS) in the FEP. We observed that the %TNS was stable across three testing sessions. Also, we observed that freezing behavior was not different between HTA and LTA mice in a retrieval session 1 day after the conditioning, which indicates that acquisition was not impaired. However, HTA presented higher freezing time during extinction training and test. Also, HTA presented higher freezing in reinstatement test, but this might be related to the poor extinction learning. Moreover, diazepam (0.25 or 1.0 mg/kg, i.p.) did not prevent the differences in extinction training and test when administer 30 min before conditioning training or 30 min before extinction training, even though diazepam (1.0 mg/kg) had anxiolytic effect in the elevated plus-maze. These results demonstrate that HTA mice presented increase in freezing during extinction training and test, as well as during reinstatement. These results indicate that increased freezing time in HTA mice is not explained by high state anxiety in a specific phase of the fear conditioning, but more likely, the overall high trait anxiety throughout the entire experiment.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Psychological , Extinction, Psychological , Animals , Anxiety , Diazepam , Fear , Mice
3.
Sci Rep ; 7: 40399, 2017 01 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28071711

ABSTRACT

Industrialisation greatly increased human night-time exposure to artificial light, which in animal models is a known cause of depressive phenotypes. Whilst many of these phenotypes are 'direct' effects of light on affect, an 'indirect' pathway via altered sleep-wake timing has been suggested. We have previously shown that the Period3 gene, which forms part of the biological clock, is associated with altered sleep-wake patterns in response to light. Here, we show that both wild-type and Per3-/- mice showed elevated levels of circulating corticosterone and increased hippocampal Bdnf expression after 3 weeks of exposure to dim light at night, but only mice deficient for the PERIOD3 protein (Per3-/-) exhibited a transient anhedonia-like phenotype, observed as reduced sucrose preference, in weeks 2-3 of dim light at night, whereas WT mice did not. Per3-/- mice also exhibited a significantly smaller delay in behavioural timing than WT mice during weeks 1, 2 and 4 of dim light at night exposure. When treated with imipramine, neither Per3-/- nor WT mice exhibited an anhedonia-like phenotype, and neither genotypes exhibited a delay in behavioural timing in responses to dLAN. While the association between both Per3-/- phenotypes remains unclear, both are alleviated by imipramine treatment during dim night-time light.


Subject(s)
Circadian Clocks/genetics , Circadian Rhythm/genetics , Period Circadian Proteins/genetics , Sleep Wake Disorders/drug therapy , Anhedonia/drug effects , Animals , Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor/genetics , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Gene Expression Regulation/drug effects , Hippocampus/metabolism , Hippocampus/physiology , Humans , Imipramine/administration & dosage , Mice , Mice, Knockout , Motor Activity/genetics , Motor Activity/physiology , Photoperiod , Sleep/genetics , Sleep/physiology , Sleep Wake Disorders/genetics , Sleep Wake Disorders/physiopathology
4.
Inflamm Res ; 66(4): 311-322, 2017 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27904941

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Although TRPA1, SP, histamine and 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) have recognized contribution to nociceptive mechanisms, little is known about how they interact with each other to mediate inflammatory pain in vivo. In this study we evaluated whether TRPA1, SP, histamine and 5-HT interact, in an interdependent way, to induce nociception in vivo. METHODS AND RESULTS: The subcutaneous injection of the TRPA1 agonist allyl isothiocyanate (AITC) into the rat's hind paw induced a dose-dependent and short lasting behavioral nociceptive response that was blocked by the co-administration of the TRPA1 antagonist, HC030031, or by the pretreatment with antisense ODN against TRPA1. AITC-induced nociception was significantly decreased by the co-administration of selective antagonists for the NK1 receptor for substance P, the H1 receptor for histamine and the 5-HT1A or 3 receptors for 5-HT. Histamine- or 5-HT-induced nociception was decreased by the pretreatment with antisense ODN against TRPA1. These findings suggest that AITC-induced nociception depends on substance P, histamine and 5-HT, while histamine- or 5-HT-induced nociception depends on TRPA1. Most important, AITC interact in a synergistic way with histamine, 5-HT or substance P, since their combination at non-nociceptive doses induced a nociceptive response much higher than that expected by the sum of the effect of each one alone. This synergistic effect is dependent on the H1, 5-HT1A or 3 receptors. CONCLUSION: Together, these findings suggest a self-sustainable cycle around TRPA1, no matter where the cycle is initiated each step is achieved and even subeffective activation of more than one step results in a synergistic activation of the overall cycle.


Subject(s)
Histamine/metabolism , Pain/metabolism , Serotonin/metabolism , Substance P/metabolism , TRPC Cation Channels/metabolism , Acetanilides/pharmacology , Animals , Histamine H1 Antagonists/pharmacology , Isothiocyanates , Male , Oligonucleotides, Antisense/pharmacology , Pain/chemically induced , Piperazines/pharmacology , Purines/pharmacology , Pyrilamine/pharmacology , Quinuclidines/pharmacology , Rats, Wistar , Receptor, Serotonin, 5-HT1A/metabolism , Receptors, Histamine H1/metabolism , Receptors, Neurokinin-1/metabolism , Receptors, Serotonin, 5-HT3/metabolism , Serotonin Antagonists/pharmacology , TRPA1 Cation Channel , TRPC Cation Channels/agonists , TRPC Cation Channels/antagonists & inhibitors , TRPC Cation Channels/genetics , p-Methoxy-N-methylphenethylamine/pharmacology
5.
CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets ; 14(8): 963-9, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26350335

ABSTRACT

Disturbances in the circadian rhythms have long been associated with depression and mania. Animal models of mania and depression exhibit differential effects upon the intrinsic circadian period and the same occurs with antidepressants and mood stabilizers treatment. The intrinsic circadian period is expressed when there are no time clues or when the light/dark cycle length is beyond the capacity of synchronization. In summary, while there is no clear association between the circadian period and mania, depressive-like behaviour is generally associated either with lengthening of the circadian period or with arrythmicity, and the improvement of depressive-like behaviour is associated with shortening of the circadian period. Thus, this review is an attempt to summarize data regarding these correlations and find a putative role of the circadian intrinsic period in mood regulation, particularly concerning the switch from depression to mania.


Subject(s)
Bipolar Disorder/drug therapy , Bipolar Disorder/physiopathology , Circadian Rhythm/drug effects , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Depressive Disorder/drug therapy , Depressive Disorder/physiopathology , Animals , Antidepressive Agents/pharmacology , Antidepressive Agents/therapeutic use , Antimanic Agents/pharmacology , Antimanic Agents/therapeutic use , Humans
6.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 232(15): 2731-9, 2015 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25797192

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: We hypothesized that the corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) system is hyperresponsive in animals with high ethanol intake, which exhibits a reduction of ethanol intake when administered with a CRF1 receptor antagonist. METHODS: Outbred Swiss mice were subjected to a long-term, three-bottle, free-choice paradigm (5 and 10 % [v/v] ethanol and water) that consisted of four phases: acquisition (AC; 10 weeks), withdrawal (W; 2 weeks), reexposure (RE; 2 weeks), and quinine-adulteration (AD; 2 weeks). Based on individual ethanol intake, the mice were classified into three groups: A group, preference for ethanol and persistently high consumption during AD phase; B group, preference for ethanol and a reduction of ethanol intake in the AD phase; and C group; preference for water during all phases. A control group only had access to water. CRF1 receptor messenger RNA (mRNA) levels in the amygdala and the effect of the CRF1 receptor antagonist CP-154,526 on ethanol and water intake in the subgroups were studied. RESULTS: CRF1 transcript levels were higher in the B group than in the control group. The highest dose of CP-154,526 reduced ethanol intake and preference, with no changes in water consumption, in the A group compared with vehicle. The B group exhibited a reduction of both ethanol and water intake, with no changes in preference. The C group exhibited no changes in response to the CRF1 antagonist. CONCLUSIONS: CRF1 receptors appear to be involved in ethanol consumption in mice with high ethanol consumption, and CRF system-mediated neuroadaptations depend on drinking profiles.


Subject(s)
Alcohol Drinking , Amygdala/metabolism , Choice Behavior/drug effects , Ethanol/administration & dosage , Pyrimidines/pharmacology , Pyrroles/pharmacology , Receptors, Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone/antagonists & inhibitors , Animals , Drinking/drug effects , Male , Mice , Receptors, Corticotropin-Releasing Hormone/metabolism
7.
Neurosci Lett ; 588: 7-11, 2015 Feb 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25545554

ABSTRACT

Circadian rhythm disruptions are often observed in depressed patients, and changes in the light/dark cycle promote depressive-like behavior in animal models. Prolonged exposure to constant light (LL) is known to lead to arrhythmicity of circadian locomotor activity and depressive-like behavior in rats. Interestingly, neonatal exposure to LL prevents both arrhythmicity and depressive behavior in adulthood. Arrhythmic rats under LL conditions that cohabitate with a rhythmic rat exhibit improvement in circadian rhythms. We tested whether such cohabitation also protects against LL-induced depressive-like behavior. Wistar rats were assigned to conditions of either neonatal constant light (neonatal-LL) on postnatal days 10-22 or a regular light/dark cycle (neonatal-LD). On day 45, the animals were assigned to three possible pair combinations. After a baseline sucrose preference test, half of the pairs were placed under LL conditions. Weekly sucrose preference tests were conducted to evaluate depressive-like behavior. The animals were isolated by an aluminum wall on the test day. At week 2 of LL, sucrose preference was reduced in neonatal-LD/neonatal-LD pairs of animals. At week 5, neonatal-LD/neonatal-LD pairs exhibited anhedonic-like behavior, but the pairs with at least one neonatal-LL rat did not. The LL cycle was returned to an LD cycle, and the neonatal-LD/neonatal-LD pairs exhibited a restoration of sucrose preference 2 weeks later. We conclude that social interaction can prevent depressive-like behavior induced by circadian rhythm disruption as long as one of the animals is more prone to present a strong rhythm.


Subject(s)
Depression/psychology , Lactation , Light , Social Behavior , Age Factors , Animals , Circadian Rhythm , Depression/prevention & control , Female , Male , Rats, Wistar
8.
Neurosci Lett ; 500(2): 95-8, 2011 Aug 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21699957

ABSTRACT

Tamoxifen, a protein kinase C (PKC) inhibitor and antiestrogenic drug, has clinical antimanic effects and blocks psychostimulant-induced hyperlocomotion. Medroxyprogesterone (MPA), which has antiestrogenic effects, also exerts some clinical benefits in female manic patients and partially blocks amphetamine-induced hyperlocomotion, indicating that the antiestrogenic effect of tamoxifen could contribute to its antimanic effect. The present study evaluated the effect of acute and chronic (21 day) treatment of two antiestrogenic drugs, MPA and clomiphene (an estrogenic receptor antagonist), on methylphenidate (MPH, 5.0mg/kg)-induced hyperlocomotion in mice, an animal model of mania. Acute and chronic tamoxifen administration was used as a positive control. Acute and chronic tamoxifen (1.0mg/kg) administration blocked MPH-induced hyperlocomotion. Acute and chronic MPA (acute: 3.0 or 6.0mg/kg; chronic: 3.0mg/kg) and clomiphene (acute: 1.5 or 3.0mg/kg; chronic: 1.5mg/kg) treatment did not alter MPH-induced hyperlocomotion. These results indicate that tamoxifen exerts antimanic-like effects, and reduced estrogenic activity does not have antimanic-like effects in this psychostimulant-induced hyperlocomotion model. Therefore, the antiestrogenic effect of tamoxifen likely does not contribute to its antimanic effect, which may instead be related to its effect on PKC activity. Therefore, PKC inhibition may be associated with the antimanic effect of mood stabilizers.


Subject(s)
Antimanic Agents/pharmacology , Clomiphene/pharmacology , Estrogen Receptor Modulators/pharmacology , Medroxyprogesterone/pharmacology , Protein Kinase C/antagonists & inhibitors , Tamoxifen/pharmacology , Animals , Central Nervous System Stimulants/pharmacology , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Estrogen Antagonists/pharmacology , Male , Methylphenidate/pharmacology , Mice , Motor Activity/drug effects
9.
Chronobiol Int ; 27(6): 1329-34, 2010 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20653458

ABSTRACT

Traditionally, chronotype classification is based on the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ). It is implicit in the classification that intermediate individuals get intermediate scores to most of the MEQ questions. However, a small group of individuals has a different pattern of answers. In some questions, they answer as "morning-types" and in some others they answer as "evening-types," resulting in an intermediate total score. "Evening-type" and "Morning-type" answers were set as A(1) and A(4), respectively. Intermediate answers were set as A(2) and A(3). The following algorithm was applied: Bimodality Index = (Sigma A(1) x Sigma A(4))(2) - (Sigma A(2) x Sigma A(3))(2). Neither-types that had positive bimodality scores were classified as bimodal. If our hypothesis is validated by objective data, an update of chronotype classification will be required.


Subject(s)
Circadian Rhythm , Adolescent , Algorithms , Animals , Brazil , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Biological , Models, Statistical , Photoperiod , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
10.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry ; 34(7): 1229-35, 2010 Oct 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20599458

ABSTRACT

Glucocorticoids play a role in memory formation, and they may contribute to memory changes in stress-related mental disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder. Cortisol may act through mineralocorticoid (MR) or glucocorticoid (GR) receptors, and the objective of the present study was to evaluate the effects of the MR antagonist spironolactone, the GR antagonist mifepristone, the MR agonist fludrocortisone, and the GR agonist dexamethasone on the extinction of contextually conditioned fear in rats. Propranolol was used as a positive control. As expected, propranolol administered before the test session increased memory extinction. Pre-test administration of spironolactone and low-dose dexamethasone also increased the extinction of an aversive memory, whereas fludrocortisone impaired extinction. High-dose dexamethasone and mifepristone were found to have no effect in this model. Post-test spironolactone treatment impaired aversive memory extinction. These results indicate that MR and GR are related to extinction of aversive memories, and MR blockade may be a promising candidate for the treatment of stress-related memory disorders.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Psychological/drug effects , Dexamethasone/pharmacology , Extinction, Psychological/drug effects , Fear/drug effects , Glucocorticoids/pharmacology , Mineralocorticoid Receptor Antagonists/pharmacology , Spironolactone/pharmacology , Adrenergic beta-Antagonists/pharmacology , Animals , Behavior, Animal/drug effects , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Male , Maze Learning/drug effects , Propranolol/pharmacology , Rats , Rats, Wistar
11.
Chronobiol Int ; 24(1): 1-8, 2007.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17364575

ABSTRACT

A Clock polymorphism T to C situated in the 3' untranslated region (3'-UTR) has been associated with human diurnal preference. At first, Clock 3111C had been reported as a marker for evening preference. However these data are controversial, and data both corroborating and denying them have been reported. This study hypothesizes that differences in Clock genotypes could be observed if extreme morning-type subjects were compared with extreme evening-type subjects, and the T3111C and T257G polymorphisms were studied. The possible relationship between both polymorphisms and delayed sleep phase syndrome (DSPS) was also investigated. An interesting and almost complete linkage disequilibrium between the polymorphisms T257G in the 5' UTR region and the T3111C in the 3' UTR region of the Clock gene is described. Almost always, a G in position 257 corresponds to a C in position 3111, and a T in position 257 corresponds to a T in position 3111. The possibility of an interaction of these two regions in the Clock messenger RNA structure that could affect gene expression was analyzed using computer software. The analyses did not reveal an interaction between those two regions, and it is unlikely that this full allele correspondence affects Clock gene expression. These results show that there is no association between either polymorphism T3111C or T257G in the Clock gene with diurnal preference or delayed sleep phase syndrome (DSPS). These controversial data could result from the possible effects of latitude and clock genes interaction on circadian phenotypes.


Subject(s)
Circadian Rhythm/genetics , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide/genetics , Trans-Activators/genetics , Adult , Amino Acids/genetics , Brazil , CLOCK Proteins , Female , Gene Frequency , Genotype , Humans , Male , Nucleic Acid Conformation , Phenotype , RNA, Messenger/chemistry
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...