Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 51
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neuroimage ; 219: 117015, 2020 10 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32505699

ABSTRACT

Perceptual decision-making is a complex task that requires multiple processing steps performed by spatially distinct brain regions interacting in order to optimize perception and motor response. Most of our knowledge on these processes and interactions were derived from unimodal stimulations of the visual system which identified the lateral intraparietal area and the posterior parietal cortex as critical regions. Unlike the visual system, the vestibular system has no primary cortical areas and it is associated with separate multisensory areas within the temporo-parietal cortex with the parieto-insular vestibular cortex, PIVC, being the core region. The aim of the presented experiment was to investigate the transition from sensation to perception and to reveal the main structures of the cortical vestibular system involved in perceptual decision-making. Therefore, an EEG analysis was performed in 35 healthy subjects during linear whole-body accelerations of different intensities on a motor-driven motion platform (hexapod). We used a discrimination task in order to judge the intensity of the accelerations. Furthermore, we manipulated the expectation of the upcoming stimulus by indicating the probability (25%, 50%, 75%, 100%) of the motion direction. The analysis of the vestibular evoked potentials (VestEPs) showed that the decision-making process leads to a second positive peak (P2b) which was not observed in previous task-free experiments. The comparison of the estimated neural generators of the P2a and P2b components showed significant activity differences in the anterior cingulus, the parahippocampal and the middle temporal gyri. Taking into account the time courses of the P2 components, the physical properties of the stimuli, and the responses given by the subjects we conclude that the P2b likely reflects the transition from the processing of sensory information to perceptual evaluation. Analyzing the decision-uncertainty reported by the subjects, a persistent divergence of the time courses starting at 188 â€‹ms after the acceleration was found at electrode Pz. This finding demonstrated that meta-cognition by means of confidence estimation starts in parallel with the decision-making process itself. Further analyses in the time-frequency domain revealed that a correct classification of acceleration intensities correlated with an inter-trial phase clustering at electrode Cz and an inter-site phase clustering of theta oscillations over frontal, central, and parietal cortical areas. The sites where the phase clustering was observed corresponded to core decision-making brain areas known from neuroimaging studies in the visual domain.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Theta Rhythm/physiology , Vestibular Evoked Myogenic Potentials/physiology , Acceleration , Adult , Brain Mapping , Electroencephalography , Female , Humans , Male , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology
2.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 82(4): 1987-1999, 2020 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31898068

ABSTRACT

Perceptual learning, the ability to improve the sensitivity of sensory perception through training, has been shown to exist in all sensory systems but the vestibular system. A previous study found no improvement of passive self-motion thresholds in the dark after intense direction discrimination training of either yaw rotations (stimulating semicircular canals) or y-translation (stimulating otoliths). The goal of the present study was to investigate whether perceptual learning of self-motion in the dark would occur when there is a simultaneous otolith and semicircular canal input, as is the case with roll tilt motion stimuli. Blindfolded subjects (n = 10) trained on a direction discrimination task with 0.2-Hz roll tilt motion stimuli (9 h of training, 1,800 trials). Before and after training, motion thresholds were measured in the dark for the trained motion and for three transfer conditions. We found that roll tilt sensitivity in the 0.2-Hz roll tilt condition was increased (i.e., thresholds decreased) after training but not for controls who were not exposed to training. This is the first demonstration of perceptual learning of passive self-motion direction discrimination in the dark. The results have potential therapeutic relevance as 0.2-Hz roll thresholds have been associated with poor performance on a clinical balance test that has been linked to more than a fivefold increase in falls.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Humans , Learning , Motion , Otolithic Membrane , Visual Perception
3.
Sci Rep ; 9(1): 15046, 2019 10 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31636281

ABSTRACT

A distorted model of a familiar multi-level building with a systematic overestimation of the height was demonstrated earlier in psychophysical and real world navigational tasks. In the current study we further investigated this phenomenon with a tablet-based application. Participants were asked to adjust height and width of the presented buildings to best match their memory of the dimensional ratio. The estimation errors between adjusted and true height-width ratios were analyzed. Additionally, familiarity with respect to in- and outside of the building as well as demographic data were acquired. A total of 142 subjects aged 21 to 90 years from the cities of Bern and Munich were tested. Major results were: (1) a median overestimation of the height of the multi-level buildings of 11%; (2) estimation errors were significantly less if the particular building was unknown to participants; (3) in contrast, the height of tower-like buildings was underestimated; (4) the height of long, flat shaped buildings was overestimated. (5) Further features, such as the architectonical complexity were critical. Overall, our internal models of large multi-level buildings are distorted due to multiple factors including geometric features and memory effects demonstrating that such individual models are not rigid but plastic with consequences for spatial orientation and navigation.


Subject(s)
Construction Industry , Space Perception , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cities , Female , Germany , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
4.
BMC Geriatr ; 15: 176, 2015 Dec 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26714495

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Crossing a street can be a very difficult task for older pedestrians. With increased age and potential cognitive decline, older people take the decision to cross a street primarily based on vehicles' distance, and not on their speed. Furthermore, older pedestrians tend to overestimate their own walking speed, and could not adapt it according to the traffic conditions. Pedestrians' behavior is often tested using virtual reality. Virtual reality presents the advantage of being safe, cost-effective, and allows using standardized test conditions. METHODS: This paper describes an observational study with older and younger adults. Street crossing behavior was investigated in 18 healthy, younger and 18 older subjects by using a virtual reality setting. The aim of the study was to measure behavioral data (such as eye and head movements) and to assess how the two age groups differ in terms of number of safe street crossings, virtual crashes, and missed street crossing opportunities. Street crossing behavior, eye and head movements, in older and younger subjects, were compared with non-parametric tests. RESULTS: The results showed that younger pedestrians behaved in a more secure manner while crossing a street, as compared to older people. The eye and head movements analysis revealed that older people looked more at the ground and less at the other side of the street to cross. CONCLUSIONS: The less secure behavior in street crossing found in older pedestrians could be explained by their reduced cognitive and visual abilities, which, in turn, resulted in difficulties in the decision-making process, especially under time pressure. Decisions to cross a street are based on the distance of the oncoming cars, rather than their speed, for both groups. Older pedestrians look more at their feet, probably because of their need of more time to plan precise stepping movement and, in turn, pay less attention to the traffic. This might help to set up guidelines for improving senior pedestrians' safety, in terms of speed limits, road design, and mixed physical-cognitive trainings.


Subject(s)
Accidents, Traffic/psychology , Attention/physiology , Behavior/physiology , Decision Making , Eye Movements/physiology , Head Movements/physiology , Pedestrians/psychology , Accidents, Traffic/prevention & control , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aging , Computer Simulation , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Walking/psychology , Young Adult
5.
Neurophysiol Clin ; 45(4-5): 241-54, 2015 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26602955

ABSTRACT

The degree to which others in our environment influence sensorimotor processing has been a particular focus of cognitive neuroscience for the past two decades. This process of self-other resonance, or shared body representation, has only recently been extended to more global bodily processes such as self-location, self-motion perception, balance and perspective taking. In this review, we outline these previously overlooked areas of research to bridge the distinct field of social neuroscience with global self-perception, vestibular processing and postural control. Firstly, we outline research showing that the presence and movement of others can modulate two fundamental experiences of the self: self-location (the experience of where the self is located in space) and self-motion perception (the experience that oneself has moved or has been moved in space). Secondly, we outline recent research that has shown perturbations in balance control as a result of instability in others in our environment. Conversely to this, we also highlight studies in virtual reality demonstrating the potential benefits of the presence of others in our environment for those undergoing vestibular rehabilitation. Thirdly, we outline studies of first- and third-person perspective taking, which is the ability to have or take a visuo-spatial perspective within and out-with the confines of our own body. These studies demonstrate a contamination of perspective taking processes (i.e. automatic, implicit, third-person perspective taking) in the presence of others. This collection of research highlights the importance of social cues in the more global processing of the self and its accompanying sensory inputs, particularly vestibular signals. Future research will need to better determine the mechanisms of self-other resonance within these processes, including the role of individual differences in the susceptibility to the influence of another.


Subject(s)
Body Image , Orientation/physiology , Postural Balance , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology
6.
Neuroscience ; 212: 159-79, 2012 Jun 14.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22516007

ABSTRACT

The vestibular system contributes to the control of posture and eye movements and is also involved in various cognitive functions including spatial navigation and memory. These functions are subtended by projections to a vestibular cortex, whose exact location in the human brain is still a matter of debate (Lopez and Blanke, 2011). The vestibular cortex can be defined as the network of all cortical areas receiving inputs from the vestibular system, including areas where vestibular signals influence the processing of other sensory (e.g. somatosensory and visual) and motor signals. Previous neuroimaging studies used caloric vestibular stimulation (CVS), galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS), and auditory stimulation (clicks and short-tone bursts) to activate the vestibular receptors and localize the vestibular cortex. However, these three methods differ regarding the receptors stimulated (otoliths, semicircular canals) and the concurrent activation of the tactile, thermal, nociceptive and auditory systems. To evaluate the convergence between these methods and provide a statistical analysis of the localization of the human vestibular cortex, we performed an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies using CVS, GVS, and auditory stimuli. We analyzed a total of 352 activation foci reported in 16 studies carried out in a total of 192 healthy participants. The results reveal that the main regions activated by CVS, GVS, or auditory stimuli were located in the Sylvian fissure, insula, retroinsular cortex, fronto-parietal operculum, superior temporal gyrus, and cingulate cortex. Conjunction analysis indicated that regions showing convergence between two stimulation methods were located in the median (short gyrus III) and posterior (long gyrus IV) insula, parietal operculum and retroinsular cortex (Ri). The only area of convergence between all three methods of stimulation was located in Ri. The data indicate that Ri, parietal operculum and posterior insula are vestibular regions where afferents converge from otoliths and semicircular canals, and may thus be involved in the processing of signals informing about body rotations, translations and tilts. Results from the meta-analysis are in agreement with electrophysiological recordings in monkeys showing main vestibular projections in the transitional zone between Ri, the insular granular field (Ig), and SII.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Cerebral Cortex/cytology , Vestibular Nerve/cytology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/cytology , Animals , Brain Mapping/statistics & numerical data , Haplorhini , Humans , Likelihood Functions , Neural Pathways/cytology
7.
J Pathol ; 216(2): 193-200, 2008 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18683853

ABSTRACT

In the histomorphological grading of prostate carcinoma, pathologists have regularly assigned comparable scores for the architectural Gleason and the now-obsolete nuclear World Health Organization (WHO) grading systems. Although both systems demonstrate good correspondence between grade and survival, they are based on fundamentally different biological criteria. We tested the hypothesis that this apparent concurrence between the two grading systems originates from an interpretation bias in the minds of diagnostic pathologists, rather than reflecting a biological reality. Three pathologists graded 178 prostatectomy specimens, assigning Gleason and WHO scores on glass slides and on digital images of nuclei isolated out of their architectural context. The results were analysed with respect to interdependencies among the grading systems, to tumour recurrence (PSA relapse > 0.1 ng/ml at 48 months) and robust nuclear morphometry, as assessed by computer-assisted image analysis. WHO and Gleason grades were strongly correlated (r = 0.82) and demonstrated identical prognostic power. However, WHO grades correlated poorly with nuclear morphology (r = 0.19). Grading of nuclei isolated out of their architectural context significantly improved accuracy for nuclear morphology (r = 0.55), but the prognostic power was virtually lost. In conclusion, the architectural organization of a tumour, which the pathologist cannot avoid noticing during initial slide viewing at low magnification, unwittingly influences the subsequent nuclear grade assignment. In our study, the prognostic power of the WHO grading system was dependent on visual assessment of tumour growth pattern. We demonstrate for the first time the influence a cognitive bias can have in the generation of an error in diagnostic pathology and highlight a considerable problem in histopathological tumour grading.


Subject(s)
Adenocarcinoma/pathology , Cognition , Pathology, Clinical/standards , Prejudice , Prostate/pathology , Prostatic Neoplasms/pathology , Adult , Aged , Cell Nucleus/ultrastructure , Clinical Competence , Diagnostic Errors , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Middle Aged , Pathology, Clinical/methods , Prognosis , Proportional Hazards Models , Prostatectomy , ROC Curve
8.
J Neural Transm (Vienna) ; 114(5): 675-86, 2007.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17160371

ABSTRACT

Mental rotation is a complex cognitive skill depending on the manipulation of mental representations. We aimed to investigate the maturing neuronal network for mental rotation by measuring brain activation in 20 children and 20 adults using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Our results indicate that brain activation patterns are very similar between children and adults. However, adults exhibit stronger activation in the left intraparietal sulcus compared to children. This finding suggests a shift of activation from a predominantly right parietal activation in children to a bilateral activation pattern in adults. Furthermore, adults show a deactivation of the posterior cingulate gyrus and precuneus, which is not observed in children. In conclusion, developmental changes of brain activation during mental rotation are leading to a bilateral parietal activation pattern and faster performance.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Brain/growth & development , Cognition/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Imagination/physiology , Adult , Brain/anatomy & histology , Brain Mapping , Child , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Nerve Net/anatomy & histology , Nerve Net/growth & development , Neuropsychological Tests , Occipital Lobe/anatomy & histology , Occipital Lobe/growth & development , Parietal Lobe/anatomy & histology , Parietal Lobe/growth & development , Reaction Time/physiology , Rotation , Sex Characteristics
9.
Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd ; 146(47): 2250-2, 2002 Nov 23.
Article in Dutch | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12481522

ABSTRACT

The availability of biomedical information in the Netherlands is largely guaranteed by the libraries of the eight medical faculties and related university hospitals together with the Netherlands Institute for Scientific Information Services (Dutch acronym: NIWI). The acquisition of biomedical journals has always been coordinated at a national level. Up until now the NIWI has played a central role in filling in the gaps left by the other collections. It also functions as a national archive and a main document delivery centre. The NIWI receives financial support from the Dutch government to perform these tasks. Recently, NIWI announced that it intends to terminate its acquisition and document delivery service for the biomedical sciences. The remaining eight libraries foresee a number of negative effects if no alternatives to the present NIWI service are developed. We are of the opinion that this situation necessitates a national, biomedical information strategy.


Subject(s)
Interlibrary Loans , Libraries, Medical , Periodicals as Topic , Computers/statistics & numerical data , Humans , Information Systems , Netherlands , Publishing
10.
J Vestib Res ; 12(5-6): 283-9, 2002.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14501104

ABSTRACT

The effects of cross-coupled stimuli on the semicircular canals are shown to be influenced by the position of the subject's head with respect to gravity and the axis of rotation, but not by the subject's head position relative to the trunk. Seventeen healthy subjects made head yaw movements out of the horizontal plane while lying on a horizontal platform (MIT short radius centrifuge) rotating at 23 rpm about an earth-vertical axis. The subjects reported the magnitude and duration of the illusory pitch or roll sensations elicited by the cross-coupled rotational stimuli acting on the semicircular canals. The results suggest an influence of head position relative to gravity. The magnitude estimation is higher and the sensation decays more slowly when the head's final position is toward nose-up (gravity in the subject's head x-z-plane) compared to when the head is turned toward the side (gravity in the subject's head y-z-plane). The results are discussed with respect to artificial gravity in space and the possible role of pre-adaptation to cross-coupled angular accelerations on earth.


Subject(s)
Gravity, Altered , Head/physiology , Illusions/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Posture/physiology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Gravitation , Head Movements/physiology , Humans , Male , Rotation/adverse effects , Semicircular Canals/physiology
11.
J Mol Cell Cardiol ; 33(12): 2083-94, 2001 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11735256

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previously we documented cellular structural changes of a non-degenerative nature in atrial myocytes after atrial fibrillation (AF) in the goat. The time course of these changes was not studied. METHODS AND RESULTS: Cellular structural changes were studied by light- and electron microscopy and immunohistochemistry in goat atria after 0-16 weeks AF. The first sign of cellular structural remodeling was a more homogeneous chromatin distribution, at 1 week of AF. Sub-structural changes in mitochondria and sarcoplasmic reticulum occurred gradually. Cellular degeneration was absent. The degree of myolysis and glycogen accumulation increased till 8 weeks of AF and did not increase further from thereon. After 16 weeks of AF, 42% of the myocytes in the right atrial free wall were affected by myolysis. The diameter of the atrial myocytes increased. Dedifferentiation of the atrial myocytes was suggested by altered expression patterns of structural proteins, such as the disappearance of cardiotin (1 week), the A-I junctional part of titin (4 weeks), desmin at the intercalated disk (ID) (8 weeks) and a gradual re-expression of alpha-smooth muscle actin. CONCLUSION: Remodeling of the cellular ultrastructure in atrial myocardium of the goat develops progressively during AF. Re-expression of fetal proteins indicate dedifferentiation of atrial myocytes, analogous to observations in hibernating myocardium of the ventricle.


Subject(s)
Atrial Fibrillation/physiopathology , Heart Atria/diagnostic imaging , Heart Atria/metabolism , Proteins/metabolism , Actinin/metabolism , Actins/metabolism , Animals , Atrial Fibrillation/metabolism , Cell Adhesion Molecules/metabolism , Cell Size , Connectin , Connective Tissue/diagnostic imaging , Disease Models, Animal , Goats , Heart Atria/pathology , Muscle Proteins/metabolism , Muscle, Smooth, Vascular/metabolism , Protein Kinases/metabolism , Time Factors , Ultrasonography
12.
Perception ; 30(8): 945-57, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11578080

ABSTRACT

When viewing a wide-angle visual display, which rotates in the frontoparallel plane around the line of sight, observers experience an illusory shift of the direction of gravity; this shift leads to an apparent tilt of the body and displaces allocentric space coordinates. In this study, subjects adjusted an indicator to the apparent horizontal while viewing a rotating display. To determine whether top down processes could affect the illusion, the subjects were asked to visualize a rotating configuration of dots onto a blank central portion of the moving visual field. Visualizing dots and actually viewing the dots deflected the spatial judgment in very similar ways. These results demonstrate that top down processing can affect allocentric space coordinates.


Subject(s)
Imagination/physiology , Kinesthesis/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Perceptual Closure/physiology
13.
Circulation ; 104(8): 957-62, 2001 Aug 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11514386

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Recently, the temporal excitable gap during atrial fibrillation (AF) has been identified as a vulnerable parameter for cardioversion of AF. In this study, we evaluated 5 methods to measure the refractory period (RP(AF)) and the excitable period (EP(AF)) during persistent AF. METHODS AND RESULTS: In 11 goats instrumented with 83 epicardial atrial electrodes, persistent AF (43+/-34 days) was induced with a median AF cycle length (CL) of 98+/-14 ms. To measure RP(AF), premature stimuli were applied to the center of the electrode array on the right or left atrium. The RP(AF) measured by mapping of premature stimuli was 70+/-12 ms ("gold standard"). The RP(AF) determined during entrainment of AF was 77+/-17 ms (R(2)=0.88, P<0.01). Statistical analysis of the effects of synchronized stimuli (each coupling interval x100) on the AFCL histogram yielded an RP(AF) of 70+/-13 ms (R(2)=0.94, P<0.01). A further simplification was to apply slow fixed-rate pacing (1 Hz) during AF. For each stimulus (n=250 to 500), the paced AFCL was plotted against its coupling interval, and capture was determined by statistical shortening of the AFCL (RP(AF) 71+/-17 ms, R(2)=0.84, P<0.01). The 5th percentile of the AFCL histogram as an index of RP(AF) was 77+/-12 ms (R(2)=0.90, P<0.01). CONCLUSIONS: During persistent AF with an AFCL of 98+/-14 ms, the RP(AF) determined by mapping of synchronized premature stimuli (gold standard) was 70+/-12 ms, with an excitable period of 28+/-8 ms. Although the indirect methods to measure RP(AF) all correlated well with the gold standard, slow fixed-rate pacing seems to be the most attractive technique because of the ease of acquiring the data and the clear graphic result.


Subject(s)
Atrial Fibrillation/physiopathology , Electrophysiologic Techniques, Cardiac/methods , Animals , Body Surface Potential Mapping , Cardiac Pacing, Artificial/methods , Disease Models, Animal , Electrodes, Implanted , Goats , Heart Atria/physiopathology , Heart Conduction System/physiopathology , Reproducibility of Results , Sensory Thresholds , Time Factors
14.
Circulation ; 102(2): 260-7, 2000 Jul 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10889140

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous studies suggest that the antifibrillatory action of class I and III drugs is due to prolongation of the atrial wavelength. The aim of the present study was to directly evaluate the electrophysiological action of antifibrillatory drugs in a goat model of chronic atrial fibrillation (AF). METHODS AND RESULTS: Six goats were instrumented with multiple atrial electrodes, and sustained AF was induced by electrical remodeling. During sustained AF, the effects of intravenous infusion of cibenzoline, hydroquinidine, flecainide, and d-sotalol on AF cycle length (AFCL), refractory period (RP(AF)), conduction velocity (CV(AF)), pathlength (PL(AF)), wavelength (WL(AF)), temporal (AFCL-RP(AF)), and spatial (PL(AF)-WL(AF)) excitable gap were studied. The RP(AF) was measured by determining the earliest moment at which single stimuli could capture the fibrillating atria. CV(AF) was measured during regional entrainment of AF. Contrary to our expectation, cardioversion of AF could not be attributed to prolongation of WL(AF). Hydroquinidine and d-sotalol did not affect WL(AF) significantly, whereas cibenzoline and flecainide even shortened WL(AF) by 18% and 36%, respectively. PL(AF) was increased by hydroquinidine and d-sotalol by 30%, whereas cibenzoline and flecainide did not prolong PL(AF). The only parameter that correlated consistently with cardioversion of AF was a widening of the temporal excitable gap (cibenzoline 176%, hydroquinidine 105%, flecainide 86%, d-sotalol 88%). CONCLUSIONS: Pharmacological cardioversion of AF cannot be explained by prolongation of WL(AF). An alternative explanation for the antifibrillatory effect of class I and III drugs may be a widening of the temporal excitable gap.


Subject(s)
Anti-Arrhythmia Agents/pharmacology , Atrial Fibrillation/drug therapy , Atrial Fibrillation/physiopathology , Electrocardiography/drug effects , Heart Conduction System/drug effects , Imidazoles/pharmacology , Action Potentials/drug effects , Animals , Female , Flecainide/pharmacology , Goats , Heart Conduction System/physiopathology , Quinidine/analogs & derivatives , Quinidine/pharmacology , Sotalol/pharmacology
15.
J Cardiovasc Electrophysiol ; 11(3): 262-73, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10749349

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: During reentrant arrhythmias, the circulating wavefront often makes a sharp turn around a functional or anatomic barrier. We tested the hypothesis that lowering the safety factor for conduction by high K+ or flecainide preferentially depresses conduction of sharply turning wavefronts. METHODS AND RESULTS: In 16 Langendorff-perfused rabbit hearts, a thin layer of anisotropic ventricular myocardium was made using a cryoprocedure. In this layer, a linear radiofrequency lesion was made parallel to the fiber orientation. The tip of the lesion was extended by a short incision. U-turning wavefronts were initiated by pacing at one side of the lesion. A mapping electrode (240 electrodes, resolution 350 to 700 microm) was used to measure conduction times and velocity of planar waves (longitudinal and transverse) and U-turning wavefronts. The safety factor for conduction was lowered by high potassium (8, 10, and 12 mmol/L) and flecainide (1 and 2 mg/L). On average, high potassium and flecainide increased the conduction times of U-turning wavefronts 1.6 times more than longitudinal or transverse planar wavefronts (P < 0.01). At a critical lowering of the excitatory current, functional conduction block occurred at the pivot point, which forced the wavefront to make a longer U-turn. In these cases, the total U-turn conduction time increased from 27+/-9 msec to 75+/-37 msec. About 40% of this delay was caused by a shift of the pivot point and consequent lengthening of the returning pathway. CONCLUSION: Lowering the amount of excitatory current by potassium or flecainide preferentially impairs U-turn conduction. The occurrence of long delays and conduction block at pivot points may explain the mode of action of Class I drugs.


Subject(s)
Anti-Arrhythmia Agents/therapeutic use , Flecainide/therapeutic use , Heart Conduction System/drug effects , Heart Ventricles/physiopathology , Myocardium/metabolism , Potassium/therapeutic use , Tachycardia, Atrioventricular Nodal Reentry/physiopathology , Animals , Body Surface Potential Mapping/drug effects , Cardiac Pacing, Artificial , Catheter Ablation/adverse effects , Disease Models, Animal , Female , Heart Conduction System/physiopathology , Heart Ventricles/drug effects , In Vitro Techniques , Male , Rabbits , Tachycardia, Atrioventricular Nodal Reentry/etiology , Tachycardia, Atrioventricular Nodal Reentry/therapy
16.
Aviat Space Environ Med ; 70(9): 879-86, 1999 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10503753

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The subjective visual vertical (SVV) and the subjective horizontal body position (SHP) perceptually dissociate and may therefore be based on a differential participation of graviceptive references. This study focuses primarily on the effects that are caused by an alteration of somatosensory information on the skin surface induced by water immersion. We expect the SHP to be selectively affected during water immersion while the SVV remains unchanged. METHODS: Four diving subjects took part in the experiments. An underwater apparatus (UWA) allowed for a determination of the SHP and the SVV during water immersion. On land, the adjustments of the SHP and SVV were made in a tilt chair apparatus (TCA). RESULTS: Three of four subjects show a significant head-upward shift between 7 degrees and 20.3 degrees of the SHP when tested under water (p < 0.02). The adjustments of the SVV were compared within corresponding physical roll inclinations and did not differ with the exception of one subject who showed a significant change of the SVV toward the physical vertical. CONCLUSIONS: The SHP and SVV perceptually dissociate on land and, as we determined for the first time, also under water. Water immersion leads to an increased roll tilt perception as measured by the SHP. However, a systematic influence on SVV could not be observed under water thus supporting the assumption that the SVV and SHP depend on gravitoreceptive information that is selectively altered during water immersion.


Subject(s)
Immersion/adverse effects , Immersion/physiopathology , Orientation/physiology , Posture/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Cues , Diving/adverse effects , Diving/physiology , Female , Gravitation , Head Movements , Humans , Male , Rotation , Tilt-Table Test
17.
J Cardiovasc Electrophysiol ; 10(8): 1101-8, 1999 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10466491

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Episodes of atrial fibrillation (AF) are known to cause both a rapid reduction in atrial refractoriness (atrial electrical remodeling) and a more delayed increase in AF stability in the chronic goat model. The aims of this study were to examine (1) the hypothesis that an AF-induced increase in AF stability might be due to a mechanism with a longer onset and offset than that of changes in refractoriness and (2) the possibility that repeated paroxysms of maintained AF might cause a cumulative increase in AF stability independent of changes in atrial refractoriness. METHODS AND RESULTS: AF was maintained by rapid atrial pacing in seven goats for three consecutive 5-day periods, each separated from each other by 48 hours of sinus rhythm. Assessments of atrial refractory periods, conduction velocity, AF inducibility, and duration of individual episodes of AF were attempted at intervals throughout the protocol. Forty-eight hours of sinus rhythm was just sufficient for refractoriness changes to fully reverse in all goats, with no evidence of any "residual" increase in AF inducibility. There was no significant difference among any of the three periods of pacing-maintained AF with regard to time to develop episodes of AF of 60-second duration (22.1+/-13, 23.8+/-16, and 30.3+/-29 hours), 1-hour duration (56.6+/-28, 61.3+/-31, and 60.1+/-32 hours), or 24-hour duration (84.0+/-31, 87.0+/-33, and 83.5+/-32 hours). CONCLUSION: There is no evidence for a cumulative effect of AF paroxysms on AF inducibility or stability independent of changes in refractoriness. These findings highlight the importance of atrial refractoriness as a potential target for antiarrhythmic strategies aimed at inhibiting the self-perpetuation of AF.


Subject(s)
Atrial Fibrillation/physiopathology , Electrocardiography , Heart Conduction System/physiopathology , Tachycardia, Paroxysmal/physiopathology , Action Potentials , Animals , Atrial Fibrillation/etiology , Disease Models, Animal , Disease Susceptibility/physiopathology , Electric Stimulation/adverse effects , Female , Goats , Recurrence , Tachycardia, Paroxysmal/etiology
18.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 871: 221-31, 1999 May 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10372074

ABSTRACT

The effects of peripheral vestibular diseases on the subjective visual vertical (SVV) are resumed and provide the basis for some insights into the otolith pathophysiology. With a normal range of 0 +/- 2 deg (when measured in an upright body position), the SVV was shifted by 11 +/- 6 deg toward the ipsilateral ear in 40 patients following an acute unilateral vestibular deafferentiation (UVD), but in the opposite direction in 9 of 52 patients after stapes surgery. These opposite effects suggest a push-pull mechanism of the pairs of otolith organs with respect to the SVV. The dissociation between the SVV and the perception of body position indicates influences by unconscious reflexive mechanisms such as ocular cyclotorsion on the SVV. In chronic UVD patients, lateral shifts of the subjects during constant angular velocity rotation into various eccentric positions (+/- 16 cm) revealed a shift of the "center of graviception" close to the remaining intact contralateral inner ear. To date, this seems to be the most consistent test for clinical identification of a chronic compensated unilateral loss of otolith function. The findings regarding asymmetries in otolithic sensitivity to medially and laterally directed roll-tilts remain controversial, probably mainly because of influences of extravestibular cues.


Subject(s)
Gravity Sensing/physiology , Otolithic Membrane/physiopathology , Vestibular Diseases/physiopathology , Visual Perception/physiology , Afferent Pathways/physiopathology , Denervation , Ear, Inner/physiopathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Motion Perception/physiology , Posture/physiology , Reflex/physiology , Vestibular Nerve/physiopathology
19.
Neuroreport ; 10(17): 3549-53, 1999 Nov 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10619642

ABSTRACT

The subjective visual vertical is determined when a subject judges the orientation of an indicator (e.g. a short line segment) as apparently vertical. The mechanisms that underlie this perceptual performance are usually assumed to be based predominantly on bottom-up processing of primarily vestibular and visual information. However, it is also possible that top-down processes play a role in such abilities. We used an interference paradigm in order to investigate the effects of mental images on the perception of the visual vertical. The results demonstrate for the first time that visual mental imagery can exert the same directional influence on the subjective visual vertical as a perception of the corresponding stimulus.


Subject(s)
Imagination/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Neurological , Photic Stimulation , Vision, Ocular/physiology
20.
J Vestib Res ; 9(6): 413-22, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10639026

ABSTRACT

Assessing the subjective visual vertical, SVV, in a static upright position is an easy clinical test in which a deviation of some 10 degrees from true vertical indicates an acute loss of unilateral (otolithic) vestibular function on the side to which the SVV is tilted. Because this deviation of the SVV is compensated during the following months, patients with chronic unilateral vestibular loss do no longer differ from normal subjects. This study presents an experimental set-up that allows for clear detection of compensated chronic loss of unilateral otolithic function by testing the SVV. 21 normals and 17 unilaterally vestibular deafferentiated (UVD) patients (vestibular neurectomies) were first rotated on a human centrifuge about an earth vertical yaw axis through the midsagittal plane of the head (240 degrees/s). This induced tilts of the gravito-inertial force (GIF) vectors, which differed at the two inner ears by 8 degrees. During constant velocity rotation, the subjects were moved in pseudo-randomized steps laterally up to 16 cm apart from the rotation axis, inducing roll tilts of the GIF vectors up to 16 degrees. Normal subjects set their SVV to pre-centrifugation values at positions with the midsagittal plane of their head close to the rotation axis, while chronic UVD patients indicated pre-centrifugation values during positions with the rotation axis 5.9 +/- 2.5 cm paramedian on the side of the intact ear. Tilts of the GIF vectors shifted the SVV with a gain of 0.70 in normals and only 0.32 in UVD patients. Roll gains for laterally directed GIF vectors relative to the intact inner ear did not differ from medially directed roll gains in the UVD patients. The roll gains observed in this experimental set-up were lower than those observed with static body tilts or during eccentric rotation with a larger radius, which might be at least partially due to conflicting stimulation between otolithic and extra-vestibular cues.


Subject(s)
Axotomy , Otolithic Membrane/physiology , Space Perception , Vestibular Nerve/injuries , Adult , Centrifugation , Chronic Disease , Gravitation , Humans , Meniere Disease/physiopathology , Meniere Disease/surgery , Middle Aged , Rotation , Vertigo/physiopathology , Vertigo/surgery
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...