Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 54
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Exp Biol Med (Maywood) ; 226(4): 349-52, 2001 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11368428

ABSTRACT

Glutathione and cysteine are major antioxidants in blood that are associated with health and longevity. To ensure their measurement, careful attention to avoid auto-oxidation is necessary to stabilize the samples. Since no report of these compounds has been reported in children, our goal was to determine their levels of reduced and oxidized glutathione (GSH and GSSG) and cysteine (Cys and CSSC), To this end, 140 healthy children, ages 2 to 9 years from the Louisville Twin Study were studied. Blood samples were collected and analyzed for GSH, GSSG, Cys, and CSSC by our HPLC dual electrochemical method. The results showed that GSH and total GSH (GSH + GSSG) levels for monozygotic (MZ) twins were significantly higher (P < 0.001) than levels for dizygotic (DZ) twins. However, the opposite occurred for Cys and total Cys (Cys + CSSC) in that the levels were significantly higher for DZ twins than for MZ twins. (P < 0.005-0.013). In spite of this marked difference in zygosity, the within-pair correlations for twin pairs used for estimating heritability suggested that there was a major environmental influence for total GSH and total Cys. Finally. GSH levels were significantly lower for young (2-9 years) children than adults (P < 0.001).


Subject(s)
Cysteine/blood , Glutathione/blood , Twins/statistics & numerical data , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Chromatography, High Pressure Liquid , Cystine/blood , Glutathione Disulfide/blood , Humans , Twins, Dizygotic , Twins, Monozygotic
2.
Twin Res ; 3(4): 242-50, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11463145

ABSTRACT

A twin study of infant attachment security at age 24 months was conducted on archival data for a sample of 99 MZ pairs and 108 DZ pairs from the Louisville Twin Study. MZ concordance for attachment was 62.6%, which was significantly greater than the DZ concordance of 44.4%. Concordances were transformed into polychoric correlations, and LISREL was used to conduct a quantitative genetic analysis of the data. Results indicated that 25% of the variability in attachment was attributable to genetic factors, and the remaining 75% was attributable to non-shared environmental effects. No evidence was found for a contribution from shared environmental influences to attachment security. Possible concerns about the validity of twin methodology are addressed and various interpretations of the results are presented.


Subject(s)
Environment , Mother-Child Relations , Object Attachment , Psychology, Child , Twins, Dizygotic/genetics , Twins, Dizygotic/psychology , Twins, Monozygotic/genetics , Twins, Monozygotic/psychology , Anxiety, Separation/genetics , Anxiety, Separation/psychology , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Kentucky , Least-Squares Analysis , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Maternal Behavior , Models, Genetic , Research Design , Temperament , Videotape Recording
3.
Behav Genet ; 28(1): 1-8, 1998 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9573641

ABSTRACT

Two pilot studies were conducted. First, the validity of a measure of infant-caregiver attachment for twins was assessed. Sixteen twin pairs from the Louisville Twin Study (LTS) were assessed in the Strange Situation (SS) at ages 19 or 25 months. Concordance between the LTS procedure and the SS procedure for assessing attachment was 78.1%, significantly greater than chance. Second, twin similarity for attachment as measured by the LTS procedure was assessed. Videotapes of 34 MZ pairs and 26 DZ pairs at ages 18 and 24 months were rated in terms of attachment behavior. MZ concordance for attachment was 67.6%, significantly greater than the DZ concordance rate of 38.5%. Results are discussed in the context of current debate in attachment theory.


Subject(s)
Infant Behavior/classification , Object Attachment , Psychometrics/standards , Twins/genetics , Caregivers , Chi-Square Distribution , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Pilot Projects , Reproducibility of Results
4.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 73(1): 129-38, 1997 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9216081

ABSTRACT

An assumption often made in the study of personality and in social psychology is that methods variance and situation-specific effects, as key components of measured behavioral variance, are environmental effects. The results of the present research refute that assumption. Nine measures-3 aspects of temperament measured in each of 3 ways-were obtained at age 24 months for twin sibships participating in the Louisville Twin Study. This report describes a new model that captures the unique information potentially available in such data, by combining multitrait-multimethod and twin-family analytic designs. The results indicated significant genetic influence on methods-situations components of variance along with genetic influence on traits. The findings support heuristics that include both situation-specific patterns of behavior and cross-situational consistencies.


Subject(s)
Behavior , Genetics , Environment , Female , Humans , Infant , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Twins/psychology
6.
Am J Med Genet ; 60(1): 64-71, 1995 Feb 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7485237

ABSTRACT

A threshold model of latent liability was applied to infant and toddler twin data on total count of injuries sustained during the interval from birth to 36 months of age. A quantitative genetic analysis of estimated twin correlations in injury liability indicated strong genetic dominance effects, but no additive genetic variance was detected. Because interpretations involving overdominance have little research support, the results may be due to low order epistasis or other interaction effects. Boys had more injuries than girls, but this effect was found only for groups whose parents were prompted and questioned in detail about their children's injuries. Activity and impulsivity are two behavioral predictors of childhood injury, and the results are discussed in relation to animal research on infant and adult activity levels, and impulsivity in adult humans. Genetic epidemiological approaches to childhood injury should aid in targeting higher risk children for preventive intervention.


Subject(s)
Wounds and Injuries/genetics , Adult , Animals , Child Behavior , Child, Preschool , Cohort Studies , Female , Humans , Impulsive Behavior , Infant , Male , Models, Genetic , Molecular Epidemiology , Motor Activity , Sex Characteristics , Wounds and Injuries/epidemiology , Wounds and Injuries/prevention & control
7.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) ; 44(2): 63-73, 1995.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8750770

ABSTRACT

Large twin samples and recent applications of multiple regression techniques to behavioral genetics methodology makes possible evaluation of genetic and environmental contributions to the articulation proficiency of individual phonemes. Factor analysis of the articulation scores from 256 MZ and DZ twins and 124 of their non-twin siblings (all children ranged from 2; 11 to 9; 8 years) were conducted to reduce a 50-item articulation test to a more manageable set of five articulation factors. The twins' factor scores were then analyzed using multiple regression procedures to determine the extent to which the individual factors resulted from genetic and/or environmental influences. The /r/ and /[symbol: see text], t[symbol: see text], d[symbol: see text]/ factors were found to have strong genetic components, while the /l, j, w/ factor was found to be strongly influenced by environmental sources of variation.


Subject(s)
Genetics, Behavioral , Phonetics , Child , Child, Preschool , Environment , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Genetic , Regression Analysis , Twins, Dizygotic , Twins, Monozygotic
8.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab ; 78(2): 310-2, 1994 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8106617

ABSTRACT

Insulin-like growth factor-I (IGF-I) levels in plasma were measured in healthy twin children. The within-pair correlation for 43 monozygotic pairs was r = 0.91 (P < or = 0.0001), an association significantly higher than that for same sex dizygotic pairs (r = 0.40; P < or = 0.06). The high correlation for monozygotic twins indicated a marked genetic influence on IGF-I levels. After correction for age and sex, the correlation between IGF-I level and height was r = 0.38 (P < or = 0.0001). These findings provide clear evidence that IGF-I levels correlate with height, a growth characteristic known to be genetically controlled.


Subject(s)
Insulin-Like Growth Factor I/analysis , Twins, Dizygotic , Twins, Monozygotic , Aging/blood , Aging/physiology , Biomarkers/blood , Body Height/physiology , Child , Child, Preschool , Family Health , Female , Humans , Insulin-Like Growth Factor I/genetics , Insulin-Like Growth Factor I/physiology , Male , Phenotype
9.
Pediatrics ; 90(5 Pt 2): 798-807, 1992 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1437410

ABSTRACT

Injury is the third leading cause of death in the United States and the leading cause for children, adolescents, and young adults. Injury results from multiple factors and so may its prevention. The first and simplest approach toward preventing injuries has been to innovatively and aggressively apply a traditional public health model. Strategically, the goal has been to remove harmful agents of injury and to make the environment safer. Tactics such as public information, product regulation, legislative action, and the like have been credited with reductions in mortality and morbidity. To expand our understanding and our prevention strategies across multiple injuries, other scientific knowledge bases and intervention models from fields such as psychology and child development are being used to study childhood injury. These approaches show that in addition to environmental determinants, psychosocial factors involving both the care giver and the child are related to injury. The research programs described here illustrate the advantage of investigating psychosocial factors at both molar and molecular levels. General characteristics of mothers and children related to injury help define families at risk, as well as suggesting vehicles for intervention. Behavioral factors influencing risk perception highlight the etiology of increased risk in adolescence. Injury episodes, even slight, as well as "near injuries" and dangerous and risky behavior can be quantified and analyzed by retrospective ("postmortem") approaches yielding data on commonly occurring consequences (and the lack thereof) for minor injury. Finally, approaches that simulate dangerous situations can identify interaction patterns that result in childhood injury. Based on such research, we are coming to view injuries as the result of patterns of behaviors that develop and persist over time, and as such these patterns can be detected and, one hopes, altered before a serious medical event occurs. The role of the pediatrician after injury occurs is clear. With regard to prevention of injuries, pediatricians' roles are being defined by those individuals who have begun to investigate causes, educate families, and advocate for regulation and prevention. However, like the causes and methods for prevention, the disciplines involved in the study and prevention of injury are multiple. Such a multidisciplinary approach that considers multiple factors, theories, models, and interventions to prevent injury may be the approach that is as simple as possible.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior , Wounds and Injuries , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Models, Theoretical , Perception , Public Health , Risk Factors , Sex Factors , Statistics as Topic , Wounds and Injuries/etiology , Wounds and Injuries/prevention & control , Wounds and Injuries/psychology
10.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) ; 39(2): 143-63, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2239101

ABSTRACT

A preliminary series of quantitative genetic models was applied to a subset of longitudinal height data, spanning birth to maturity, gathered from twin families in the Louisville Twin Study. Descriptive Cholesky factor parameterization was found to give more satisfactory results than did a system of constraints based on a model of developmental transmission of a time-constant and time-specific factors. The results from application of two autosomal sex-limitation models are contrasted with those from a model specifying both autosomal and sex-chromosomal patterns of inheritance. The latter model was more conducive to parameter reduction. Although these models do not constitute conclusive tests of autosomal sex-limitation versus sex-linkage, the more parsimonious model is consistent with previous research suggesting a stature locus on the long arm of the Y chromosome. Heritability of height is estimated at about 90% or greater from 6 years of age on. Substantial and fairly constant longitudinal genetic correlations are found from 3 years of age on. Shared environmental effects unrelated to parental height were seen for birth length, corrected for gestational age, to height at 3 years of age, but these are not satisfactorily differentiated from possible twin effects in the present sample. The genetic consequences of assortative mating are emphasized since failure to take assortment into account can lead to overestimation of shared environmental effects and under-estimation of genetic effects. The results indicate that about 20% of within-gender variability for mature height can be attributed to the genetic consequences of assortment, even though the phenotypic marital correlation of 0.22 is quite modest. The importance of testing the assumption of multivariate normality underlying the application of the method of maximum-likelihood is also highlighted.


Subject(s)
Body Height/genetics , Models, Genetic , Genetic Variation , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Sex Factors , Twins/genetics
11.
Clin Perinatol ; 16(2): 565-76, 1989 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2663316

ABSTRACT

The limitations on predictions of intelligence provided by single-point neonatal and infant assessments continue to hamper efforts to certify the clinical outcome of children at risk. Although new methods of clinical assessment show promise of offsetting these limitations, each method alone has not markedly improved predictions. From a developmental perspective, a strategy for assessment should include a broad range of measures combined empirically to form a developmental risk registry. The strategy takes into account evolving changes, as well as levels of competencies of the infant, and considers these within the evolving environmental context of the family.


Subject(s)
Child Development , Intelligence , Cognition , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Intelligence Tests , Neurologic Examination
12.
J Pers ; 57(2): 215-35, 1989 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2769555

ABSTRACT

Ratings of behaviors pertaining to inhibition were observed for 130 twins participating in a longitudinal study. Ratings were available for four ages (12, 18, 24, and 30 months) and from three sources at each age: direct observations obtained in a laboratory setting, direct observations obtained in conjunction with infant mental testing, and a temperament measure from a questionnaire completed by parents. For the individual twins, the age-to-age correlations were in the moderate range (.26 to .64). The situation-to-situation correlations were generally in the same range (.17 to .64). When the twins were recombined into twin pairs, within-pair (intraclass) correlations indicated that monozygotic (MZ) twins were more concordant than dizygotic (DZ) twins for each of the behaviors at each of the ages. Also, the MZ twins were more concordant for the direction and degree of behavioral change from age to age or from situation to situation. These data provide additional evidence for the biological influence on behavioral inhibition, a characteristic that has been studied in temperament and personality research. The results suggest that the trait of behavioral inhibition and a change in the trait are genetically conditioned. In addition, it is suggested that the concept of trait be expanded to include the person-centered biological regulation of change.


Subject(s)
Inhibition, Psychological , Personality Development , Personality , Temperament , Twins/psychology , Affect , Humans , Infant , Longitudinal Studies , Models, Genetic , Shyness , Twins, Dizygotic/psychology , Twins, Monozygotic/psychology
13.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) ; 36(2): 135-43, 1987.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3434125

ABSTRACT

In the Louisville Twin Study, pairs of 3- and 4-year-old twins were provided with standardized competitive or cooperative tasks in a laboratory setting. Some tasks required a pair of twins to share toys; other fostered a more competitive engagement between the twins. Behavioral ratings identified temperament and social components at both ages, and between the two ages, there were transformations in the links between the components. Parental ratings of the twins' temperament at the same ages were moderately correlated with the laboratory observations, but the pattern of the relations changed from one age to the next. The combined sets of measures were subjected to twin analyses for 43 pairs of twins. The results are discussed in terms of the similarity of MZ and DZ pairs for the dimensions of temperament and the transformations of temperament.


Subject(s)
Personality Development , Personality , Temperament , Twins, Dizygotic/psychology , Twins, Monozygotic/psychology , Twins/psychology , Child, Preschool , Humans , Personality Tests
15.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) ; 34(1-2): 15-31, 1985.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-4050292

ABSTRACT

The predictive relationship between neonatal behavior and temperament at 6 months was assessed in 62 pairs of twins. The neonatal assessment evaluated irritability, resistance to soothing, reactivity, and activity level across various activities. Temperament at 6 months was appraised by a laboratory assessment, ratings on the Infant Behavior Record, and a questionnaire completed by the mother. Examination for temperament consistency from the neonatal period to 6 months indicated that neonates rated by an examiner as irritable and difficult to soothe were reported by the mother to be more negative, irritable, and slower to adapt at 6 months; neonates rated as more irritable and active were more attentive and responsive during mental testing at 6 months; there was no relationship between the neonatal measures and the laboratory ratings, possibly reflecting methodological problems influenced by maturation and vegetative processes at 6 months. Analyses for patterns of concordance within the various data sets for MZ and DZ twins indicated that there was no pattern of differential concordance favoring MZ twins for the temperament measures at birth or at 6 months. It was concluded that a modest predictive relationship between temperament variables had been demonstrated from birth to 6 months, but no specific genetic effects were detected at these early ages.


Subject(s)
Personality , Temperament , Twins/psychology , Attention/physiology , Behavior/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Female , Humans , Infant , Infant, Newborn , Male , Movement , Surveys and Questionnaires , Twins, Dizygotic , Twins, Monozygotic
17.
Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) ; 33(2): 181-9, 1984.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6540950

ABSTRACT

In the Louisville Twin Study, laboratory observations of twin infants' temperament at 12, 18, and 24 months were linked with parental ratings from temperament questionnaires. Core dimensions of temperament were extracted by factor analysis applied to each set of measures at each age. The laboratory temperament dimension was recurrently represented by emotional tone, social orientation, attentiveness, and reaction to restraint. The questionnaire temperament dimension was recurrently represented by mood, approach/withdrawal and adaptability. The laboratory and questionnaire dimensions were found to be correlated at each age (convergent correlations: 0.38 to 0.52) and to be stable across ages (stability correlations 0.37 to 0.66). The temperament dimensions were used to demonstrate that temperament profiles were more concordant for identical than for fraternal twin pairs. The results demonstrate the genetic influences on the primary dimensions of temperament and the developmental transformations of temperament.


Subject(s)
Personality , Temperament , Twins/psychology , Child, Preschool , Female , Genetics, Behavioral , Humans , Infant , Longitudinal Studies , Personality Development , Pregnancy , Twins, Dizygotic/psychology , Twins, Monozygotic/psychology
18.
Child Dev ; 54(2): 356-60, 1983 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6683619

ABSTRACT

Factor scores from Bayley's Infant Behavior Record, obtained from about 300-400 infant twins at 6, 12, 18, and 24 months, were selected to represent 3 aspects of infant behavior: task orientation, test affect-extraversion, and activity. Age-to-age correlations for the scores yielded a simplex pattern of low to moderate order. Further age-to-age analyses by twin pairs generally showed that the identical twins, as compared with same-sex fraternal twins, generated more within-pair similarities for profiles of the factor scores across 2 overlapping age ranges: 6-18 months, and 12-24 months. Overall, the findings indicated that there is reordering of individual differences for aspects of infant behavior from one age to the next, but the sequence of reordering is somewhat structured in accord with genotypic similarity. The longitudinal findings are also discussed within the context of change and continuity of the individuality of infant temperament.


Subject(s)
Behavior/physiology , Twins/psychology , Child, Preschool , Extraversion, Psychological , Female , Genetics, Behavioral , Humans , Infant , Longitudinal Studies , Motor Activity/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Pregnancy , Temperament , Time Factors , Twins, Dizygotic , Twins, Monozygotic
19.
Child Dev ; 52(2): 579-88, 1981 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7195794

ABSTRACT

Semistructured interviews with 305 mothers of same-sex twins were conducted periodically over the course of the twins' first 6 years. During each of the interviews, the mothers were asked to contrast the twins on a variety of specific behaviors. From the body of reports, 5 ages and 16 behaviors were selected for analysis as most illustrative of the main developmental trends for intrapair differences and similarities. For each of the behaviors, intrapair contrasts were moderately stable across age spans as long as 18-36 months. A set of behaviors related to negative aspects of temperament and a set of behaviors related to sociability were obtained at 6 months and were replicated at later ages. Concordance rates for the identical twin pairs were uniformly higher than for the fraternal pairs, suggesting that genotypic similarity was associated with intrapair similarity for aspects of emotionality and sociability.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior , Twins/psychology , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Pregnancy , Social Behavior , Temperament , Twins, Dizygotic/psychology , Twins, Monozygotic/psychology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...