Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 59
Filter
1.
Vaccine ; 31(2): 362-6, 2013 Jan 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23146674

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Intense efforts to vaccinate pregnant women against 2009 H1N1 influenza resulted in much higher vaccine uptake than previously reported. We surveyed postpartum women to determine whether high vaccination rates were sustained during the 2010-11 influenza season. METHODS: We performed cross-sectional surveys of postpartum women delivering at our institution during February-April 2010 and February-March 2011. The surveys ascertained maternal characteristics, history of influenza vaccination, and reasons for lack of vaccination. RESULTS: During the 2010-11 season, 165 (55%) of 300 women surveyed reported receiving influenza vaccination, compared to 191 of 307 (62%) during 2009-10 (p=0.08). Vaccination by an obstetrical provider was common, but decreased compared to 2009-10 (60% vs. 71%, p=0.04). While most women (76%) in 2010-11 reported that their provider recommended influenza vaccination, significantly more reported lack of discussion about vaccination (24% vs. 11%, p<0.01) compared to 2009-10. Vaccine safety concerns were cited by most (66%) women declining vaccination during 2009-10 but only 27% of women who declined in 2010-11. CONCLUSION: The vaccination rate among pregnant women at our institution was relatively sustained, although fewer providers appear to be discussing influenza vaccination in pregnancy. Concern about vaccine safety, the primary barrier during 2009-10, was much less prominent.


Subject(s)
Influenza A Virus, H1N1 Subtype/immunology , Influenza Vaccines/administration & dosage , Influenza, Human/prevention & control , Pregnancy Complications, Infectious/prevention & control , Adult , Cross-Sectional Studies , Data Collection , Female , Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice , Humans , Influenza Vaccines/adverse effects , Influenza Vaccines/immunology , Influenza, Human/immunology , Patient Acceptance of Health Care , Postpartum Period/immunology , Pregnancy , Pregnancy Complications, Infectious/immunology , Pregnancy Complications, Infectious/virology , Seasons , Vaccination
3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 273(1582): 91-9, 2006 Jan 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16519240

ABSTRACT

Analysis of the colour and motion of objects is widely believed to take place within segregated processing pathways in the primate visual system. However, it is apparent that this segregation cannot remain absolute and that there must be some capacity for integration across these sub-modalities. In this study, we have assessed the extent to which colour constitutes a separable entity in human motion processing by measuring the chromatic selectivity of two kinds of after-effect resulting from motion adaptation. First, the traditional motion after-effect, where prolonged inspection of a unidirectional moving stimulus results in illusory motion in the opposite direction, was found to exhibit a high degree of chromatic selectivity. The second type of after-effect, in which motion adaptation induces misperceptions in the spatial position of stationary objects, was completely insensitive to chromatic composition. This dissociation between the chromatic selectivities of these after-effects shows that chromatic inputs remain segregated at early stages of motion analysis, while at higher levels of cortical processing there is integration across chromatic, as well as achromatic inputs, to produce a unified perceptual output.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Figural Aftereffect , Motion Perception/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological , Color Perception Tests , Humans , Photic Stimulation
4.
Vis Neurosci ; 21(3): 275-82, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15518200

ABSTRACT

We have studied the influence of chromatic adaptation upon the perceived visual position of a test stimulus using a Vernier alignment task. Maximum and minimum offsets in spatial position are generated when the adapting and test stimuli lie on the same and orthogonal axes in MBDKL color space, respectively. When the test stimuli lie on intermediate color axes, the measured positional shifts decrease as a function of the angular separation in color space (phi) from the adapting stimulus. At low stimulus contrasts, these shifts follow a sinusoidal function of phi and exhibit broad chromatic tuning and can be accounted for by a model in which the centroid is extracted from the linear combination of after-image, formed by the adapting stimulus, and the test stimulus. Such linear, broadband behavior is consistent with the response properties of chromatic neurons in the precortical visual pathway. At high contrast, and when adaptation gets closer to the S/(L+M) axis, the tuning functions become narrower and require sinusoids raised to increasingly higher exponents in order to describe the data. This narrowing of chromatic tuning is consistent with the tuning properties of chromatic neurons in the striate cortex, and implies the operation of a nonlinear mechanism in the combination of cone outputs.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Vision Tests
5.
Vision Res ; 44(25): 2875-84, 2004 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15380993

ABSTRACT

Auditory signals have been shown to exert a marked influence on visual perception in a wide range of tasks. However, the mechanisms of these interactions are, at present, poorly understood. Here we present a series of experiments where a temporal cue within the auditory domain can significantly affect the localisation of a moving visual target. To investigate the mechanism of this interaction, we first modulated the spatial positional uncertainty of the visual target by varying its size. When visual positional uncertainty was low (small target size), auditory signals had little or no influence on perceived visual location. However, with increasing visual uncertainty (larger target sizes), auditory signals exerted a significantly greater influence on perceived visual location. We then altered the temporal profile of the auditory signal by modulating the spread of its Gaussian temporal envelope. Introducing this temporal uncertainty to the auditory signal greatly reduced its effect on visual localisation judgements. These findings support the view that the relative uncertainty in individual sensory domains governs the perceptual outcome of multisensory integration.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Motion Perception , Uncertainty , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Cues , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods , Sensory Thresholds
6.
Vision Res ; 41(23): 2951-60, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11704234

ABSTRACT

The purpose of the study was to determine whether amblyopes show detection loss for second-order spatial information, and if present, whether the loss is explained by the loss of first-order spatial input. We psychophysically determined detection thresholds for the amblyopic and non-amblyopic eyes of five adult amblyopes and the dominant eyes of three control observers. We found that four amblyopic eyes and two non-amblyopic eyes showed second-order loss relative to the control eyes. The second-order loss was greater than the first-order loss at the carrier spatial frequency (first-order input). The extra second-order loss indicates an early amplification of cortical neural loss that we speculate is due to deficient binocular input to second-order neurons.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Amblyopia/complications , Anisometropia/complications , Anisometropia/physiopathology , Case-Control Studies , Differential Threshold/physiology , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Psychometrics , Psychophysics , Strabismus/complications , Strabismus/physiopathology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology
7.
AJNR Am J Neuroradiol ; 22(8): 1583-6, 2001 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11559511

ABSTRACT

Phenylketonuria (PKU) is an autosomal recessive disorder caused by a deficiency of the enzyme phenylalanine hydroxylase (EC 1.14.16.1). Affected patients develop elevated plasma and tissue levels of phenylalanine and its related ketoacids. Untreated patients usually exhibit severe mental retardation and poor motor function, with characteristic T2 white matter signal abnormalities on conventional MR images. In the present study, we performed diffusion-weighted imaging in three PKU patients. All three patients demonstrated significantly restricted diffusion in all white matter areas examined.


Subject(s)
Brain/pathology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Phenylketonurias/diagnosis , Adult , Diffusion , Humans , Male
8.
Neuroimaging Clin N Am ; 11(2): 343-53, x, 2001 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11489743

ABSTRACT

Functional MR imaging (fMRI) has been a useful tool in the evaluation of language both in normal individuals and patient populations. The purpose of this article is to use various models of language as a framework to review fMRI studies. Specifically, fMRI language studies are subdivided into the following categories: word generation or fluency, passive listening, orthography, phonology, semantics, and syntax.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Language , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Speech Perception/physiology , Speech/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Phonetics , Reference Values , Semantics
9.
Plant Physiol ; 124(4): 1800-13, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11115895

ABSTRACT

Saccharomyces cerevisiae opi3 mutant strains do not have the phospholipid N-methyltransferase that catalyzes the two terminal methylations in the phosphatidylcholine (PC) biosynthetic pathway. This results in a build up of the intermediate phosphatidylmonomethylethanolamine, causing a temperature-sensitive growth phenotype. An Arabidopsis cDNA library was used to isolate three overlapping plasmids that complemented the temperature-sensitive phenotype. Phospholipid analysis showed that the presence of the cloned cDNA caused a 65-fold reduction in the level of phosphatidylmonomethylethanolamine and a significant, though not equivalent, increase in the production of PC. Sequence analysis established that the cDNA was not homologous to OPI3 or to CHO2, the only other yeast phospholipid N-methyltransferase, but was similar to several other classes of methyltransferases. S-adenosyl-Met:phospho-base N-methyltransferase assays revealed that the cDNA catalyzed the three sequential methylations of phospho-ethanolamine to form phospho-choline. Phospho-choline is converted to PC by the CDP-choline pathway, explaining the phenotype conferred upon the yeast mutant strain by the cDNA. In accordance with this the gene has been named AtNMT1. The identification of this enzyme and the failure to isolate a plant phospholipid N-methyltransferase suggests that there are fundamental differences between the pathways utilized by yeast and by some plants for synthesis of PC.


Subject(s)
Arabidopsis Proteins , Arabidopsis/genetics , Methyltransferases/genetics , S-Adenosylmethionine/metabolism , Saccharomyces cerevisiae/genetics , Amino Acid Sequence , Arabidopsis/enzymology , Base Sequence , DNA, Complementary/chemistry , DNA, Complementary/genetics , DNA, Complementary/isolation & purification , Genetic Complementation Test , Molecular Sequence Data , Mutation , Phenotype , Phospholipids/metabolism , Sequence Alignment , Sequence Analysis, DNA , Sequence Homology, Amino Acid
11.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 20(3): 173-84, 2000 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10897339

ABSTRACT

Whilst the methodology of adult letter acuity measurement has been substantially refined over the last two decades, relatively little development has occurred in methods for quantifying letter acuity in young children. This study compares a recently developed visual acuity test (Glasgow Acuity Cards), which incorporates several key design features used in adult test charts to improve the sensitivity and reliability of visual acuity measurements. The equivalence of acuity measurements made with Glasgow Acuity Cards were compared with the Bailey-Lovie logMAR chart and Snellen chart in adults, and with traditional Single Letter Acuity and a modified Single Letter Acuity test in children. The test-retest reliability of acuity measurements made with Glasgow Acuity Cards and the Single Letter Acuity tests were also assessed in a large group of visually normal children. In addition, the ability of the pre-school letter acuity tests to detect differences in acuity between the two eyes, and to detect amblyopia were examined. Ninety-five percent of vision measurements made with the Bailey-Lovie chart and Glasgow Acuity Cards differ by less than 0.07 log unit. Furthermore, the sensitivity of Glasgow Acuity Cards to detecting changes in acuity longitudinally and inter-ocular differences in acuity is considerably greater as compared with traditional Single Letter Acuity tests. Improvements in paediatric acuity chart design are important for the effective detection and management of children with amblyopia.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/diagnosis , Vision Tests/standards , Visual Acuity , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Child , Child, Preschool , Humans , Middle Aged , Reproducibility of Results , Sensitivity and Specificity , Vision Tests/methods
12.
Vision Res ; 40(8): 973-88, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10720667

ABSTRACT

The present paper asks whether Vernier offset discrimination is limited by the observer's sensitivity to local contrast change in both central and peripheral vision. To answer this question we compared Vernier discrimination and contrast discrimination thresholds (specified in the same units) for a pair of narrow ribbons of cosine gratings. Because the ribbons are narrow, both the offset information (for Vernier discrimination) and the contrast information (for contrast discrimination) are highly localized. We found that when the stimuli are narrow ribbons, the local contrast cue is the limiting factor in Vernier discrimination. However, our results also show that integration of information along the length of the gratings (the ribbon width) is: (i) different for Vernier and contrast discrimination, and (ii) for Vernier discrimination the integration of information along the length of the gratings differs qualitatively in central and peripheral vision. For narrow ribbons, the peripheral 'template' for ribbon Vernier acuity is not as well matched to the stimulus (in two-dimensional spatial frequency space) as the foveal 'template'.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds , Visual Fields/physiology
13.
Br J Ophthalmol ; 84(2): 165-70, 2000 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10655192

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Despite the fact that visual function has an important role in the quality of life in later years, very few studies have measured visual acuity in population based nationwide samples of British elderly people. Such measurements were carried out in the context of the national diet and nutrition survey of people aged 65 years or over (NDNS). METHODS: NDNS participants, who were living in 80 different randomly selected postcode areas of mainland Britain, were visited at their home by a nurse who measured visual acuity at 3 metres, using the Glasgow acuity card (GAC) method. In addition, a brief questionnaire related to ocular health was administered. RESULTS: Visual acuity was measured in 1362 NDNS participants who were not classified as mentally impaired. Visual impairment (using the WHO low vision criteria) was measured in 195 (14.3%) subjects. Prevalence of visual impairment increased significantly with age (65-74 years 3.1%; 75-84 years 11.6%; 85+ years 35.5%, p<0.001 for trend). Impaired vision was more common in subjects living in a nursing home (odds ratio adjusted for age 2.59 (95% CI 2.23 to 2. 96)) and in women (odds ratio adjusted for age 1.55 (95% CI 1.21 to 1.89)). 132 (9.7%) subjects had previously undergone cataract surgery and another 157 (11.5%) had been told that they currently had cataract. Vision improved 0.2 log units or more (at least one Snellen line) with the aid of a pinhole occluder in 289 subjects (21. 2%). CONCLUSION: Results of this nationwide, community based study confirm that problems with poor distance visual acuity exist in a substantial part of the elderly community, particularly in women and people living in nursing homes.


Subject(s)
Vision Disorders/epidemiology , Visual Acuity/physiology , Aged , Feasibility Studies , Female , Health Surveys , Humans , Male , Nursing Homes , Nutrition Surveys , Prevalence , United Kingdom/epidemiology , Vision Screening , Visually Impaired Persons
15.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 40(12): 2859-71, 1999 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10549646

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The aim of this study was to define the nature of functional visual loss in amblyopia and to identify those subjects whose amblyopia is chiefly due to one or more of the following deficits: abnormal contour interaction, abnormal eye movements, abnormal contrast perception, or positional uncertainty. METHODS: Fifty amblyopic children with a mean age of 5.6+/-1.3 years were referred from diverse sources. In addition to routine orthoptic and optometric evaluation the principal visual deficits in the amblyopic eye of each subject were identified using the following measures of visual acuity: high contrast linear, single optotype, repeat letter and low contrast linear, plus Vernier and displacement thresholds. These measures were repeated as the children underwent a prescribed occlusion therapy regime, after parental consent. RESULTS: All amblyopic subjects demonstrated a functional loss in each of the tests used, and occlusion therapy appeared to improve all aspects of the amblyopia. High contrast visual acuity was not always the primary deficit in visual function, and when amblyopic subjects were divided according to their primary visual loss, this visual function was found to show the greatest improvement with treatment. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that to successfully identify the primary visual deficit and monitor the success of occlusion therapy it is necessary to assess other aspects of visual function in amblyopia.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/physiopathology , Amblyopia/therapy , Sensory Deprivation , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Child , Child, Preschool , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Eyeglasses , Humans , Sensory Thresholds , Vision Tests , Visual Acuity/physiology
16.
Vision Res ; 39(4): 789-801, 1999 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10341965

ABSTRACT

Gaussian and Gabor patches can be accurately localized; however, it is not yet clear which cues (or location tags) the visual system utilizes for localization. To determine the cues used in spatial alignment, we measured and modelled the perceived shifts for asymmetric Gaussian and Gabor patches over a wide range of separations, patch sizes and orientations. For Gaussian patches we observed perceived shifts that were generally consistent with that of the centroid of the envelope. For Gabor patches we found that the perceived shift depends on the carrier orientation (whether co-axial or orthoaxial with the patch arrangement), separation (in units of carrier wavelength) and patch size (number of cycles per standard deviation). Gabor patches with the carrier orthoaxial (horizontal) to the three vertically arranged patches, were similar to Gaussian patches. However, Gabor patches with the carrier coaxial (vertical) to the three vertically arranged patches resulted in perceived shifts that were consistent with a number of alternate localization primitives. The selection of primitives was dependent on mainly the separation and patch size. Our results support the suggestion that the visual system can use multiple tags for location (Hess et al., Vis Res 1994;34:2439-2451; Badcock et al., Vis Res 1996;36:1467-1472).


Subject(s)
Cues , Space Perception/physiology , Humans , Models, Psychological , Psychological Tests
17.
Nat Neurosci ; 2(5): 479-84, 1999 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10321254

ABSTRACT

The visual system is thought to process luminance (first-order) and contrast (second-order) information by dedicated cortical streams. To explore the spatial characteristics of the second-order pathway, we examined the effect of adaptation on spatial localization in human subjects. We show that, unlike first-order adaptation, second-order positional adaptation via cortical mechanisms transfers across orientations but not across spatial frequencies. These results support physiological evidence that these two processing streams are distinct and suggest that the cortical mechanism mediating second-order positional adaptation maintains spatial frequency information but sums signals across orientations.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Humans , Photic Stimulation
18.
Exp Brain Res ; 125(2): 122-8, 1999 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10204765

ABSTRACT

The visual mechanism by which human observers determine the separation between objects has long been of interest. This study examines the extent to which separation in visual space can be misperceived in foveal and extrafoveal vision. Foveally, vertical separations were consistently overestimated relative to horizontal separations, a result which is consistent with the well-documented horizontal-vertical illusion (HVI). Extrafoveally, much larger misrepresentations of visual space were perceived. In addition, separations tangential to fixation were consistently perceived as being greater than separations in a radial direction. These marked misperceptions of visual space which occur in extrafoveal vision take the form of a radial/tangential anisotropy combined with an overestimation of vertical distance. The results have important implications for meridional anisotropies which have previously been documented in a number of visual performance tasks.


Subject(s)
Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiopathology , Adult , Anisotropy , Fovea Centralis/physiopathology , Humans , Male
19.
Ann Emerg Med ; 33(1): 9-14, 1999 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9867881

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: Thrombolytic therapy has been advocated as an effective treatment for acute ischemic stroke. In an attempt to promote maximum benefit while reducing the risk of hemorrhagic complications, numerous exclusions to the use of thrombolytic therapy for acute ischemic stroke have been promulgated. This study was conducted to identify the number of acute ischemic stroke patients eligible for thrombolytic therapy and to determine the reasons those deemed ineligible were excluded. METHODS: This observational study was conducted from September 15, 1996, to May 1, 1997, at an emergency department with an annual census of 70,000. Patients with a chief complaint suggestive of acute ischemic stroke were categorized as "eligible" if thrombolytic therapy was not contraindicated and could be initiated within 3 hours of symptom onset. Patients were deemed "ineligible" if the time to thrombolytic therapy would have exceeded 3 hours, or if other specific contraindications to thrombolytic therapy were present. For all categories, 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) were determined. RESULTS: Of the 214 patients with acute ischemic stroke who were screened, 6 (2.8%+/-2.2%) were eligible. Ninety-five patients (44%+/-7%) were excluded solely on the basis of the time interval from onset of symptoms to eligibility for thrombolytic therapy exceeding 3 hours. Other common reasons for exclusion included resolution of symptoms in 31 patients (14%+/-4%), intracranial hemorrhage as determined by head computed tomography in 22 (10%+/-4%), and minor symptoms in 20 (9%+/-4%). CONCLUSION: The majority of acute ischemic stroke patients do not meet accepted criteria for thrombolytic therapy. Most are ineligible because of delays in obtaining treatment. Strategies should be devised to reduce the time to treatment if thrombolytic therapy is to achieve widespread use in the treatment of acute ischemic stroke.


Subject(s)
Brain Ischemia/drug therapy , Emergency Service, Hospital/statistics & numerical data , Medical Futility , Thrombolytic Therapy/statistics & numerical data , Acute Disease , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Delaware , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prospective Studies , Time Factors
20.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 19(3): 253-60, 1999 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10627844

ABSTRACT

We investigate the influence of stimulus contrast upon contour interaction in normal and amblyopic subjects. Using a computer generated acuity task, flanked and unflanked acuities were measured psychometrically at both high contrast (80%) and low contrast (6%), in a group of 19 normal and 11 amblyopic subjects. The crowding ratio for high contrast letters was found to be significantly higher than that for low contrast letters. The extent of the crowding zone was measured at high and low contrast by varying the separation of the optotype and flanking bars. The crowding zone measurement was repeated for the high contrast optotypes using dioptric blur. The position of the flanking contours was found to have a significant effect on letter resolution at high contrast but no significant effect was demonstrable at low contrast. With the addition of dioptric blur the effect of contour interaction became negligible at high contrast. These findings support the hypothesis that the crowding effect is: (1) similar in normal and amblyopic eyes when tested at threshold; (2) is contrast dependent appearing only for high contrast optotypes.


Subject(s)
Amblyopia/psychology , Contrast Sensitivity , Adult , Amblyopia/physiopathology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychometrics , Sensory Thresholds , Visual Acuity
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...