Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 28(1): 10-34, 2022 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34435848

ABSTRACT

Healthcare workers often monitor patients while moving between different locations and tasks, and away from conventional monitoring displays. Vibrotactile displays can provide patient information in vibrotactile patterns that are felt regardless of the worker's location. We examined how effectively participants could identify changes in vibrotactile representations of patient heart rate (HR) and oxygen saturation (SpO2). In Experiment 1, participants identified changes in HR and SpO2 with greater than 90% accuracy while using vibrotactile displays configured in either an integrated or a separated format. In Experiment 2, incidental auditory and visual cues were removed and performance was still greater than 90% for the integrated display. In Experiments 3 and 4, ongoing tasks with low or high task load were introduced; high load worsened participants' response accuracy and speed at identifying vital signs. In Experiments 5 and 6, alternative designs were tested, including a design with a seemingly more natural mapping of HR to vibrotactile stimulation. However, no design supported more accurate performance than the integrated display. Results are interpreted with respect to multiple resource theory, and constraints on conforming to design guidelines are noted. Vibrotactile displays appear to be viable and therefore potentially suitable for use in healthcare and other contexts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Cues , Vital Signs , Humans , Monitoring, Physiologic , Vibration
2.
Resuscitation ; 153: 28-34, 2020 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32504769

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Early warning tools have been widely implemented without evidence to guide (a) recognition and (b) response team expertise optimisation. With growing databases from MET-calls and digital hospitals, we now have access to guiding information. The Queensland Adult-Deterioration-Detection-System (Q-ADDS) is widely used and requires validation. AIM: Compare the accuracy of Q-ADDS to National Early Warning Score (NEWS), Between-the-Flags (BTF) and the electronic Cardiac Arrest Risk Triage Score (eCART)). METHODS: Data from the Chicago University hospital database were used. Clinical deterioration was defined as unplanned admission to ICU or death. Currently used NEWS, BTF and eCART trigger thresholds were compared with a clinically endorsed Q-ADDS variant. RESULTS: Of 224,912 admissions, 11,706 (5%) experienced clinical deterioration. Q-ADDS (AUC 0.71) and NEWS (AUC 0.72) had similar predictive accuracy, BTF (AUC 0.64) had the lowest, and eCART (AUC 0.76) the highest. Early warning alert (advising ward MO review) had similar NPV (99.2-99.3%), for all the four tools however sensitivity varied (%: Q-ADDS = 47/NEWS = 49/BTF = 66/eCART = 40), as did alerting rate (% vitals sets: Q-ADDS = 1.4/NEWS = 3.5/BTF = 4.1/eCART = 3.4). MET alert (advising MET/critical-care review) had similar NPV for all the four tools (99.1-99.2%), however sensitivity varied (%: Q-ADDS = 14/NEWS = 24/BTF = 19/eCART = 29), as did MET alerting rate (%: Q-ADDS = 1.4/NEWS = 3.5/BTF = 4.1/eCART = 3.4). High-severity alert (advising advanced ward review, Q-ADDS only): NPV = 99.1%, sensitivity = 26%, alerting rate = 3.5%. CONCLUSION: The accuracy of Q-ADDS is comparable to NEWS, and higher than BTF, with eCART being the most accurate. Q-ADDS provides an additional high-severity ward alert, and generated significantly fewer MET alerts. Impacts of increased ward awareness and fewer MET alerts on actual MET call numbers and patient outcomes requires further evaluation.


Subject(s)
Clinical Deterioration , Heart Arrest , Adult , Humans , Chicago , Electronics , Hospital Mortality , Queensland/epidemiology , Retrospective Studies , Risk Assessment , Triage
3.
Resuscitation ; 105: 41-4, 2016 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27224448

ABSTRACT

AIMS: Time without ventilation is often much longer than an intubation attempt, yet patient stability relies on effective gas exchange. We argue that in addition to existing performance criteria, intubation performance measures should include interruption to effective ventilation. METHODS: We reviewed video recorded resuscitations of 31 term and preterm newborns that included at least one intubation attempt. Time stamps were recorded at the end of mask ventilation, laryngoscope insertion and removal (laryngoscope duration), and re-commencement of ventilation via mask or endotracheal tube (ETT). Intubation attempts were defined as Successful (subsequent ventilation via ETT), or Failed (ETT incorrectly placed) or Withdrawn (laryngoscope removed before ETT insertion attempt). RESULTS: During intubation, total time without ventilation varied from 31 to 273s, compared to laryngoscope duration of 12-149s. Time without ventilation as Median [min-max] was greater for failed attempts 64 [48-273]s, yet laryngoscope duration was shortest for failed attempts 33 [21-46]s. Time between ceasing ventilation and commencing intubation was 5 [1-46]s suggesting room for improvement during transitions within the procedure. CONCLUSIONS: Time without ventilation is a more physiologically important measure of a resuscitation team's intubation expertise than laryngoscope duration. Since successful attempts took longer than failed attempts, emphasising haste during vocal cord visualisation and tube insertion may reduce success rates. Reducing the time without ventilation at either end of the procedure may be achievable with better team coordination and could be just as important to patient wellbeing as technical precision.


Subject(s)
Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation/methods , Intubation, Intratracheal/statistics & numerical data , Laryngoscopy/statistics & numerical data , Pulmonary Ventilation , Resuscitation/methods , Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation/mortality , Clinical Competence , Humans , Infant, Newborn , Infant, Premature , Intubation, Intratracheal/adverse effects , Intubation, Intratracheal/standards , Laryngeal Masks , Laryngoscopy/adverse effects , Patient Care Team , Retrospective Studies , Time Factors , Video Recording
4.
PLoS One ; 9(2): e87677, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24504048

ABSTRACT

People who are high in causal uncertainty doubt their own ability to understand the causes of social events. In three studies, we examined the effects of target and perceiver causal uncertainty on attitudes toward the target. Target causal uncertainty was manipulated via responses on a causal uncertainty scale in Studies 1 and 2, and with a scenario in Study 3. In Studies 1 and 2, we found that participants liked the low causal uncertainty target more than the high causal uncertainty target. This preference was stronger for low relative to high causal uncertainty participants because high causal uncertainty participants held more uncertain ideals. In Study 3, we examined the value individuals place upon causal understanding (causal importance) as an additional moderator. We found that regardless of their own causal uncertainty level, participants who were high in causal importance liked the low causal uncertainty target more than the high causal uncertainty target. However, when participants were low in causal importance, low causal uncertainty perceivers showed no preference and high causal uncertainty perceivers preferred the high causal uncertainty target. These findings reveal that goal importance and ideals can influence how perceivers respond to causal uncertainty in others.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Uncertainty , Adolescent , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...