Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 6 de 6
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Front Psychol ; 1: 191, 2010.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21833251

ABSTRACT

Zero-sum bias describes intuitively judging a situation to be zero-sum (i.e., resources gained by one party are matched by corresponding losses to another party) when it is actually non-zero-sum. The experimental participants were students at a university where students' grades are determined by how the quality of their work compares to a predetermined standard of quality rather than to the quality of the work produced by other students. This creates a non-zero-sum situation in which high grades are an unlimited resource. In three experiments, participants were shown the grade distribution after a majority of the students in a course had completed an assigned presentation, and asked to predict the grade of the next presenter. When many high grades had already been given, there was a corresponding increase in low grade predictions. This suggests a zero-sum bias, in which people perceive a competition for a limited resource despite unlimited resource availability. Interestingly, when many low grades had already been given, there was not a corresponding increase in high grade predictions. This suggests that a zero-sum heuristic is only applied in response to the allocation of desirable resources. A plausible explanation for the findings is that a zero-sum heuristic evolved as a cognitive adaptation to enable successful intra-group competition for limited resources. Implications for understanding inter-group interaction are also discussed.

2.
Am J Bioeth ; 8(1): 9-20, 2008 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18236327

ABSTRACT

There is considerable interest in the use of neuroimaging techniques for forensic purposes. Memory detection techniques, including the well-publicized Brain Fingerprinting technique (Brain Fingerprinting Laboratories, Inc., Seattle WA), exploit the fact that the brain responds differently to sensory stimuli to which it has been exposed before. When a stimulus is specifically associated with a crime, the resulting brain activity should differentiate between someone who was present at the crime and someone who was not. This article reviews the scientific literature on three such techniques: priming, old/new, and P300 effects. The forensic potential of these techniques is evaluated based on four criteria: specificity, automaticity, encoding flexibility, and longevity. This article concludes that none of the techniques are devoid of forensic potential, although much research is yet to be done. Ethical issues, including rights to privacy and against self-incrimination, are discussed. A discussion of legal issues concludes that current memory detection techniques do not yet meet United States standards of legal admissibility.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Science/ethics , Criminal Law , Forensic Psychiatry/ethics , Forensic Psychiatry/methods , Lie Detection , Memory , Neurosciences/ethics , Privacy , Automatism , Crime/psychology , Criminal Law/ethics , Criminal Law/methods , Criminal Law/standards , Criminal Law/trends , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials , False Negative Reactions , False Positive Reactions , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Privacy/legislation & jurisprudence , Sensitivity and Specificity , Time Factors , United States
3.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 32(2): 413-22, 2006 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16634679

ABSTRACT

Two experiments used Müller-Lyer stimuli to test the predictions of the planning-control model (S. Glover, 2002) for aiming movements. In Experiment 1, participants aimed to stimuli that either remained the same or changed upon movement initiation. Experiment 2 was identical except that the duration of visual feedback for online control was manipulated. The authors found that the figures visible during movement planning and online control had additive effects on endpoint bias, even when participants had ample time to use visual feedback to modify their movements (Experiment 2). These findings are problematic not only for the planning-control model but also for A. D. Milner and M. A. Goodale's (1995) two visual system explanation of illusory bias. Although our results are consistent with the idea that a single representation is used for perception, movement planning, and online control (e.g., V. H. Franz, 2001), other work from our laboratory and elsewhere suggests that the manner in which space is coded depends on constraints associated with the specific task, such as the visual cues available to the performer.


Subject(s)
Distance Perception/physiology , Feedback, Psychological/physiology , Motor Skills/physiology , Optical Illusions , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Humans , Intention , Movement/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reference Values
4.
Neuroimage ; 25(4): 1043-55, 2005 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15850723

ABSTRACT

Many neuroimaging studies have been designed to differentiate domain-specific processes in the brain. A common design constraint is to use identical stimuli for different domain-specific tasks. For example, an experiment investigating spatial versus identity processing would present compound spatial-identity stimuli in both spatial and identity tasks, and participants would be instructed to attend to, encode, maintain, or retrieve spatial information in the spatial task, and identity information in the identity task. An assumption in such studies is that spatial information will not be processed in the identity task, as it is irrelevant for that task. We report three experiments demonstrating violations of this assumption. Our results suggest that comparisons of spatial and identity tasks in existing neuroimaging studies have underestimated the amount of brain activation that is spatial-specific. For future neuroimaging studies, we recommend unique stimulus displays for each domain-specific task, and event-related measurement of post-stimulus processing.


Subject(s)
Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Calibration , Diagnostic Imaging , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Imagination/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory/physiology , Reading , Research Design , Visual Perception/physiology
5.
Exp Brain Res ; 155(1): 37-47, 2004 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15064883

ABSTRACT

Participants made perceptual judgments about the length of, and manual aiming movements to the opposite end of, formerly visible Müller-Lyer stimuli. The Müller-Lyer illusion affected both perceptual judgments and aiming amplitude. Manipulations of stimulus duration (10 ms or 3000 ms) and memory delay length (10 ms or 3000 ms) had no impact on the illusory effect. Aiming movements executed with vision of the hand were less affected by the illusion than movements executed without vision of the hand. The effect of the illusion on aiming amplitude remained the same between peak velocity and the end of the movement even though participants were engaged in on-line control between peak deceleration and the end of the movement. This latter finding was counter to the predictions of a hypothesis (Glover 2002) stating that illusions should only affect the early (planning) stages of movement and not the late (control) stages of movement. We conclude that a single visual representation is used for perception, motor planning, and motor control.


Subject(s)
Illusions/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Feedback/physiology , Humans , Memory/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods
6.
Neuroimage ; 21(3): 1026-36, 2004 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15006670

ABSTRACT

When functional neuroimaging researchers draw conclusions about the sensory, cognitive, or motor processes that are associated with changes in brain activity, they are making assumptions about the component processes involved in performing a complex behavioral task. We demonstrate the danger in making such assumptions using, as an example, the n-back task, which has been widely used in neuroimaging studies of working memory. Neuroimaging researchers have assumed that the letter n-back task only engages processes involved in the short-term maintenance and manipulation of verbal information. We report three behavioral experiments demonstrating that the letter n-back task additionally recruits spatial processes. A fourth experiment suggested that the location n-back task may recruit verbal processes in addition to spatial processes. These results call into question conclusions that have been drawn about the neural basis of working memory. More broadly, our results demonstrate that task analysis is a vital partner of neuroimaging in the cognitive neuroscience enterprise.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Nervous System Physiological Phenomena , Nervous System/anatomy & histology , Task Performance and Analysis , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Verbal Learning/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL