Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 110(3): 451-473, 2018 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30270438

ABSTRACT

In tests of the derivation of complex relations such as transitivity, extant cues might fail to evoke effective responding, necessitating the construction of supplemental stimuli prior to their solution. The significance of this process was investigated by within-subject manipulation of an instructional variable designed to produce different levels of construction of supplemental stimulation concerning relationships among stimulus elements of concatenated conditional discriminations. In two experimental sessions, serial training of three 5-member stimulus classes occurred, either with the instruction to simply name the component stimuli or to both name them and generate a tale serially linking the stimuli named; such constructed stimuli might be spontaneously reconstructed by precurrent acts in subsequent tests of "emergent" relations. Participants whose supplemental stimulus construction at the first session was limited to name-giving derived significantly more relations when, in training at session two, they generated tales linking stimulus elements; this same near-errorless derivation was obtained at the first session whenever relational stimuli beyond bidirectional naming were constructed. In some cases the uninstructed construction of supplementary relational stimuli occurred at the first session, to equivalent effect; such construction might constitute a typically unobserved component of the derivation of relations among stimulus elements entailed in multiple conditional discriminations.


Subject(s)
Problem Solving , Reinforcement, Psychology , Acoustic Stimulation , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Humans , Learning , Male , Photic Stimulation , Time Factors
2.
J ECT ; 31(4): 246-52, 2015 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25973768

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: This study is a follow-up of a previous one reporting that the neuropsychological profile of pharmacoresistant patients with major depressive disorder referred for electroconvulsive therapy (ECT, ECT group) contrasted with that of their pharmacorespondent counterparts (NECT group). The NECT group exhibited severe visuospatial memory and minor executive deficits; the ECT group presented the reverse pattern. In that same ECT group, the current follow-up study examined the effects of clinically effective ECT on both cognitive domains 2 months later. METHODS: Fifteen ECT patients were administered Hamilton Depression (HAMD-24), Hamilton Anxiety (HAMA), Mini-Mental State Examination Scales and 5 tests of Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery at intake (pre-ECT), end of ECT course (post-ECT), and 2 months thereafter (follow-up). RESULTS: Electroconvulsive therapy was effective in relieving clinical depression. After a post-ECT decline, the patients exhibited significant improvement in both Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, paired associate learning, and Stockings of Cambridge. By contrast, their major pre-ECT deficit in intra/extradimensional set shifting remained virtually unaffected. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings suggest that attentional flexibility deficits may constitute a neuropsychological trait-like feature of pharmacoresistant, ECT-referred major depressive disorder patients. However, this deficit does not seem generalized, given patient improvement in episodic visual learning/memory and some indication of improvement in spatial planning after ECT.


Subject(s)
Attention , Depressive Disorder, Major/psychology , Depressive Disorder, Major/therapy , Electroconvulsive Therapy/adverse effects , Electroconvulsive Therapy/psychology , Learning , Memory, Episodic , Association Learning , Depressive Disorder, Treatment-Resistant/psychology , Depressive Disorder, Treatment-Resistant/therapy , Executive Function , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Spatial Memory , Treatment Outcome
3.
Int J Emerg Ment Health ; 13(1): 11-26, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21957753

ABSTRACT

Psychological distress is common in the aftermath of a disaster. This study investigated psychological distress and morbidity in individuals who had experienced severe exposure to a wildfire disaster in a part of Greece. The study was a cross sectional case control of an adult population (18-65 years old). Face to face interviews were used in the collection of the data which were demographics, the type and number of losses and the Symptom Checklist 90-Revised for assessment of psychological symptoms. The results showed that those exposed to wildfires disaster scored significantly higher on the symptoms of somatization, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, and paranoia; had significantly more symptoms of psychopathology and were more distressed, compared to controls. Risk factors for potential psychiatric cases were those exposed to disaster; those who had lower education, and those who were widowed. It was concluded that wildfires may cause considerable psychological symptoms comparable to other disasters and there are reasons to create services to help and improve the mental health of those affected.


Subject(s)
Disasters , Fires , Rural Population , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/epidemiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Case-Control Studies , Checklist , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Greece , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Personality Inventory/statistics & numerical data , Psychometrics , Risk Factors , Socioeconomic Factors , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Young Adult
4.
J Anxiety Disord ; 25(6): 829-34, 2011 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21565462

ABSTRACT

Schoolchildren reported their parents' use of aversive control and positive reinforcement contingencies in their educational interventions, as well as parental non-responsiveness to their requests for educational assistance. They also reported their own levels of six dimensions of anxiety disorder-related phenomena. Both parental use of aversive control and non-responsiveness were directly related to overall levels of child anxiety disorder-related behavior; these correlations were more robust than those observed in previous investigations of more diffuse dimensions of parenting style and trait anxiety. Panic disorder/agoraphobia and Generalized anxiety disorder were the dimensions most strongly correlated with both parental aversive control and non-responsiveness, while Compulsive behavior was uniquely uncorrelated with parental non-responsiveness and uniquely correlated with parental use of positive reinforcement contingencies. Differences in the magnitudes of correlations between anxiety disorder-related dimensions and parental educational practices are interpreted in terms of the probable differential effectiveness of their constituent behaviors in terminating parent-mediated negative reinforcers.


Subject(s)
Anxiety Disorders/psychology , Child Behavior/psychology , Parenting/psychology , Parents/psychology , Child , Humans , Parent-Child Relations , Reinforcement, Psychology
5.
Behav Res Ther ; 47(10): 868-75, 2009 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19628202

ABSTRACT

This study provided controlled observations of a potential mechanism for the determination of the repetitive, aberrant perceptions or interpretations of everyday events that figure prominently in a range of psychological disorders: the adventitious reinforcement of acts of cognition by the actual consequences of concurrent motor acts. Adults made a series of two-choice brightness discriminations; on 60% of trials, choosing the brighter stimulus produced a "correct" signal while errors produced an aversive sound. On 40% of trials, the choice stimuli did not in fact differ in brightness; the consequences of responding on these "identical stimuli" trials differed across blocks of trials. Thus, on these trials perceptual judgments were directly followed by events that they did not produce. When all choices on identical stimuli trials were punished with the "error" sound, subjects showed little preference for the left-side or right-side identical stimuli, but when all choices of identical stimuli were reinforced with the "correct" light, individual preferences for the left-side or the right-side stimuli substantially increased. As the consequences of responding on identical stimuli trials were independent of the stimuli chosen, these findings provide evidence for superstitious perception, the reinforcement of perceptual acts by events that do not depend upon their occurrence.


Subject(s)
Attention , Discrimination Learning , Punishment , Reinforcement, Psychology , Superstitions/psychology , Choice Behavior , Emotions , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Visual Perception , Young Adult
6.
J Trauma Stress ; 22(3): 189-96, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19452533

ABSTRACT

This study investigated whether relations between beliefs about the personal controllability of reinforcing events and levels of psychopathology were differentiated with respect to levels of trauma and loss experienced in a series of devastating wildfires. In contrast with studies of combat veterans and professional firefighters, in wildfire survivors external locus of control beliefs and psychopathology were correlated only in respondents who experienced higher levels of trauma and loss; specifically, for residents of designated disaster areas (N = 409), but not for a demographically matched sample of residents of adjacent, non-fire-damaged areas (N = 391). The conflicting findings across studies are interpreted with respect to probable differences in contingencies of reinforcement for causal attributions in professionals and in novices in disaster management.


Subject(s)
Fires , Grief , Internal-External Control , Psychopathology , Survivors/psychology , Wounds and Injuries/physiopathology , Wounds and Injuries/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Female , Greece , Humans , Interviews as Topic , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
7.
Environ Sci Technol ; 41(20): 6901-8, 2007 Oct 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17993126

ABSTRACT

The experiment described in this paper compared the effects of two systems for labeling recycled water on potential consumers' intention to use this resource; the current identifying symbol used worldwide (purple color for pipes, containers, and tanks) and a new, empirically derived and validated set of symbols that provide specific information on water quality and positive comparative information. In total, 807 face-to-face surveys were carried out. Subjects were asked to rate their intentions for using different qualities of recycled water for commercial agriculture as well as for using products and facilities irrigated with recycled water, either in the presence of the established symbol or the new, empirically derived symbols. Potential consumers' self-reported intentions to use recycled water were reliably higher in the presence of empirically derived symbols with positive comparative information than in the presence of the conventional identification symbol. This program of research provides a model for the application of principles of behavioral psychology to enhance the consumer acceptance of technological advances that preserve natural resources and protect ecosystems.


Subject(s)
Conservation of Natural Resources , Water Supply , Data Collection , Probability
8.
J Anxiety Disord ; 21(1): 1-21, 2007.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16650964

ABSTRACT

The generality of the DSM-IV diagnostic structure for children's anxiety disorders, as measured by the Spence Children's Anxiety Scale (SCAS) was investigated with a Greek-language version of the scale. An exploratory factor analysis produced a six-factor solution in general accord with the DSM-IV-based theoretical structure of responding. However, a generalized anxiety factor incorporated three unexpected items interpreted as representing excessive worry, including two items intended to measure obsessions, raising the question of children's ability to discriminate the intrusiveness of vexatious cognition. Anxiety scores were negatively correlated with school adjustment and performance, and decreased with age, with the exception of social phobia scores, which increased. Anxiety scores were substantially higher than those observed in most cultures, particularly on social phobia and compulsive behavior subscales. Hellenic children might regard compulsive behaviors as more socially acceptable than other anxiety disorder-related behaviors, whereas higher overall anxiety scores appear to be related to socio-economic circumstances.


Subject(s)
Achievement , Anxiety Disorders/ethnology , Child Behavior Disorders/ethnology , Cognition Disorders/ethnology , Compulsive Behavior/ethnology , Phobic Disorders/ethnology , Social Adjustment , Anxiety Disorders/diagnosis , Anxiety Disorders/psychology , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Compulsive Behavior/diagnosis , Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , Disease Progression , Ethnicity/statistics & numerical data , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Greece/epidemiology , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Phobic Disorders/diagnosis , Phobic Disorders/psychology , Pilot Projects , Prevalence , Psychometrics/statistics & numerical data , Reproducibility of Results , Severity of Illness Index , Surveys and Questionnaires
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...