Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 25
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Ir Med J ; 111(6): 769, 2018 06 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30518784

ABSTRACT

Using telecommunications technology it would be possible to link a patient and paramedic to a Doctor in the Emergency Department (ED) at the point of first patient contact. A questionnaire-based study on telemedicine in the pre-hospital environment involving patients, paramedics, doctors and nurses in the ED, was performed to assess if they would want and accept telemedicine in pre-hospital emergency care. When asked 98.5% (55) of patients, 89% (11) of doctors, 76% (14) of nurses and 91% (42) of ambulance personnel saw the potential of an audio-visual link from the pre-hospital environment to the ED. The potential benefits were felt to be in diagnosis of time-dependent illnesses, time management, increased hospital preparedness for incoming patients and increased triage efficiency. Stakeholder enthusiasm for pre-hospital telemedicine must be met with the technological requirements to provide such a service. As noted by one patient a pre-hospital audio-visual link to the ED could be "potentially a life saving service".

2.
J Child Lang ; 25(1): 121-47, 1998 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9604571

ABSTRACT

An empirical puzzle regarding toddlers' fast mapping motivated the current investigation. Whereas children between 1;10 and 2;1 have shown only a modest rate of mapping novel nouns onto unfamiliar rather than familiar objects, a very high rate has been observed in those between 1;4 and 1;8 (Mervis & Bertrand, 1991). Study 1 examined whether young two-year-olds (N = 40, mean age = 2;1) might map at a higher rate when tested with procedures unique to Mervis & Bertrand's assessment--strong corrective feedback rather than mild positive non-contingent feedback; large sets of test objects rather than pairs; presentation of easier tests first. Only the first variable affected performance in a manner that could solve the puzzle. Unfamiliar kinds were selected at a much higher rate under corrective (0.86) than non-contingent (0.57) feedback. Although nearly every child in the non-contingent group chose correctly on the first trial, many failed to do so thereafter. In Study 2, rather than presenting a test word to the children (N = 16, mean age = 2;2), the experimenter merely asked for 'the one I want'. Unfamiliar kinds were selected much less often than in Study 1, suggesting that at least one lexical principle proposed in the literature underlies the noun mapping preference. Changes over trials in the two studies indicated that the noun mapping preference is quite prevalent, but unless initial choices are strongly reinforced, an increase in the salience of familiar kinds after the first trial lures some children into error. Consistent with this analysis, toddlers in Study 3 (N = 24, mean age = 2:1) who received non-contingent strong acceptance for their noun mapping decisions, selected unfamiliar kinds more often than those who had received non-contingent mild acceptance in Study 1.


Subject(s)
Child Language , Language Development , Vocabulary , Age Factors , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male
3.
Child Dev ; 68(2): 211-28, 1997 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9179999

ABSTRACT

Children under 2 1/2 years old tend to interpret novel words in accordance with the Mutual Exclusivity Principle, but tend not to reinterpret familiar words this way. Because alternative principles have been proposed that only predict the novel word effects, and because tests of the familiar word effects may have been flawed, a new test was administered. In Experiment 1 (N = 32), 24- to 25-month-olds heard stories in which a novel noun was used for an atypical exemplar of a familiar noun. When asked to select exemplars of the familiar noun, they showed a small but reliable tendency to avoid the object from the story. In Experiment 2 (N = 16), the novel nouns in the stories were replaced by pronouns and proper names, and the children did not avoid the story object in the test of the familiar noun. Thus, the aversion to this object that was observed in Experiment 1 was not due to its greater exposure or its being referenced immediately before testing, but to toddlers' Mutual Exclusivity bias. Their bias is hypothesized to be a form of implicit probabilistic knowledge that derives from the competitive nature of category retrieval.


Subject(s)
Attention , Language Development , Verbal Learning , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Semantics , Vocabulary
4.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 63(3): 466-98, 1996 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8953224

ABSTRACT

Children tend to select novel objects over familiar ones as the likely referents of novel nouns. This finding is of central importance to several accounts of early word learning. In the current studies, 2-year-olds were shown pairs of videotaped actions, one familiar and one novel, and were asked to select the referents of novel verbs. For actions that did not involve objects, children tended to select the novel action over the familiar one in each of four experiments. For example, they chose the woman who was turning in circles while leaning backwards as "the one who is glarving" more often than the woman who was running. For actions involving objects, novel actions (e.g., shuffling balls) were chosen more often than familiar ones (e.g., kicking balls) in only two of the four experiments. An object-name-blocking mechanism was proposed to account for this last result. The preference for novel actions was also found to be strengthened by preexposing both actions from a test pair, but to be unaffected by preexposing just the novel actions.


Subject(s)
Language Development , Semantics , Verbal Learning , Attention , Child, Preschool , Concept Formation , Female , Humans , Male , Psycholinguistics , Psychomotor Performance , Visual Perception
5.
Percept Mot Skills ; 83(3 Pt 1): 999-1004, 1996 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8961337

ABSTRACT

Lack of prior research led to this study on the effect of a physical fitness program combined with a token economy reinforcement system on the physical fitness of 22 dually diagnosed adults of a day-treatment program. Between pre- and posttests, subjects participated in a 12-wk, training program of 40 to 45 min. of exercise three times a week. Subjects received tokens after each session in which they participated actively. Analysis indicated significant differences between pre- and posttest scores for muscular endurance, cardiorespiratory endurance, and flexibility but not for body composition.


Subject(s)
Intellectual Disability/rehabilitation , Mental Disorders/rehabilitation , Physical Fitness/psychology , Adult , Day Care, Medical , Diagnosis, Dual (Psychiatry) , Education of Intellectually Disabled , Female , Humans , Intellectual Disability/psychology , Male , Mental Disorders/psychology , Middle Aged , Token Economy
6.
Percept Mot Skills ; 81(3 Pt 2): 1211-6, 1995 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8684915

ABSTRACT

This study was undertaken to explore the relationship between language skills and gross-motor skills of 28 preschool children from two private preschools in New York City. Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients were calculated for language (revised Preschool Language Scale) and gross motor (Test of Gross Motor Development) scores. Locomotor skills were significantly related to both auditory comprehension and verbal ability while object control scores did not correlate significantly with either language score. These results were discussed in terms of previous research and with reference to dynamical systems theory. Suggestions for research were made.


Subject(s)
Child, Preschool , Language , Motor Skills , Humans
7.
Child Dev ; 66(6): 1890-908, 1995 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8556906

ABSTRACT

A new word-learning phenomenon is demonstrated and a new word-learning principle is proposed to account for it. In Study 1, 60 3-year-olds were shown a pair of objects and heard a novel label used repeatedly for one, but not for the other. In a forced-choice test of generalization of the label, the latter object was selected less often by the children than one that had not been present during training. This so-called Nominal Passover Effect was the same whether the speaker had completely ignored the comparison object during training or had referred to it with pronouns. The performance of a no-word control group (N = 24) indicated that the effect was not due to a preference for the less exposed of the two choice objects. The effect is consistent with the Exhaustive Reference Principle, which stipulates that whenever a new generic word is used to name something, expect it to be extended to all entities in a situation that the speaker perceives and believes to be exemplars of the name. In Study 2 (N = 48), the Nominal Passover Effect was replicated with 3 new sets of objects and with training language that contained only indefinite forms of reference. The passover experience was often sufficient to counteract children's tendency to generalize a novel label on the basis of perceptual similarity. The passover effect was not evident in free-choice name generalization tests in either study.


Subject(s)
Attention , Generalization, Stimulus , Language Development , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Verbal Learning , Child, Preschool , Color Perception , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall
8.
J Child Lang ; 22(1): 129-49, 1995 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7759575

ABSTRACT

Thirty-six three-, four- and five-year-olds were asked to select labels for deceptive stimuli (e.g. for an eraser that looked like a pencil). Three types of labelling were investigated--simple (e.g. 'is an eraser'); appearance-predicated (e.g. 'looks like an eraser'); and reality-predicated (e.g. 'is really and truly an eraser'). An age-related appearance--reality shift was observed in simple labelling (e.g. older children were more likely than younger ones to accept eraser and reject pencil as simple names for the pencil-eraser). This trend was robust over method and semantic domain, though weaker with object than with colour labels. As in previous research, older children were more likely than younger ones to map different appearance- than reality-predicated labels onto an item (e.g. to accept that the pencil-eraser looks like a pencil, but is really and truly an eraser); however, all age groups were reluctant to extend more than one name to a stimulus via a common predicate (e.g. to accept two reality-predicated labels for the same object). This one-label-perpredicate pattern was observed more frequently within reality than within appearance predicates; more frequently with colour than with object names, and with questions blocked by predicate than by name. It is argued that younger children maintained this pattern because of inflexible encoding, but that older ones did so because of better understanding of the appearance-reality distinction, greater reality dominance, and a Mutual Exclusivity bias.


Subject(s)
Concept Formation , Language Development , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reality Testing , Vocabulary , Child , Child Language , Child, Preschool , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Semantics , Verbal Learning
9.
Percept Mot Skills ; 77(3 Pt 1): 816-8, 1993 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8284159

ABSTRACT

This investigation compared the priority given to 14 sport-related values by 11 wrestlers with normal sight and 11 with visual impairments. The Survey of Values in Sport was given to 22 high school wrestlers during the competitive season. Independent t tests were used to evaluate differences in priority of values between the two groups. Significant differences were found on cooperation, group coordination, being a good sport, and being in a good place, which wrestlers with visual impairments valued higher in priority than the wrestlers with normal sight.


Subject(s)
Blindness/psychology , Social Values , Wrestling/psychology , Adolescent , Competitive Behavior , Humans , Internal-External Control , Male , Social Environment
10.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 56(3): 412-30, 1993 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8301246

ABSTRACT

Preschool-age children show a very strong tendency to map a novel noun onto an unfamiliar rather than a familiar object. This so-called disambiguation effect has been interpreted as evidence that youngers expect object labels to be mutually exclusive. In Experiment 1, 4-year-olds were observed to map a novel verb onto an unfamiliar action about twice as often as they mapped it onto a familiar one. When the unfamiliar action had been preexposed, but the familiar one had not, the mapping preference was eliminated. In Experiment 2, 4-year-olds mapped a novel noun onto an unfamiliar object about five times as often as they mapped it onto a familiar object, and this tendency was not affected by preexposure. Even when action embedding and question complexity were controlled, the disambiguation effect was stronger for object than for action words. An account is presented in which two lexical principles, Mutual Exclusivity and Feeling of Novelty, are hypothesized to apply differently to action than to object words.


Subject(s)
Child Language , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Verbal Behavior , Videotape Recording
11.
Healthc Inform ; 10(4): 58, 60, 1993 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10125091

ABSTRACT

In summary, automation of chart tracking is the place to begin full-scale medical record automation. Its collected data provides the framework for design and implementation of other systems, frees resources to use data to best advantage, and allows employees to make a comfortable transition from manual methods to computerized charts. A properly designed tracking system is the first step on the path to the electronic medical record.


Subject(s)
Medical Records Department, Hospital/organization & administration , Medical Records Systems, Computerized/organization & administration , Hospital Bed Capacity, 500 and over , New York City , Planning Techniques , Task Performance and Analysis
12.
Percept Mot Skills ; 76(1): 287-92, 1993 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8451138

ABSTRACT

206 students, with emotional disorders, ages 14 to 21 years in mainstreamed and adapted physical education classes, were surveyed concerning their participation in physical activity, attitude toward physical activity, and the influence of significant others in their participation. Significant relations were found between participation in and attitude toward physical activity, influence of social systems, and physical education placement. Subjects showed activity preferences and rates of participation in physical activity similar to those of nondisabled peers. Subjects expressed positive attitudes toward physical activity and indicated that encouragement from parents, siblings, peers, teachers, and coaches was important in their participation.


Subject(s)
Personality Disorders/psychology , Socialization , Sports/psychology , Adolescent , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Schools , Students
13.
J Child Lang ; 20(1): 101-18, 1993 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8454677

ABSTRACT

The relative importance of appearance and potential function in children's object naming was examined. Potential function is an object capability that may not be currently realized (e.g. an empty mug has the potential to hold coffee). In Study 1, sixteen children from each of three age groups (3;8, 4;8, and 6;1) were taught novel names for unfamiliar objects; they then had to decide whether these applied to items that resembled the training objects in either appearance or potential function. The youngsters were also shown deceptive objects (e.g., an eraser that looked like a pencil) and had to choose between familiar appearance and function names for them (e.g., pencil or eraser). The frequency of function-based responding in both tasks increased with age. In Study 2, the name training procedure was revised so that equal emphasis was given to both apparent and functional features. The main results of the first study were replicated. Neither study obtained evidence of a strong relation between the appearance-function shift and increased understanding of the appearance-reality distinction.


Subject(s)
Language Development , Verbal Behavior , Visual Perception , Child Language , Child, Preschool , Color Perception , Concept Formation , Female , Humans , Male , Semantics , Vocabulary
14.
Child Dev ; 62(6): 1288-301, 1991 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1786716

ABSTRACT

Children show a disambiguation effect--a tendency to select unfamiliar rather than familiar things as the referents of new names. In previous studies, this effect has been reversed in young 2-year-olds, but not older children, by preexposing the unfamiliar objects, suggesting that attraction to novelty controls 2-years-olds' choices of referents for new names, but a mutual exclusivity and/or lexical gap-filling principle determines preschoolers' selections. Both the disambiguation effect and its reversal by preexposure were replicated in the present study; however, 24-month-olds' rate of selecting unfamiliar over familiar kinds was less when they were simply asked to choose between the items than when they were asked to identify the referents of unfamiliar names. Thus, some young children may have both an attraction to novel tokens and a tendency to honor an abstract lexical principle. Referent selections were also affected by object typicality and word similarity. Correlations between the tendency to acknowledge a new name's unfamiliarity and to treat it like a similar-sounding familiar name suggested that youngsters' phonological matching skills affect their interpretation of new names. Also, 4-year-olds who most often mapped distinctive-sounding new names to unfamiliar kinds tended to admit their unfamiliarity with these names most frequently, suggesting that children's increasing awareness of their own knowledge begins to affect their lexical processing during the preschool years.


Subject(s)
Attention , Language Development , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Phonetics , Semantics , Verbal Learning , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Paired-Associate Learning , Pilot Projects , Vocabulary
15.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 120(3): 288-300, 1991 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1836492

ABSTRACT

Support for Mervis's (1987) proposal that youngsters map new common nouns onto attribute cluster categories was obtained. In 3 studies, preschoolers interpreted a novel noun as if it designated a preexposed category. Experiment 3 included a group of adults who behaved similarly. Mapping was independent of the tendency to generalize according to shape. Neither age group mapped onto preexisting categories when making object similarity judgments, but preexposure did bias their naming of the objects' colors. Those who had examined sets containing 2 colors, both of which covaried with other bivalent dimensions, used more distinctive names for these hues than those preexposed to sets containing 6 colors, none of which covaried with bivalent dimensions.


Subject(s)
Generalization, Stimulus , Language Development , Mental Recall , Thinking , Verbal Learning , Child, Preschool , Color Perception , Discrimination Learning , Female , Form Perception , Humans , Male , Retention, Psychology
16.
Monogr Soc Res Child Dev ; 54(3-4): 1-132, 1989.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2608077

ABSTRACT

Nearly every recent account of children's word learning has addressed the claim that children are biased to construct mutually exclusive extensions, that is, that they are disposed to keep the set of referents of one word from overlapping with those of others. Three basic positions have been taken--that children have the bias when they first start to learn words, that they never have it, and that they acquire it during early childhood. A review of diary and test evidence as well as the results of four experiments provide strong support for this last view and indicate that the bias develops in the months following the second birthday but does not gain full strength or become accessible to consciousness until sometime after the third birthday. Several studies also show that, after this point, it can still be counteracted by information in input or by a strong belief that something belongs to the extension of a particular word. The full body of evidence is compatible with the view that mutual exclusivity is the default option in children's and adults' procedures for integrating the extensions of new and old words. We present several arguments for the adaptive value of this kind of bias.


Subject(s)
Language Development , Association Learning , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Vocabulary
17.
Child Dev ; 58(1): 276-81, 1987 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3816349

ABSTRACT

Preschool children, young adults, and old adults viewed a series of familiar scenes and were asked to remember 1 item from each. The incidental memory of both children and old adults was less accurate than that of young adults. The result for children contrasts with the typical result of selective memorization research. Memory for high-expectancy items exceeded that for low-expectancy items by a greater margin when items were incidental, suggesting that even preschool children activate scene schemas during encoding. Only the young adults, however, showed the predicted tendency to recognize low-expectancy items better than high-expectancy items when items were intentional. These results may be reconcilable if some, but not all, schema-mediated encoding effects on memory depend on strategic encoding.


Subject(s)
Attention , Human Development , Memory , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Form Perception , Humans , Male , Middle Aged
18.
Child Dev ; 56(1): 138-51, 1985 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3987399

ABSTRACT

Long-term memory retrieval efficiency was investigated as a potential underlying source of individual and developmental differences in cognitive functioning. Fourth-grade, eighth-grade, and college-aged subjects participated in a task using the Posner letter-matching paradigm. Letter pairs were presented simultaneously under physical-match and name-match instruction conditions. Reaction times were used to estimate parameters of long-term memory retrieval efficiency, basic encoding, decision, and response time, and name and physical output interference. Psychometric tests of verbal and spatial ability were included to assess convergent and discriminant validity of hypothesized relationships between aptitude test performance and basic cognitive processes. Developmental differences were observed in most but not all of the processing variables. Individual difference analyses indicated that less confounded estimates of processing parameters were not systematically related to verbal ability at any age level. Basic encoding and response speed was the most consistent correlate of spatial ability. The results suggest difficulties in previous interpretations of NIPI-verbal ability relationships. The study of cognitive processes in interaction and embedded in meaningful tasks is discussed.


Subject(s)
Human Development , Memory , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Child , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reaction Time , Sex Factors , Space Perception , Verbal Behavior
19.
J Mot Behav ; 9(1): 1-9, 1977 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23961964

ABSTRACT

The hypotheses (a) that the memory trace and perceptual trace are independent memory states and (b) that they develop as a positive function of the amount of practice on the criterion movement were tested. The criterion movement studied was a linear displacement of a stylus in two dimensions (direction and extent) in the horizontal plane. The experiment had two phases: 92 acquisition trials with knowledge of results (KR) and 20 transfer trials without KR. Four groups of 10 male and female college students performed the acquisition trials under one of four movement conditions: (a) criterion extent and criterion direction, (b) criterion extent and varied direction, (c) varied extent and criterion direction, and (d) varied extent and varied direction. The data, showing support for the assumption and the hypothesis under test, were interpreted as evidence in favor of Adams' (1971) closed-loop theory of motor learning.

20.
J Chromatogr ; 118(1): 34-46, 1976 Mar 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1249172

ABSTRACT

Analytical and preparative isotachophoresis has been carried out using amino acids and peptides as discrete spacers in contrast to the usually employed continuous mobility spectrum Ampholine. Analytical isotachoresis in free solution, using the LKB Tachophor, demonstrated the separation of human serum into distinct mobility subgroups, n spacers giving rise to n + 1 protein subgroups. Preparative fractionation on polyacrylamide gel was carried out on the LKB Uniphor using threonine and glycine as spacers. Immunoelectrophoretic analysis showed that eight out of ten proteins assayed were clearly resolved in the three subgroups obtained, thus demonstrating the sharpness of isotachophoretic resolution.


Subject(s)
Amino Acids , Blood Protein Electrophoresis/methods , Blood Proteins/classification , Electrophoresis, Polyacrylamide Gel , Humans , Immunoelectrophoresis , Peptides
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...