Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J R Soc Interface ; 15(145)2018 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30089685

ABSTRACT

Even though previously described iron-containing structures in the upper beak of pigeons were almost certainly macrophages, not magnetosensitive neurons, behavioural and neurobiological evidence still supports the involvement of the ophthalmic branch of the trigeminal nerve (V1) in magnetoreception. In previous behavioural studies, inactivation of putative V1-associated magnetoreceptors involved either application of the surface anaesthetic lidocaine to the upper beak or sectioning of V1. Here, we compared the effects of lidocaine treatment, V1 ablations and sham ablations on magnetic field-driven neuronal activation in V1-recipient brain regions in European robins. V1 sectioning led to significantly fewer Egr-1-expressing neurons in the trigeminal brainstem than in the sham-ablated birds, whereas lidocaine treatment had no effect on neuronal activation. Furthermore, Prussian blue staining showed that nearly all iron-containing cells in the subepidermal layer of the upper beak are nucleated and are thus not part of the trigeminal nerve, and iron-containing cells appeared in highly variable numbers at inconsistent locations between individual robins and showed no systematic colocalization with a neuronal marker. Our data suggest that lidocaine treatment has been a nocebo to the birds and a placebo for the experimenters. Currently, the nature and location of any V1-associated magnetosensor remains elusive.


Subject(s)
Brain Stem , Lidocaine/pharmacology , Magnetic Fields , Orientation/drug effects , Songbirds/physiology , Trigeminal Nerve/physiology , Animals , Beak/anatomy & histology , Beak/physiology , Brain Stem/cytology , Brain Stem/physiology , Nocebo Effect , Songbirds/anatomy & histology , Trigeminal Nerve/cytology
2.
Sci Rep ; 5: 14323, 2015 Sep 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26388258

ABSTRACT

Migratory birds can use a variety of environmental cues for orientation. A primary calibration between the celestial and magnetic compasses seems to be fundamental prior to a bird's first autumn migration. Releasing hand-raised or rescued young birds back into the wild might therefore be a problem because they might not have established a functional orientation system during their first calendar year. Here, we test whether hand-raised European robins that did not develop any functional compass before or during their first autumn migration could relearn to orient if they were exposed to natural celestial cues during the subsequent winter and spring. When tested in the geomagnetic field without access to celestial cues, these birds could orient in their species-specific spring migratory direction. In contrast, control birds that were deprived of any natural celestial cues throughout remained unable to orient. Our experiments suggest that European robins are still capable of establishing a functional orientation system after their first autumn. Although the external reference remains speculative, most likely, natural celestial cues enabled our birds to calibrate their magnetic compass. Our data suggest that avian compass systems are more flexible than previously believed and have implications for the release of hand-reared migratory birds.


Subject(s)
Magnetics , Orientation , Sensation , Songbirds/physiology , Animal Migration , Animals , Calibration , Seasons
3.
PLoS One ; 10(3): e0119919, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25807499

ABSTRACT

Birds can rely on a variety of cues for orientation during migration and homing. Celestial rotation provides the key information for the development of a functioning star and/or sun compass. This celestial compass seems to be the primary reference for calibrating the other orientation systems including the magnetic compass. Thus, detection of the celestial rotational axis is crucial for bird orientation. Here, we use operant conditioning to demonstrate that homing pigeons can principally learn to detect a rotational centre in a rotating dot pattern and we examine their behavioural response strategies in a series of experiments. Initially, most pigeons applied a strategy based on local stimulus information such as movement characteristics of single dots. One pigeon seemed to immediately ignore eccentric stationary dots. After special training, all pigeons could shift their attention to more global cues, which implies that pigeons can learn the concept of a rotational axis. In our experiments, the ability to precisely locate the rotational centre was strongly dependent on the rotational velocity of the dot pattern and it crashed at velocities that were still much faster than natural celestial rotation. We therefore suggest that the axis of the very slow, natural, celestial rotation could be perceived by birds through the movement itself, but that a time-delayed pattern comparison should also be considered as a very likely alternative strategy.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , Columbidae/physiology , Conditioning, Operant/physiology , Learning/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Animals , Cues , Rotation , Visual Perception/physiology
4.
Nature ; 509(7500): 353-6, 2014 May 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24805233

ABSTRACT

Electromagnetic noise is emitted everywhere humans use electronic devices. For decades, it has been hotly debated whether man-made electric and magnetic fields affect biological processes, including human health. So far, no putative effect of anthropogenic electromagnetic noise at intensities below the guidelines adopted by the World Health Organization has withstood the test of independent replication under truly blinded experimental conditions. No effect has therefore been widely accepted as scientifically proven. Here we show that migratory birds are unable to use their magnetic compass in the presence of urban electromagnetic noise. When European robins, Erithacus rubecula, were exposed to the background electromagnetic noise present in unscreened wooden huts at the University of Oldenburg campus, they could not orient using their magnetic compass. Their magnetic orientation capabilities reappeared in electrically grounded, aluminium-screened huts, which attenuated electromagnetic noise in the frequency range from 50 kHz to 5 MHz by approximately two orders of magnitude. When the grounding was removed or when broadband electromagnetic noise was deliberately generated inside the screened and grounded huts, the birds again lost their magnetic orientation capabilities. The disruptive effect of radiofrequency electromagnetic fields is not confined to a narrow frequency band and birds tested far from sources of electromagnetic noise required no screening to orient with their magnetic compass. These fully double-blinded tests document a reproducible effect of anthropogenic electromagnetic noise on the behaviour of an intact vertebrate.


Subject(s)
Animal Migration/physiology , Electromagnetic Fields/adverse effects , Magnetic Fields , Orientation/physiology , Songbirds/physiology , Aluminum , Animals , Cities , Conservation of Natural Resources , Double-Blind Method , Electricity/adverse effects , Electronics/instrumentation , Germany , Housing , Radio Waves/adverse effects , Reproducibility of Results , Seasons , Universities
5.
Rapid Commun Mass Spectrom ; 24(7): 875-8, 2010 Apr 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20196191

ABSTRACT

Bird feathers are employed in a wide range of carbon and nitrogen isotope studies relating to diet and migration. Feathers are chemically inert with respect to carbon and nitrogen, after synthesis. It has always been assumed that feathers show isotope values characteristic of keratin, a fibrous structural protein from which they are formed. Little attention has been paid to other components of feathers such as melanin or carotenoids. Melanin is synthesized from tyrosine, which is depleted in both (13)C and (15)N. We compared isotope values of coeval black and white feathers in four different species. Black feather parts were in all cases significantly depleted in (13)C relative to white feather parts but in most species no clear trend was discernable for (15)N. We suggest that additional evaluation may be required to characterize the carbon and nitrogen isotope contribution of feather pigments like carotenoids. Care should be taken in future stable isotope studies when comparing differently coloured feathers.


Subject(s)
Carbon Isotopes/chemistry , Feathers/chemistry , Mass Spectrometry/methods , Melanins/chemistry , Nitrogen Isotopes/chemistry , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Birds , Carbon Isotopes/metabolism , Feathers/metabolism , Linear Models , Melanins/metabolism , Nitrogen Isotopes/metabolism , Pigmentation , Tyrosine/chemistry , Tyrosine/metabolism
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...