Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 88
Filter
1.
Hear Res ; 161(1-2): 81-6, 2001 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11744284

ABSTRACT

Wave V of the auditory brainstem response was measured to two 50-ms broadband noise bursts separated by silent gaps of varied duration (4, 8, 32, or 64 ms) for younger and older adults with normal hearing. All subjects had measurable wave V responses to the first noise burst. However, for the second noise burst, three of eight older adults did not have responses with gap durations of 4 and 8 ms, and one of eight younger adults did not have a measurable response with a gap duration of 4 ms. When responses were present for older adults, latencies were similar to those of younger subjects but amplitudes were smaller. These results suggest age-related deficits in gap detection at the level of the brainstem in a group of aged subjects with no threshold elevation. Results are similar to those of Boettcher et al. (1996) using an identical paradigm in young and aged Mongolian gerbils.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory, Brain Stem , Noise , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Aged , Humans , Middle Aged , Reaction Time , Reference Values
2.
Hear Res ; 153(1-2): 32-42, 2001 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11223295

ABSTRACT

The amplitude-modulation following response (AMFR) is a steady-state auditory response which may be an objective measure of intensity discrimination. Aged subjects with normal hearing have poorer intensity discrimination for low-frequency tones measured behaviorally, which would predict poorer AMFRs for low-frequency carriers. Experiment 1 was designed to assess age-related differences in AMFR characteristics. Response amplitudes were not significantly different among the young and aged groups for either carrier frequency (520 or 4000 Hz) or modulation depth (0--100%). Response phase did not vary systematically among groups. These results suggest that the AMFR may not be directly comparable to behavioral measures of intensity discrimination in aged subjects with normal hearing. To assess the contribution of high-frequency hearing loss on the AMFR in aged subjects, Experiment 2 compared AMFR amplitudes in aged subjects and in young subjects under the condition of high-pass masking. The amplitude of the AMFR was reduced at 520 Hz for both aged subjects and masked young subjects compared to unmasked young subjects, suggesting that reduced amplitudes in aged subjects with high-frequency hearing loss were associated with threshold elevations. Furthermore, the results suggest that the base of the cochlea contributes to the AMFR for low carrier frequencies.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Hearing/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Cochlea/physiology , Hearing Loss, High-Frequency/physiopathology , Humans , Middle Aged , Pitch Discrimination/physiology , Presbycusis/physiopathology
3.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 106(4 Pt 1): 1969-76, 1999 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10530021

ABSTRACT

Psychometric functions are used to relate the responses of a subject to physical stimuli in a variety of psychophysical tasks. However, it is time consuming to obtain data to determine a psychometric function if many stimulus levels and many trials are required. A computer simulation was conducted to determine the minimum number of data points needed for such a determination. The computer simulation also determined the optimal placements of the stimuli and the number of trials per datum point for psychometric function determinations. Results indicate that a 2-point sampling method with 30-50 trials per point at optimal locations can produce a psychometric function with accurate spread and threshold estimates in a yes-no paradigm. However, the 4-point sampling method yields statistically smaller variances of the estimates. For the 2-alternative forced-choice paradigm, at least 120 trials per point are needed for the 2-point sampling method's estimated parameters to differ from the known parameter values by less than 5%. The simulation results suggest that 3-alternative or 4-alternative forced-choice is preferable to 2-alternative. Furthermore, when a criterion-free paradigm is not required, the yes-no paradigm is a better procedure than m-alternative forced-choice for obtaining the corresponding psychometric function because of smaller standard deviation of the estimates and smaller number of trials/point required.


Subject(s)
Computer Simulation , Models, Biological , Physical Stimulation/methods , Psychometrics/statistics & numerical data , Humans , Psychophysics
4.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 106(2): 966-78, 1999 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10462802

ABSTRACT

Psychometric functions for gap detection of temporal gaps in wideband noise were measured in a "yes/no" paradigm from normal-hearing young and aged subjects with closely matched audiograms. The effects of noise-burst duration, gap location, and uncertainty of gap location were tested. A typical psychometric function obtained in this study featured a steep slope, which was independent of most experimental conditions as well as age. However, gap thresholds were generally improved with increasing duration of the noise burst for both young and aged subjects. Gap location and uncertainty had no significant effects on the thresholds for the young subjects. For the aged subjects, whenever the gap was sufficiently away from the onset or offset of the noise burst, detectability was robust despite uncertainty about the gap location. Significant differences between young and aged subjects could be observed only when the gap was very close to the signal onset and offset.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Noise , Time Perception/physiology , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aging/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychometrics , Time Factors
5.
Ann N Y Acad Sci ; 884: 381-8, 1999 Nov 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10842608

ABSTRACT

As part of a study of human presbyacusis, a questionnaire on medicinal drug usage was given to 357 subjects (184 females, 173 males). Previous results from 211 subjects showed gender effects, that is, for males, none of the drugs had any measurable effects on hearing, whereas women taking calcium channel blockers (CCBs) had hearing levels 12 dB better than women not taking them; women taking beta adrenergic medication had hearing levels 20 dB poorer, and women taking antihistamine/cold preparations had hearing levels 9 dB poorer. Results from the original 211 subjects were confirmed when the sample size was increased from 211 to 357 subjects only for the beta adrenergic medications. Results for antihistamine/cold preparation medications showed small effects only for female subjects. Data from 13 additional female subjects who used CCBs showed hearing levels 10-14 dB poorer than predicted from the original data. Male data were consistent in both samples. The inconsistency for females could reflect sampling error. A more likely possibility is that since the original 10 subjects using CCBs had a mean age of 72 yr and the second sample of 13 had a mean age of 79.5 yr, poorer hearing levels might be anticipated because of the difference in chronological age and possibly duration of drug usage.


Subject(s)
Adrenergic beta-Agonists/pharmacology , Adrenergic beta-Antagonists/pharmacology , Calcium Channel Blockers/pharmacology , Hearing/drug effects , Histamine H1 Antagonists/pharmacology , Presbycusis/chemically induced , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Health Surveys , Hearing/physiology , Humans , Male , Presbycusis/physiopathology , Sex Factors
7.
Ear Hear ; 19(3): 180-90, 1998 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9657593

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: As part of an ongoing study of presbyacusis, the relationship between blood chemistry levels and hearing levels was investigated. Previous reports often used small sets of blood chemistry measures, and results were inconclusive. This experiment examined hearing levels and 27 measures of blood chemistry using various univariate and multivariate statistical procedures. DESIGN: Blood from 89 female and 128 male human subjects was collected. Subjects' ages ranged from 60 to 82 yr, and hearing levels ranged from normal to moderate/severe. Subjects with a history of middle ear disease were excluded. Electrolyte panel (Na, K, Cl, CO2, Ca, urea nitrogen, glucose, creatinine, and Mg), hematology panel (WBC, RBC, Hgb, hematocrit, platelet, etc.), serum lipids (total cholesterol, low-density lipoprotein [LDL], and high-density lipoprotein [HDL]), immunoglobulins (IgG, IgA, IgM, and IgE), and thyroxine were analyzed using univariate and multivariate statistical procedures. RESULTS: Blood chemistry levels of most subjects were within normal ranges as defined by our laboratory. Correlation between blood chemistry measures and pure-tone averages (PTAs) ranged from minimal to low. Results of factor analysis, discriminant analysis, and canonical analysis showed that combining blood chemistry measures from the same panel still could not predict PTA effectively. One exception to this was a gender-specific effect of cholesterol. Hearing levels of women with high LDL/HDL ratios were 5 dB better than those of women with low LDL/HDL ratios. The comparable difference in men was only 1 dB. CONCLUSION: Results suggest that blood chemistry measures that are primarily within the normal range have very little value in predicting pure-tone thresholds in older subjects.


Subject(s)
Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/blood , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/diagnosis , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Audiometry, Pure-Tone/methods , Auditory Threshold , Blood Chemical Analysis , Blood Viscosity , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Factors
8.
Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg ; 118(2): 221-7, 1998 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9482556

ABSTRACT

As part of a large-scale study of presbyacusis, responses to a medicinal drug questionnaire from 85 female and 126 male human subjects were analyzed. Medicinal drugs were divided into 35 categories based on their pharmacologic effects. Subjects' ages ranged from 60 to 82 years. At least 10% of subjects reported taking drugs in 14 of 35 categories. Results were significantly different between female and male subjects. In men, none of the 14 categories showed a statistically significant relation to the pure-tone average (PTA) of 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz. In women, 3 of the 14 categories showed a statistically significant relation to the PTA. First, the average PTA of female subjects taking beta-adrenergic medication was 20 dB higher (poorer) than those not taking beta-adrenergic medication. Second, women taking antihistamine/cold preparations had an average PTA 9 dB higher (poorer) than those not taking antihistamine/cold preparations. Third, the average PTA of women taking calcium-channel blockers (CCBs) was 12 dB lower (better) than those not taking CCBs. In men, however, these drugs produced effects on the PTA of less than 3 dB. Differences between women and men were not explainable by differences in age or hearing level.


Subject(s)
Adrenergic beta-Agonists/pharmacology , Calcium Channel Blockers/pharmacology , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/diagnosis , Histamine H1 Antagonists/pharmacology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Audiometry, Pure-Tone , Auditory Threshold/drug effects , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Distribution
9.
Scand Audiol Suppl ; 48: 117-22, 1998.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9505304

ABSTRACT

A medical-legal and scientific topic of longstanding interest is the interaction between presbyacusis and noise-induced permanent threshold shift (NIPTS). Current medical-legal practices as well as international standard, ISO 1999 International Organization for Standards: Acoustics: Determination of Occupational Noise Exposure and Estimation of Noise-Induced Hearing Impairment. ISO 1999. Geneva, International for Standards, 1990 assume that NIPTS and hearing loss caused by the aging process add in dB. Results of laboratory studies with animals are inconsistent in their support of the "additivity assumption". When intense, short-duration exposures are used, the predictions of the combined effects of noise and age are too large. The additivity model appears to be supported with long-duration exposures, but we question the accuracy of such predictions. The animal studies reviewed here suggest that the allocation of hearing loss in an older individual into a noise component and an aging component is much more complex than "additivity in dB".


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Hearing Loss, Noise-Induced/diagnosis , Presbycusis/diagnosis , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Animals , Auditory Threshold , Evoked Potentials, Auditory, Brain Stem , Gerbillinae , Humans , Middle Aged , Noise/adverse effects
10.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 103(1): 553-65, 1998 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9440340

ABSTRACT

A maximum-likelihood method was applied in measurements of frequency and intensity discrimination for aged and young normal-hearing subjects with closely matched audiograms. This method was preferred over other psychophysical procedures because it is efficient and controls experimental variance, features that are highly desirable for testing aged subjects. In order to implement the method, psychometric functions for each task were also measured from young subjects using a constant-stimuli procedure. For the young subjects, the differential thresholds obtained from these two procedures were generally comparable. Further, both sets of data were consistent with previous literature, indicating that the maximum-likelihood method was successfully applied for frequency and intensity discrimination. A frequency-dependent difference between young and aged subjects in both frequency and intensity discrimination was observed. Even with closely matched audiograms, aged subjects demonstrated poorer discrimination abilities than young subjects. The age-related difference was always largest at 500 Hz and decreased as frequency increased.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Hearing/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Humans , Middle Aged
11.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 40(2): 444-52, 1997 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9130212

ABSTRACT

Previous studies of older listeners suggest age-related declines in speech recognition. However, the interpretation of these results is not straightforward because auditory thresholds, which account for the largest proportion of the variance in speech-recognition scores, also vary considerably with age. Here, effects of age, gender, and auditory thresholds on several measures of speech recognition were assessed for a large sample of individuals enrolled in a longitudinal study of age-related hearing loss. Participants ranged in age from 55-84 years. They were evaluated with a battery of conventional audiometric measures and speech-recognition materials, including NU-6 monosyllabic words, Synthetic Sentence Identification sentences, and high-context and low-context sentences from the Speech Perception in Noise test. Two analyses were conducted to assure that changes in speech-recognition scores with age were examined independently of age-related changes in auditory thresholds. In the first analysis, no significant differences in speech recognition were observed for individuals in three age groups (55-64, 65-74, 75-84 years) who were selected so that average puretone thresholds for the groups were within 5 dB. In the second analysis, using partial correlations to adjust both score and age for their association with average thresholds, significant declines with age were observed for males in maximum word recognition, maximum synthetic sentence identification, and keyword recognition in high-context sentences. For females, no significant changes in speech recognition with age were observed for any test.


Subject(s)
Speech Perception , Age Factors , Aged , Aging , Audiometry, Pure-Tone , Auditory Threshold , Cochlea/physiopathology , Female , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/physiopathology , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Factors
12.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 101(3): 1681-6, 1997 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9069635

ABSTRACT

Current medical-legal practices as well as an international standard (ISO 1999) assume the permanent threshold shifts produced by exposure to noise add (in dB) to the threshold shifts caused by increased chronological age (presbyacusis). This assumption, known as the additivity rule, was tested in an animal model. Mongolian gerbils, born and raised in a quiet vivarium, were exposed at age 18 months to a 3.5-kHz pure tone for 1 h at 113 dB SPL. At 6-weeks post-exposure, permanent threshold shifts in the exposed ear were approximately 20 dB in the 4- to 8-kHz region. Thresholds in the nonexposed, control ear were unaffected by the exposure. Animals were then allowed to age in the quiet vivarium until age 36 months and then were retested. Thus in a given animal, aging-only effects were assessed in one ear (internal control) and noise-plus-aging effects were assessed in the other (test) ear. A second control was mean age-related threshold shift measured in 48 gerbils who were born and raised in the quiet vivarium. This group is referred to as a non-noise-exposed population (population control). Using the additivity rule, predictions with either the internal or population control significantly overestimated noise-plus-aging effects. Use of the ISO 1999 compression factor reduced the overestimations by 0-5 dB. The intensity rule produced the most accurate predictions. These results suggest that the interaction of noise-induced permanent threshold shift and age-related threshold shift is not straightforward and that current medical-legal methods using the additivity rule overestimate the contribution of "noise effects".


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Noise , Age Factors , Animals , Auditory Threshold , Evoked Potentials, Auditory, Brain Stem , Gerbillinae
13.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 40(1): 208-14, 1997 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9113871

ABSTRACT

Most measures of auditory sensitivity at extended high frequencies (frequencies greater than 8 kHz) have been obtained from listeners with normal hearing less than 40 years of age. The purpose of this study was (a) to measure thresholds at frequencies above 8 kHz in older listeners who, as a group, have elevated thresholds at lower frequencies, and (b) to assess test-retest reliability, age and gender effects, and the influence of thresholds below 8 kHz. Extended high-frequency (EHF) thresholds were measured for 162 older listeners (60-79 years) using a commercially available high-frequency audiometer, with a frequency range of 8 to 18 kHz and an intensity range of 0 to 110 dB SPL. Thresholds were measured once at the beginning of a 1- to 2-hour test session and then remeasured at the end of the test session. EHF thresholds of older listeners with normal hearing at conventional audiometric frequencies were substantially higher than the thresholds reported for younger listeners, with normal hearing by Dreschler and van der Hulst (1987). EHF thresholds of older listeners with hearing loss at conventional audiometric frequencies were further elevated as compared to older listeners with normal hearing. Differences in EHF thresholds between females and males were either not present or were reduced when gender differences in conventional audiometric thresholds were taken into account. No significant differences were seen in thresholds at 8 kHz and higher between the 60- to 69-and 70- to 79-year-old age groups. Results also indicated that thresholds above 8 kHz can be measured in older listeners within a clinically acceptable +/- 10 dB test-retest range.


Subject(s)
Hearing Disorders/diagnosis , Age Factors , Aged , Aging , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Factors
14.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 102(6): 3697-703, 1997 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9407661

ABSTRACT

Although a psychometric function describing a subject's responses to some physical stimuli is of considerable value, characterizing such functions is time consuming and, hence, is not carried out routinely in psychophysical experiments. A principal reason for the lack of efficiency in characterizing a psychometric function is the use of sampling methods that either converge on a single point on the psychometric function, such as the PEST method, or which distribute observations uniformly over a wide range, such as the constant stimuli method. As an alternative, a multimodal four-point sampling method has been proposed [C. F. Lam, J. H. Mills, and J. R. Dubno, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 99, 3689-3693 (1996)]. A psychometric function is then fitted to the four points (each with several trials) to estimate the threshold and slope parameters of the psychometric function. Adaptive methods, such as the up-down methods [H. Levitt, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 49, 467-477 (1971)], can be used to provide good initial estimates of the threshold and spread parameters of a psychometric function described by a logistic function. In ongoing studies of age-related changes in auditory masking and discrimination, this new four-point sampling method has been applied to determine psychometric functions for absolute thresholds as a function of duration, thresholds in simultaneous and forward masking, frequency discrimination, and intensity discrimination in both young and aged human subjects. Results indicate that a reduction in data collection time of about 50% with no increase in variance can be achieved. This increase in efficiency applies to simple detection tasks by normal hearing subjects as well as to complex discrimination tasks by older subjects with hearing loss.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Adult , Aged , Auditory Threshold , Humans , Middle Aged , Models, Theoretical , Psychometrics , Psychophysics , Time Factors
15.
Hear Res ; 102(1-2): 167-78, 1996 Dec 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8951460

ABSTRACT

The compound action potential (CAP) and the auditory brainstem response (ABR; waves ii and iv) were recorded in young (4-8 month) and aged (33-37 month) gerbils using a paradigm similar to that used in some psychophysical studies of gap detection (a pair of identical low-pass noises separated by a silent gap). Response amplitudes were analyzed in terms of absolute amplitudes and the 'amplitude ratio' (the amplitude of the response to the second noise of a pair divided by that to the first). Response latencies were analyzed in terms of the absolute latencies as well as the 'latency shift' (the latency of the response to the second noise minus that to the first). Response amplitudes were much smaller in the aged subjects for both the first and second stimuli of a pair. There were minimal changes in amplitude ratios across age for both the CAP and ABR. Absolute latencies were similar between groups for the first stimulus of a pair, but latencies to wave iv were much longer for the aged subjects when the gap was short. Thus, the latency shift for the aged group was much longer for wave iv in the aged compared to the young group, but were similar between groups for the CAP or wave ii of the ABR. The results suggest that there may be changes in coding of temporal information in the auditory brainstem of aged gerbils which are not a direct result of abnormal temporal processing in the auditory periphery.


Subject(s)
Action Potentials/physiology , Aging/pathology , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory, Brain Stem/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Disease Models, Animal , Gerbillinae , Presbycusis/etiology , Statistics as Topic
16.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 100(5): 3336-44, 1996 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8914315

ABSTRACT

Contralateral masking occurs when the threshold of a signal in one ear is elevated by the presence of a masker in the other, contralateral ear. The classic data and theory on contralateral masking were provided by Zwislocki [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 52, 644-659 (1972)] who observed a 3- to 18-dB threshold shift (masking) for a gated pure-tone signal in one ear when a gated pure-tone masker was presented via insert earphones to the other ear. Zwislocki referred to this phenomenon as "central masking." Here, using two psychophysical methods (Yes-No; two-interval forced-choice), Zwislocki's original results, obtained with other psychophysical methods, were successfully replicated. Similar results using several psychophysical methods suggest that contralateral masking is indicative of a sensory phenomenon rather than observer bias and other response proclivities. In a second experiment, psychophysical tuning curves were obtained using either an ipsilateral masker or a contralateral masker. Tuning curves obtained with a contralateral masker had steeper slopes on both the low- and high-frequency sides than tuning curves obtained with an ipsilateral masker. Thus, although substantially smaller in effect than ipsilateral masking, contralateral masking is more sharply tuned. The sharp tuning of contralateral masking reflects a greater compression of the input/output functions for contralateral masking than for ipsilateral masking. The closest correspondence between the tuning curves reported here for contralateral masking and those predicted by Zwislocki's theory and data (on central masking) occurred for tuning curves where the ratio of driven activity to spontaneous activity was about six. A remaining issue is the role, if any, of the efferent auditory system, especially the olivocochlear bundle, in threshold shifts measured using the Zwislocki (central masking) paradigm.


Subject(s)
Functional Laterality , Perceptual Masking , Adult , Hearing/physiology , Humans , Psychophysics
17.
J Neurophysiol ; 76(4): 2799-803, 1996 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8899648

ABSTRACT

1. Characteristic frequencies (CF), spontaneous rates (SR), and thresholds were recorded from single fibers in the auditory nerves of gerbils aged for 36 mo in a quiet vivarium. The data from the quiet-aged animals were compared with similar data obtained previously from young controls. Fibers were classified as "low-SR" if their spontaneous rates were < or = 18 spikes/s and "high SR" for higher rates. 2. For CFs > 6 kHz, the percentage of low-SR fibers contacted declined from 57% of the population in young gerbils to 29% in the aged gerbils. This population change is statistically significant (P < 0.01). At CFs < 6 kHz, the population demographics did not change significantly with age, with the low-SR fibers comprising 30 and 39% of the population, respectively, for the young and aged animals. 3. To further test the hypothesis that low-SR fibers with CFs > 6 kHz become less active with age, additional experiments were conducted to examine the recovery of the compound action potential (CAP) response from prior high-level stimuli. Previous work has shown that the CAP recovery curve has two segments: a fast segment associated with the high-SR fibers and a slow segment associated with the low-SR fibers. The curves obtained from quiet aged gerbils show a faster recovery than young controls for probe tones at 8 and 16 kHz, but not at 2 and 4 kHz. Thus these results agree with our single-fiber data indicating that there is a loss of low-SR activity for CFs > 6 kHz in the aged animals. 4. Low-SR fibers typically have larger dynamic ranges than those of high-SR fibers, are better able to preserve information concerning stimulus timing and amplitude modulation, and their responses are more robust in the presence of masking noise. Moreover, low-SR fibers are likely inputs to the crossed-olivocochlear reflex, a reflex that serves an antimasking role in the detection of sounds in a binaural noise field. If true for humans, the loss of the low-SR system could explain many of the hearing deficits often seen in older individuals; e.g., decreased ability to understand speech in noise, changes in masking level differences, and decreased ability to localize sound sources using binaural cues.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Nerve Degeneration/physiology , Nerve Fibers/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Vestibulocochlear Nerve/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Aging/pathology , Animals , Gerbillinae , Linear Models , Nerve Fibers/ultrastructure , Vestibulocochlear Nerve/ultrastructure
18.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 99(6): 3689-93, 1996 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8655800

ABSTRACT

Psychometric functions for different psychophysical tasks describe the relationship between physical stimuli and subjects' responses. Although a psychometric function is of considerable value, characterizing the function is time consuming and, hence, is not carried out routinely in psychophysical experiments. A principal reason for the lack of efficiency in characterizing the psychometric function is the use of unimodal (e.g., Gaussian and uniform) sampling methods. As an alternative, a multimodal four-point sampling method is proposed. A psychometric function is then fitted to the four data points (each with several trials) to estimate the threshold and slope parameters of the psychometric function. Results from three examples demonstrate that a 60% savings in data-collection time can be achieved.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception , Psychometrics , Humans , Models, Theoretical , Psychophysics
19.
Hear Res ; 89(1-2): 1-13, 1995 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8600113

ABSTRACT

Auditory brainstem responses (ABR) were recorded in the presence of low-pass (1 kHz cutoff) or high-pass (8 kHz cutoff) filtered noise in young (4-8 month) and aged (36 month) gerbils. For low-pass maskers, aged gerbils had higher masked thresholds at 2 and 4 kHz than young subjects. This was true for all aged subjects, including those with quiet thresholds similar to those of young controls. For high-pass masking, the majority of aged subjects had higher masked thresholds at 2 and 4 kHz than young controls; however, aged subjects with relatively normal quiet thresholds had masked thresholds similar to those of young subjects. A modified power-law (MPL) model was used to predict masked thresholds for aged subjects. Thresholds measured in the presence of low-pass noise were higher than predicted in many of the aged subjects, particularly those with near-normal quiet thresholds. In contrast, thresholds measured in the presence of the high-pass masker were similar to the predicted thresholds. These results suggest that: (a) excess masking occurred in aged subjects for low-pass, but not high-pass, maskers; (b) the excess masking occurred independently of quiet thresholds; and (c) excess upward spread of masking was related to the spectrum of the masker and not the 2 and 4 kHz regions of the auditory periphery.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory, Brain Stem/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Aging/psychology , Animals , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Gerbillinae , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/physiopathology , Humans , Models, Biological , Noise , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Psychoacoustics
20.
Hear Res ; 87(1-2): 208-19, 1995 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8567437

ABSTRACT

The auditory brainstem response (ABR) was recorded non-invasively from Mongolian gerbils ranging in age from 6 to 36 months. The ABR was elicited using gaussian tone bursts at octave intervals from 1 to 16 kHz. Responses were bandpass filtered from 30 to 300 Hz (LF-ABR; low-frequency component) and from 300 to 3000 Hz (HF-ABR; high-frequency component). In Experiment A, the thresholds of the two components (HF- and LF-ABR) were compared in 6- and 36-month subjects. The LF-ABR varied more with age than did the HF-ABR, particularly at stimulus frequencies of 2 kHz and above. As shown previously for the HF-ABR, the latencies of the LF-ABR increased as a function of hearing loss in aged gerbils whereas amplitudes of the LF-ABR were reduced in all aged gerbils, regardless of age-related threshold elevation. In Experiment B, tone bursts were presented at rates of 11-91/s to gerbils aged 6, 18, 30, and 36 months. Increased repetition rate resulted in an increase in the latency of both the HF- and LF-ABR, but to the same degree in each age group. Similarly, the interpeak intervals of the HF-ABR increased as a function of repetition rate in all subjects to the same degree. Increased age and increased repetition rate both resulted in significant reductions in ABR amplitudes, but rate did not interact with age. The data suggest that (1) the LF-ABR may be more sensitive to aging than is the HF-ABR and (2) there are no age-related changes in the HF- or LF-ABR which are dependent upon the repetition rate.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Auditory Threshold/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory, Brain Stem/physiology , Gerbillinae/physiology , Presbycusis/etiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Auditory Cortex/pathology , Disease Models, Animal , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/etiology , Hearing Loss, Sensorineural/physiopathology , Normal Distribution , Presbycusis/physiopathology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...