Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 15 de 15
Filter
1.
Front Neurosci ; 11: 322, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28670264

ABSTRACT

The processing of EEG data routinely involves subjective removal of artifacts during a preprocessing stage. Preprocessing inter-rater reliability (IRR) and how differences in preprocessing may affect outcomes of primary event-related potential (ERP) analyses has not been previously assessed. Three raters independently preprocessed EEG data of 16 cognitively healthy adult participants (ages 18-39 years) who performed a memory task. Using intraclass correlations (ICCs), IRR was assessed for Early-frontal, Late-frontal, and Parietal Old/new memory effects contrasts across eight regions of interest (ROIs). IRR was good to excellent for all ROIs; 22 of 26 ICCs were above 0.80. Raters were highly consistent in preprocessing across ROIs, although the frontal pole ROI (ICC range 0.60-0.90) showed less consistency. Old/new parietal effects had highest ICCs with the lowest variability. Rater preprocessing differences did not alter primary ERP results. IRR for EEG preprocessing was good to excellent, and subjective rater-removal of EEG artifacts did not alter primary memory-task ERP results. Findings provide preliminary support for robustness of cognitive/memory task-related ERP results against significant inter-rater preprocessing variability and suggest reliability of EEG to assess cognitive-neurophysiological processes multiple preprocessors are involved.

2.
Brain Imaging Behav ; 10(2): 408-23, 2016 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26116280

ABSTRACT

We sought to characterize electrophysiological, eye-tracking and behavioral correlates of face-name recognition memory in healthy younger adults using high-density electroencephalography (EEG), infrared eye-tracking (ET), and neuropsychological measures. Twenty-one participants first studied 40 face-name (FN) pairs; 20 were presented four times (4R) and 20 were shown once (1R). Recognition memory was assessed by asking participants to make old/new judgments for 80 FN pairs, of which half were previously studied items and half were novel FN pairs (N). Simultaneous EEG and ET recording were collected during recognition trials. Comparisons of event-related potentials (ERPs) for correctly identified FN pairs were compared across the three item types revealing classic ERP old/new effects including 1) relative positivity (1R > N) bi-frontally from 300 to 500 ms, reflecting enhanced familiarity, 2) relative positivity (4R > 1R and 4R > N) in parietal areas from 500 to 800 ms, reflecting enhanced recollection, and 3) late frontal effects (1R > N) from 1000 to 1800 ms in right frontal areas, reflecting post-retrieval monitoring. ET analysis also revealed significant differences in eye movements across conditions. Exploration of cross-modality relationships suggested associations between memory and executive function measures and the three ERP effects. Executive function measures were associated with several indicators of saccadic eye movements and fixations, which were also associated with all three ERP effects. This novel characterization of face-name recognition memory performance using simultaneous EEG and ET reproduced classic ERP and ET effects, supports the construct validity of the multimodal FN paradigm, and holds promise as an integrative tool to probe brain networks supporting memory and executive functioning.


Subject(s)
Executive Function/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Brain/physiology , Brain Mapping , Electroencephalography/methods , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Face , Female , Healthy Volunteers , Humans , Male , Memory/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology
3.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23379531

ABSTRACT

We previously presented normative data from a relatively large, population-based sample (n = 244) of centenarians and a reference group of octogenarians (n = 80) for several brief, global neurocognitive tasks adapted for use for older adults with physical and sensory limitations ( Miller et al., 2010 , Neuropsychological, Development, and Cognition. Section B: Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 17, 575). Here, we present additional normative data on several domain-specific tasks from these samples from Phase III of the Georgia Centenarian Study, including measures of verbal abstract reasoning, fluency, memory, and motor function. Expected age differences were demonstrated across all cognitive measures, and, consistent with our previous findings, centenarians showed a stronger association between age and performance. Normative tables are presented unweighted as well as population-weighted, and stratified by age and education level. These findings offer a unique contribution to the literature on cognitive aging, as normative performance in this age group is understudied and largely unavailable to clinicians and researchers.


Subject(s)
Aging , Language , Memory , Motor Skills , Problem Solving , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male
4.
J Appl Gerontol ; 32(1): 3-30, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25473924

ABSTRACT

We compared the extent to which subjective report of activities of daily living (ADLs) by caregivers and older adults were associated with objective measures of older adults' cognition. In independent studies (Study 1 N = 238; Study 2 N = 295), bivariate correlations and multiple regression analyses examined the association of caregiver and self-rated reports of older adult basic, instrumental, and total ADLs and older adult cognition. We examined the magnitude of the caregiver/self-report discrepancy and older adult cognition. In both studies, caregiver reports more accurately accounted for older adult cognitive differences. Older adult visuospatial/constructional deficits were uniquely related to caregiver basic ADL reports. Results indicate that caregiver reports of older adult ADLs are more reliable indicators of older adult cognition than self-reports, and this difference grows as older adult cognition decreases. Thus, older adult ADL assessment may be useful in providing information on potential cognitive decline.


Subject(s)
Activities of Daily Living , Aging/psychology , Caregivers/psychology , Cognition/physiology , Geriatric Assessment/methods , Self Report , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Retrospective Studies , Young Adult
5.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 18(6): 1071-80, 2012 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23039909

ABSTRACT

Accurate measurement of cognitive function is critical for understanding the disease course of Alzheimer's disease (AD). Detecting cognitive change over time can be confounded by level of premorbid intellectual function or cognitive reserve and lead to under- or over-diagnosis of cognitive impairment and AD. Statistical models of cognitive performance that include cognitive reserve can improve sensitivity to change and clinical efficacy. We used confirmatory factor analysis to test a four-factor model composed of memory/language, processing speed/executive function, attention, and cognitive reserve factors in a group of cognitively healthy older adults and a group of participants along the spectrum of amnestic mild cognitive impairment to AD (aMCI-AD). The model showed excellent fit for the control group (χ(2) = 100; df = 78; CFI = .962; RMSEA = .049) and adequate fit for the aMCI-AD group (χ(2) = 1750; df = 78; CFI = .932; RMSEA = .085). Although strict invariance criteria were not met, invariance testing to determine if factor structures are similar across groups yielded acceptable absolute model fits and provide evidence in support of configural, metric, and scalar invariance. These results provide further support for the construct validity of cognitive reserve in healthy and memory impaired older adults.


Subject(s)
Aging , Alzheimer Disease/complications , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Neuropsychological Tests , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales
6.
J Aging Res ; 2012: 493598, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23029615

ABSTRACT

The present study used a coordinated analyses approach to examine the association of physical activity and cognitive change in four longitudinal studies. A series of multilevel growth models with physical activity included both as a fixed (between-person) and time-varying (within-person) predictor of four domains of cognitive function (reasoning, memory, fluency, and semantic knowledge) was used. Baseline physical activity predicted fluency, reasoning and memory in two studies. However, there was a consistent pattern of positive relationships between time-specific changes in physical activity and time-specific changes in cognition, controlling for expected linear trajectories over time, across all four studies. This pattern was most evident for the domains of reasoning and fluency.

7.
J Aging Res ; 2012: 461592, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23024862

ABSTRACT

Engagement in cognitively stimulating activities has been considered to maintain or strengthen cognitive skills, thereby minimizing age-related cognitive decline. While the idea that there may be a modifiable behavior that could lower risk for cognitive decline is appealing and potentially empowering for older adults, research findings have not consistently supported the beneficial effects of engaging in cognitively stimulating tasks. Using observational studies of naturalistic cognitive activities, we report a series of mixed effects models that include baseline and change in cognitive activity predicting cognitive outcomes over up to 21 years in four longitudinal studies of aging. Consistent evidence was found for cross-sectional relationships between level of cognitive activity and cognitive test performance. Baseline activity at an earlier age did not, however, predict rate of decline later in life, thus not supporting the concept that engaging in cognitive activity at an earlier point in time increases one's ability to mitigate future age-related cognitive decline. In contrast, change in activity was associated with relative change in cognitive performance. Results therefore suggest that change in cognitive activity from one's previous level has at least a transitory association with cognitive performance measured at the same point in time.

8.
J Aging Res ; 2012: 287438, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22991665

ABSTRACT

Social activity is typically viewed as part of an engaged lifestyle that may help mitigate the deleterious effects of advanced age on cognitive function. As such, social activity has been examined in relation to cognitive abilities later in life. However, longitudinal evidence for this hypothesis thus far remains inconclusive. The current study sought to clarify the relationship between social activity and cognitive function over time using a coordinated data analysis approach across four longitudinal studies. A series of multilevel growth models with social activity included as a covariate is presented. Four domains of cognitive function were assessed: reasoning, memory, fluency, and semantic knowledge. Results suggest that baseline social activity is related to some, but not all, cognitive functions. Baseline social activity levels failed to predict rate of decline in most cognitive abilities. Changes in social activity were not consistently associated with cognitive functioning. Our findings do not provide consistent evidence that changes in social activity correspond to immediate benefits in cognitive functioning, except perhaps for verbal fluency.

9.
Brain Imaging Behav ; 6(4): 584-98, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22718430

ABSTRACT

Recent changes in diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer's disease (AD) state that biomarkers can enhance certainty in a diagnosis of AD. In the present study, we combined cognitive function and brain morphology, a potential imaging biomarker, to predict conversion from mild cognitive impairment to AD. We identified four biomarkers, or cortical signatures of cognition (CSC), from regressions of cortical thickness on neuropsychological factors representing memory, executive function/processing speed, language, and visuospatial function among participants in the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Neuropsychological factor scores were created from a previously validated multidimensional factor structure of the neuropsychological battery in ADNI. Mean thickness of each CSC at the baseline study visit was used to evaluate risk of conversion to clinical AD among participants with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and rate of decline on the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale Sum of Boxes (CDR-SB) score. Of 307 MCI participants, 119 converted to AD. For all domain-specific CSC, a one standard deviation thinner cortical thickness was associated with an approximately 50% higher hazard of conversion and an increase of approximately 0.30 points annually on the CDR-SB. In combined models with a domain-specific CSC and neuropsychological factor score, both CSC and factor scores predicted conversion to AD and increasing clinical severity. The present study indicated that factor scores and CSCs for memory and language both significantly predicted risk of conversion to AD and accelerated deterioration in dementia severity. We conclude that predictive models are best when they utilize both neuropsychological measures and imaging biomarkers.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Brain/pathology , Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Diagnosis, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychometrics/methods , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Alzheimer Disease/complications , Cognitive Dysfunction/complications , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reproducibility of Results , Sensitivity and Specificity , Severity of Illness Index
10.
Neurodegener Dis ; 9(4): 176-86, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22456451

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous studies have revealed that functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal in specific brain regions correlates with cross-sectional performance on standardized clinical trial measures in Alzheimer's disease (AD); however, the relationship between longitudinal change in fMRI-BOLD signal and neuropsychological performance remains unknown. OBJECTIVE: To identify changes in regional fMRI-BOLD activity that tracks change in neuropsychological performance in mild AD dementia over 6 months. METHODS: Twenty-four subjects (mean age 71.6) with mild AD dementia (mean Mini Mental State Examination 21.7, Global Clinical Dementia Rating 1.0) on stable donepezil dosing participated in two task-related fMRI sessions consisting of a face-name paired associative encoding memory paradigm 24 weeks apart during a randomized placebo-controlled pharmaco-fMRI drug study. Regression analysis was used to identify regions where the change in fMRI activity for Novel > Repeated stimulus contrast was associated with the change scores on postscan memory tests and the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test (FCSRT). RESULTS: Correlations between changes in postscan memory accuracy and changes in fMRI activity were observed in regions including the angular gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus and cerebellum. Correlations between changes in FCSRT-free recall and changes in fMRI were observed in regions including the inferior parietal lobule, precuneus, hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus. CONCLUSION: Changes in encoding-related fMRI activity in regions implicated in mnemonic networks correlated with changes in psychometric measures of episodic memory retrieval performed outside the scanner. These exploratory results support the potential of fMRI activity to track cognitive change and detect signals of short-term pharmacologic effect in early-phase AD studies.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/pathology , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Cognition/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxygen/blood , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/drug therapy , Brain Mapping , Donepezil , Dopamine Agents/therapeutic use , Female , Humans , Indans/therapeutic use , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Memantine/therapeutic use , Nootropic Agents/therapeutic use , Piperidines/therapeutic use , Regression Analysis
11.
Alzheimers Res Ther ; 3(6): 32, 2011 Nov 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22078663

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: With the recent publication of new criteria for the diagnosis of preclinical Alzheimer's disease (AD), there is a need for neuropsychological tools that take premorbid functioning into account in order to detect subtle cognitive decline. Using demographic adjustments is one method for increasing the sensitivity of commonly used measures. We sought to provide a useful online z-score calculator that yields estimates of percentile ranges and adjusts individual performance based on sex, age and/or education for each of the neuropsychological tests of the National Alzheimer's Coordinating Center Uniform Data Set (NACC, UDS). In addition, we aimed to provide an easily accessible method of creating norms for other clinical researchers for their own, unique data sets. METHODS: Data from 3,268 clinically cognitively-normal older UDS subjects from a cohort reported by Weintraub and colleagues (2009) were included. For all neuropsychological tests, z-scores were estimated by subtracting the raw score from the predicted mean and then dividing this difference score by the root mean squared error term (RMSE) for a given linear regression model. RESULTS: For each neuropsychological test, an estimated z-score was calculated for any raw score based on five different models that adjust for the demographic predictors of SEX, AGE and EDUCATION, either concurrently, individually or without covariates. The interactive online calculator allows the entry of a raw score and provides five corresponding estimated z-scores based on predictions from each corresponding linear regression model. The calculator produces percentile ranks and graphical output. CONCLUSIONS: An interactive, regression-based, normative score online calculator was created to serve as an additional resource for UDS clinical researchers, especially in guiding interpretation of individual performances that appear to fall in borderline realms and may be of particular utility for operationalizing subtle cognitive impairment present according to the newly proposed criteria for Stage 3 preclinical Alzheimer's disease.

12.
Gerontologist ; 51(2): 179-89, 2011 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20974657

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE OF THE STUDY: There is lack of consensus on the best method of functional assessment, and there is a paucity of studies on daily functioning in centenarians. We sought to compare associations between performance-based, self-report, and proxy report of functional status in centenarians. We expected the strongest relationships between proxy reports and observed performance of basic activities of daily living (BADLs) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs). We hypothesized that the discrepancy between self-report and observed daily functioning would be modified by cognitive status. We additionally sought to provide clinicians with estimates of centenarians' observed daily functioning based on their mental status in combination with subjective measures of activities of daily living (ADLs). DESIGN AND METHODS: Two hundred and forty-four centenarians from the Georgia Centenarian Study were included in this cross-sectional population-based study. Measures included the Direct Assessment of Functional Status, self-report and proxy report of functional status, and the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). RESULTS: Associations between observed and proxy reports were stronger than between observed and self-report across BADL and IADL measures. A significant MMSE by type of report interaction was found, indicating that lower MMSE performance is associated with a greater discrepancy between subjective and objective ADL measures. IMPLICATIONS: Results demonstrate associations between 3 methods of assessing functional status and suggest proxy reports are generally more accurate than self-report measures. Cognitive status accounted for some of the discrepancy between observed and self-reports, and we provide clinicians with tables to estimate centenarians' performance on observed functional measures based on MMSE and subjective report of functional status.


Subject(s)
Activities of Daily Living , Geriatric Assessment/methods , Observer Variation , Psychomotor Performance , Self-Assessment , Aged, 80 and over , Aging/physiology , Aging/psychology , Cognition , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Georgia , Humans , Male , Mental Status Schedule , Neuropsychological Tests , Regression, Psychology , Self Report
13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20521181

ABSTRACT

We present normative data from a large population-based sample of centenarians for several brief, global neurocognitive tasks amenable for frail elders. Comparative data from octogenarians are included. A total of 244 centenarians and 80 octogenarians from Phase III of the Georgia Centenarian Study were administered the Mini-Mental Status Examination, Severe Impairment Battery, and Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale. Centenarians (age 98-107) were stratified into three age cohorts (98-99, 100-101, 102-107), octogenarians into two 5- year cohorts (80-84, 85-89). Highly significant differences were observed between groups on all measures, with greater variation and dispersion in performance among centenarians, as well as stronger associations between age and performance. Descriptive statistics and normative ranges (unweighted and population-weighted) are provided by age cohort. Additional statistics are provided by education level. While most previous centenarian studies have used convenience samples, ours is population-based and likely more valid for comparison in applied settings. Results suggest centenarians look different than do even the oldest age range of most normative aging datasets (e.g., 85-90). Results support using global measures of neurocognition to describe cognitive status in the oldest old, and we provide normative comparisons to do so.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Cognition Disorders/physiopathology , Cognition/physiology , Geriatric Assessment , Age Factors , Aged, 80 and over , Aging/psychology , Cognition Disorders/epidemiology , Cohort Studies , Community Health Planning , Executive Function/physiology , Georgia/epidemiology , Humans , Mental Status Schedule , Neuropsychological Tests , Severity of Illness Index , Statistics as Topic
14.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 16(4): 672-8, 2010 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20441687

ABSTRACT

The immunoglobulins (IgGs) for beta amyloid (Abeta) and receptors for the advanced glycation end products (RAGE) have previously been shown to be related to memory and language measures in a mixed neurological sample of older adults. In this study, we examined group differences in Abeta and RAGE IgGs, as well as the relationship between both IgGs and cognitive performance in nondiabetic older adults with normal cognition, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and probable Alzheimer's disease (AD). We found RAGE and Abeta levels to be elevated in some AD participants, leading to significant AD-control group differences. While there was an overall correlation between both IgG levels and global cognition across all three groups, this relationship was largely attributable to group differences in cognition, highlighted by considerable variability within groups in the relationship between IgG levels and cognition. While findings do not support a consistent relationship between cognition and either IgG, further research with larger samples is needed to better characterize cognitive differences between AD participants with high versus low Abeta and RAGE titers.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/immunology , Amyloid beta-Peptides/immunology , Cognition Disorders/immunology , Immunoglobulin G/blood , Peptide Fragments/immunology , Receptor for Advanced Glycation End Products/immunology , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Alzheimer Disease/blood , Analysis of Variance , Cognition Disorders/blood , Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Statistics as Topic
15.
J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci ; 64(2): 264-71, 2009 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19196906

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Blood-based immunoglobulins (IgGs) may mark the presence of amyloid plaques characterizing the progression of Alzheimer's disease (AD). Previous studies suggest that anti-RAGE and anti-Abeta IgGs increase proportionately with accumulation of amyloid-beta (Abeta) peptides at receptor sites for advanced glycation end products (RAGE), within cortical areas of brain tissue. We assessed the relationship between these potential markers and an AD-type cognitive profile. We hypothesized that these specific IgG levels would be positively correlated with Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) scores as well as index scores on the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) in domains associated with cortical function. METHODS: Participants were 118 older adults (mean age = 74, standard deviation = 10.5) drawn from the community and local physician referrals. Participants were reassigned into five groups based on CDR. Blood IgG levels were determined through an affinity purification process. RESULTS: Analysis of covariance analyses revealed that CDR scores were significantly related to anti-RAGE, F(4,106) = 12.93, p < .001, and anti-Abeta, F(4,106) = 17.08, p < .001, after controlling for age and total IgG levels. Regression analyses indicated significant variance accounted for by anti-RAGE and anti-Abeta above and beyond total IgG effects. Additional regression identified specific RBANS domains accounting for significant variance in anti-RAGE levels including language (t = -3.74, p < .001) and delayed memory (t = -2.31, p < .05), whereas language accounted for a significant amount of variance in anti-Abeta levels (t = -3.96, p < .001). CONCLUSIONS: Anti-RAGE and anti-Abeta IgGs correlate strongly with global scores of dementia. Furthermore, they are associated with a profile of deficiency in domains associated with specific cortical function. Results suggest potential for anti-Abeta and anti-RAGE IgGs as blood biomarkers for AD.


Subject(s)
Aging/blood , Amyloid beta-Peptides/metabolism , Cognition Disorders/immunology , Dementia/immunology , Immunoglobulin G/blood , Receptors, Immunologic/blood , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aging/physiology , Amyloid beta-Peptides/blood , Biomarkers/blood , Cognition Disorders/blood , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Cohort Studies , Dementia/blood , Dementia/diagnosis , Disease Progression , Female , Humans , Immunoglobulin G/metabolism , Logistic Models , Male , Multivariate Analysis , Probability , Prognosis , Receptor for Advanced Glycation End Products , Receptors, Immunologic/antagonists & inhibitors , Receptors, Immunologic/metabolism , Retrospective Studies , Risk Factors , Sensitivity and Specificity , Severity of Illness Index , Sex Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...