Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 64
Filter
1.
Evol Hum Sci ; 6: e26, 2024.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38689896

ABSTRACT

While humans are highly cooperative, they can also behave spitefully. Yet spite remains understudied. Spite can be normatively driven and while previous experiments have found some evidence that cooperation and punishment may spread via social learning, no experiments have considered the social transmission of spiteful behaviour. Here we present an online experiment where, following an opportunity to earn wealth, we asked participants to choose an action towards an anonymous partner across a full spectrum of social behaviour, from spite to altruism. In accordance with cultural evolutionary theory, participants were presented with social information that varied in source and content. Across six conditions, we informed participants that either the majority or the highest earner had chosen to behave spitefully, neutrally or altruistically. We found an overall tendency towards altruism, but at lower levels among those exposed to spite compared with altruism. We found no difference between social information that came from the majority or the highest earner. Exploratory analysis revealed that participants' earnings negatively correlated with altruistic behaviour. Our results contrast with previous literature that report high rates of spite in experimental samples and a greater propensity for individuals to copy successful individuals over the majority.

2.
MethodsX ; 11: 102292, 2023 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37593412

ABSTRACT

The density of epidermal ridges in a fingerprint varies predictably by age and sex. Archaeologists are therefore interested in using recovered fingerprints to learn about the ancient people who produced them. Recent studies focus on estimating the age and sex of individuals by measuring their fingerprints with one of two similar metrics: mean ridge breadth (MRB) or ridge density (RD). Yet these attempts face several critical problems: expected values for adult females and adolescent males are inherently indistinguishable, and inter-assemblage variation caused by biological and technological differences cannot be easily estimated. Each of these factors greatly decreases the accuracy of predictions based on individual prints, and together they condemn this strategy to relative uselessness. However, information in fingerprints from across an assemblage can be pooled to generate a more accurate depiction of potter demographics. We present a new approach to epidermal ridge density analysis using Bayesian mixture models with the following key benefits:•Age and sex are estimated more accurately than existing methods by incorporating a data-driven understanding of how demographics and ridge density covary.•Uncertainty in demographic estimates is automatically quantified and included in output.•The Bayesian framework can be easily adapted to fit the unique needs of different researchers.

3.
J Clin Monit Comput ; 37(5): 1303-1311, 2023 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37004663

ABSTRACT

We investigated whether machine learning (ML) analysis of ICU monitoring data incorporating volumetric capnography measurements of mean alveolar PCO2 can partition venous admixture (VenAd) into its shunt and low V/Q components without manipulating the inspired oxygen fraction (FiO2). From a 21-compartment ventilation / perfusion (V/Q) model of pulmonary blood flow we generated blood gas and mean alveolar PCO2 data in simulated scenarios with shunt values from 7.3% to 36.5% and a range of FiO2 settings, indirect calorimetry and cardiac output measurements and acid- base and hemoglobin oxygen affinity conditions. A 'deep learning' ML application, trained and validated solely on single FiO2 bedside monitoring data from 14,736 scenarios, then recovered shunt values in 500 test scenarios with true shunt values 'held back'. ML shunt estimates versus true values (n = 500) produced a linear regression model with slope = 0.987, intercept = -0.001 and R2 = 0.999. Kernel density estimate and error plots confirmed close agreement. With corresponding VenAd values calculated from the same bedside data, low V/Q flow can be reported as VenAd-shunt. ML analysis of blood gas, indirect calorimetry, volumetric capnography and cardiac output measurements can quantify pulmonary oxygenation deficits as percentage shunt flow (V/Q = 0) versus percentage low V/Q flow (V/Q > 0). High fidelity reports are possible from analysis of data collected solely at the operating FiO2.


Subject(s)
Capnography , Lung , Humans , Ventilation-Perfusion Ratio/physiology , Computer Simulation , Oxygen , Pulmonary Gas Exchange/physiology
4.
J Clin Monit Comput ; 37(1): 201-210, 2023 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35691965

ABSTRACT

Using computer simulation we investigated whether machine learning (ML) analysis of selected ICU monitoring data can quantify pulmonary gas exchange in multi-compartment format. A 21 compartment ventilation/perfusion (V/Q) model of pulmonary blood flow processed 34,551 combinations of cardiac output, hemoglobin concentration, standard P50, base excess, VO2 and VCO2 plus three model-defining parameters: shunt, log SD and mean V/Q. From these inputs the model produced paired arterial blood gases, first with the inspired O2 fraction (FiO2) adjusted to arterial saturation (SaO2) = 0.90, and second with FiO2 increased by 0.1. 'Stacked regressor' ML ensembles were trained/validated on 90% of this dataset. The remainder with shunt, log SD, and mean 'held back' formed the test-set. 'Two-Point' ML estimates of shunt, log SD and mean utilized data from both FiO2 settings. 'Single-Point' estimates used only data from SaO2 = 0.90. From 3454 test gas exchange scenarios, two-point shunt, log SD and mean estimates produced linear regression models versus true values with slopes ~ 1.00, intercepts ~ 0.00 and R2 ~ 1.00. Kernel density and Bland-Altman plots confirmed close agreement. Single-point estimates were less accurate: R2 = 0.77-0.89, slope = 0.991-0.993, intercept = 0.009-0.334. ML applications using blood gas, indirect calorimetry, and cardiac output data can quantify pulmonary gas exchange in terms describing a 20 compartment V/Q model of pulmonary blood flow. High fidelity reports require data from two FiO2 settings.


Subject(s)
Lung , Pulmonary Gas Exchange , Humans , Pulmonary Gas Exchange/physiology , Computer Simulation , Lung/physiology , Pulmonary Circulation , Respiration , Ventilation-Perfusion Ratio/physiology
5.
Proc Biol Sci ; 289(1986): 20221614, 2022 11 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36321489

ABSTRACT

The past 2 Myr have seen both unprecedented environmental instability and the evolution of the human capacity for complex culture. This, along with the observation that cultural evolution occurs faster than genetic evolution, has led to the suggestion that culture is an adaptation to an unstable environment. We test this hypothesis by examining the ability of human social learning to respond to environmental changes. We do this by inserting human participants (n = 4800) into evolutionary simulations with a changing environment while varying the social information available to individuals across five conditions. We find that human social learning shows some signs of adaptation to environmental instability, including critical social learning, the adoption of up-and-coming traits and, unexpectedly, contrariness. However, these are insufficient to avoid significant fitness declines when the environment changes, and many individuals are highly conformist, which exacerbates the fitness effects of environmental change. We conclude that human social learning reflects a compromise between the competing needs for flexibility to accommodate environmental change and fidelity to accurately transmit valuable cultural information.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Social Learning , Humans , Adaptation, Physiological , Biological Evolution , Culture
6.
PLoS One ; 16(8): e0255346, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34379646

ABSTRACT

Prestige-biased social learning (henceforth "prestige-bias") occurs when individuals predominantly choose to learn from a prestigious member of their group, i.e. someone who has gained attention, respect and admiration for their success in some domain. Prestige-bias is proposed as an adaptive social-learning strategy as it provides a short-cut to identifying successful group members, without having to assess each person's success individually. Previous work has documented prestige-bias and verified that it is used adaptively. However, the domain-specificity and generality of prestige-bias has not yet been explicitly addressed experimentally. By domain-specific prestige-bias we mean that individuals choose to learn from a prestigious model only within the domain of expertise in which the model acquired their prestige. By domain-general prestige-bias we mean that individuals choose to learn from prestigious models in general, regardless of the domain in which their prestige was earned. To distinguish between domain specific and domain general prestige we ran an online experiment (n = 397) in which participants could copy each other to score points on a general-knowledge quiz with varying topics (domains). Prestige in our task was an emergent property of participants' copying behaviour. We found participants overwhelmingly preferred domain-specific (same topic) prestige cues to domain-general (across topic) prestige cues. However, when only domain-general or cross-domain (different topic) cues were available, participants overwhelmingly favoured domain-general cues. Finally, when given the choice between cross-domain prestige cues and randomly generated Player IDs, participants favoured cross-domain prestige cues. These results suggest participants were sensitive to the source of prestige, and that they preferred domain-specific cues even though these cues were based on fewer samples (being calculated from one topic) than the domain-general cues (being calculated from all topics). We suggest that the extent to which people employ a domain-specific or domain-general prestige-bias may depend on their experience and understanding of the relationships between domains.


Subject(s)
Social Learning/physiology , Trust/psychology , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Internet-Based Intervention , Knowledge , Male , Middle Aged , Social Desirability , Social Perception , Young Adult
7.
Biol Lett ; 17(6): 20200767, 2021 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34157236

ABSTRACT

Social learning enables adaptive information acquisition provided that it is not random but selective. To understand species typical decision-making and to trace the evolutionary origins of social learning, the heuristics social learners use need to be identified. Here, we experimentally tested the nature of majority influence in the zebra finch. Subjects simultaneously observed two demonstrator groups differing in relative and absolute numbers (ratios 1 : 2/2 : 4/3 : 3/1 : 5) foraging from two novel food sources (black and white feeders). We find that demonstrator groups influenced observers' feeder choices (social learning), but that zebra finches did not copy the majority of individuals. Instead, observers were influenced by the foraging activity (pecks) of the demonstrators and in an anti-conformist fashion. These results indicate that zebra finches are not conformist, but are public information users.


Subject(s)
Finches , Social Learning , Animals , Vocalization, Animal
9.
J Clin Oncol ; 39(7): 840-859, 2021 03 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33405943

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: The aim of this joint guideline is to provide evidence-based recommendations to practicing physicians and other healthcare providers on definitive-intent chemoradiotherapy for patients with stage II-IVA nasopharyngeal carcinoma (NPC). METHODS: The Chinese Society of Clinical Oncology (CSCO) and ASCO convened an expert panel of radiation oncology, medical oncology, surgery, and advocacy representatives. The literature search included systematic reviews, meta-analyses, and randomized controlled trials published from 1990 through 2020. Outcomes of interest included survival, distant and locoregional disease control, and quality of life. Expert panel members used this evidence and informal consensus to develop evidence-based guideline recommendations. RESULTS: The literature search identified 108 relevant studies to inform the evidence base for this guideline. Five overarching clinical questions were addressed, which included subquestions on radiotherapy (RT), chemotherapy sequence, and concurrent, induction, and adjuvant chemotherapy options. RECOMMENDATIONS: Evidence-based recommendations were developed to address aspects of care related to chemotherapy in combination with RT for the definitive-intent treatment of stage II to IVA NPC.Additional information is available at www.asco.org/head-neck-cancer-guidelines.


Subject(s)
Chemoradiotherapy/standards , Medical Oncology/standards , Nasopharyngeal Carcinoma/therapy , Nasopharyngeal Neoplasms/therapy , Chemoradiotherapy/adverse effects , Chemoradiotherapy/mortality , Consensus , Humans , Nasopharyngeal Carcinoma/mortality , Nasopharyngeal Carcinoma/secondary , Nasopharyngeal Neoplasms/mortality , Nasopharyngeal Neoplasms/pathology , Neoplasm Staging , Quality of Life , Radiation Oncology/standards , Treatment Outcome
10.
Nat Hum Behav ; 5(7): 857-867, 2021 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33510392

ABSTRACT

Incentives for priority of discovery are hypothesized to harm scientific reliability. Here, we evaluate this hypothesis by developing an evolutionary agent-based model of a competitive scientific process. We find that rewarding priority of discovery causes populations to culturally evolve towards conducting research with smaller samples. This reduces research reliability and the information value of the average study. Increased start-up costs for setting up single studies and increased payoffs for secondary results (also known as scoop protection) attenuate the negative effects of competition. Furthermore, large rewards for negative results promote the evolution of smaller sample sizes. Our results confirm the logical coherence of scoop protection reforms at several journals. Our results also imply that reforms to increase scientific efficiency, such as rapid journal turnaround times, may produce collateral damage by incentivizing lower-quality research; in contrast, reforms that increase start-up costs, such as pre-registration and registered reports, may generate incentives for higher-quality research.


Subject(s)
Competitive Behavior , Motivation , Reproducibility of Results , Research , Reward , Humans , Registries
11.
J Clin Monit Comput ; 35(4): 757-764, 2021 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32435932

ABSTRACT

Hyperlactatemia is a documented complication of diabetic ketoacidosis (DKA). Lactate responses during DKA treatment have not been studied and were the focus of this investigation. Blood gas and electrolyte data from 25 DKA admissions to ICU were sequenced over 24 h from the first Emergency Department sample. Hyperlactatemia (> 2 mmol/L) was present in 22 of 25 DKA presentations [mean concentration = 3.2 mmol/L]. In 18 time-series (72%), all concentrations normalized in ≤ 2.6 h (aggregate decay t1/2 = 2.29 h). In the remaining 7 (28%), hyperlactatemia persisted > 12 h. These were females (P = 0.04) with relative anemia (hemoglobin concentrations 131 v 155 g/L; P = 0.004) and lower nadir glucose concentrations (5.2 v 8.0 mmol/L, P = 0.003). Their aggregate glucose decay curve commenced higher (42 mmol/L v 29 mmol/L), descending towards a lower asymptote (8 mmol/L v 11 mmol/L). Tonicity decay showed similar disparities. There was equivalent resolution of metabolic acidosis and similar lengths of stay in both groups. Hyperlactatemia is common in DKA. Resolution is often rapid, but high lactates can persist. Females with high glucose concentrations corrected aggressively are more at risk. Limiting initial hyperglycemia correction to ≥ 11 mmol/L may benefit.


Subject(s)
Diabetic Ketoacidosis , Hyperlactatemia , Critical Care , Diabetic Ketoacidosis/complications , Female , Hospitalization , Humans , Lactic Acid
12.
Biol Lett ; 16(11): 20200660, 2020 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33232652

ABSTRACT

Conformist transmission is a cognitively simple decision-making process by which observers are disproportionately likely to follow the majority. It has been studied in multiple species because theory suggests it can create stable cultural variation. However, the current theory assumes that while conformist transmission favours the majority, it is otherwise unbiased and does not systematically transform information, even though such biases are widely documented. Here, we relax this assumption, requiring conformist observers to infer the size of the majority from finite observations of their group mates. Because such inference can be subject to bias, it can lead to the biased transformation of transmitted information. We find that when individuals are biased (even weakly) the capacity of conformist transmission to sustain traditions is reduced and, in many cases, removed entirely. This suggests that the emphasis on conformist transmission as a source of stable cultural variation may be misplaced.


Subject(s)
Learning , Social Conformity , Humans
13.
Behav Brain Sci ; 43: e171, 2020 08 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32772971

ABSTRACT

What promised to be a refreshing addition to cumulative cultural evolution, by moving the focus from cultural transmission to technological innovation, falls flat through a lack of thoroughness, explanatory power, and data. A comprehensive theory of cumulative cultural change must carefully integrate all existing evidence in a cohesive multi-level account. We argue that the manuscript fails to do so convincingly.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Humans , Inventions
14.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 11974, 2020 07 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32686722

ABSTRACT

The use of non-nutritive sweeteners such as saccharin is widely prevalent. Although saccharin is considered safe for human consumption, it produces behavioral changes in experimental animals. We report that saccharin's behavioral effects are much more pervasive than currently recognized. In a mouse model, saccharin exposure produced motor impulsivity not only in the saccharin-exposed males but also in their offspring. In addition, the offspring showed locomotor hyperactivity and working memory deficit not observed in fathers. Spermatazoal DNA was hypermethylated in the saccharin-exposed fathers, especially at dopamine receptor promoter regions, suggesting that epigenetic modification of germ cell DNA may mediate transgenerational transmission of behavioral phenotypes. Dopamine's role in hyperactivity was further highlighted by the finding that the stimulant drug methylphenidate mitigated the hyperactivity. Nicotine is another substance that is widely used. Its use via smokeless tobacco products, some of which contain saccharin, is on the rise contributing to concerns about adverse outcomes of co-exposure to saccharin and nicotine. We found that co-exposure of male mice to saccharin and nicotine produced significant behavioral impairment in their offspring. Thus, our data point to potential adverse neurobehavioral consequences of exposure to saccharin alone or saccharin and nicotine for the exposed individuals and their descendants.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/drug effects , Nicotine/pharmacology , Saccharin/pharmacology , Animals , Body Weight/drug effects , Crosses, Genetic , DNA Methylation/drug effects , DNA Methylation/genetics , Drinking Behavior/drug effects , Female , Male , Methylphenidate/pharmacology , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Motor Activity/drug effects , Phenotype , Spermatozoa/drug effects , Spermatozoa/metabolism
15.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 375(1803): 20190504, 2020 07 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32475322

ABSTRACT

Humans possess an unusual combination of traits, including our cognition, life history, demographics and geographical distribution. Many theories propose that these traits have coevolved. Such hypotheses have been explored both theoretically and empirically, with experiments examining whether human behaviour meets theoretical expectations. However, theory must make assumptions about the human mind, creating a potentially problematic gap between models and reality. Here, we employ a series of 'experimental evolutionary simulations' to reduce this gap and to explore the coevolution of learning, memory and childhood. The approach combines aspects of theory and experiment by inserting human participants as agents within an evolutionary simulation. Across experiments, we find that human behaviour supports the coevolution of learning, memory and childhood, but that this is dampened by rapid environmental change. We conclude by discussing both the implications of these findings for theories of human evolution and the utility of experimental evolutionary simulations more generally. This article is part of the theme issue 'Life history and learning: how childhood, caregiving and old age shape cognition and culture in humans and other animals'.


Subject(s)
Biological Coevolution , Cognition , Cultural Evolution , Learning , Life History Traits , Memory , Computer Simulation , Humans
16.
Cognition ; 197: 104165, 2020 04.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31958668

ABSTRACT

In a process known as the Baldwin Effect, developmental plasticity, such as learning, has been argued to accelerate the biological evolution of high-fitness traits, including language and complex intelligence. Here we investigate the evolutionary consequences of developmental plasticity by asking which aspects of a plastic trait are the focus of genetic change. The aspects we consider are: (i) dependencies between elements of a trait, (ii) the importance of each element to fitness, and (iii) the difficulty of acquiring each element through plasticity. We also explore (iv) how cultural inheritance changes the relationship between plasticity and genetic change. We find that evolution by natural selection preferentially fixes elements that are depended upon by others, important to fitness, or difficult to acquire through plasticity, but that cultural inheritance can suppress and even reverse genetic change. We replicate some of these effects in experimental evolutionary simulations with human learners. We conclude that what the Baldwin Effect affects depends upon the mechanism of plasticity, which for behavior and cognition includes the psychology of learning.


Subject(s)
Biological Evolution , Learning , Cognition , Humans , Phenotype
17.
Evol Hum Sci ; 2: e43, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37588362

ABSTRACT

Humans are remarkable in their reliance on cultural inheritance, and the ecological success this has produced. Nonetheless, we lack a thorough understanding of how the cognitive underpinnings of cultural transmission affect cultural adaptation across diverse tasks. Here, we use an agent-based simulation to investigate how different learning mechanisms (both social and asocial) interact with task structure to affect cultural adaptation. Specifically, we compared learning through refinement, recombination or both, in tasks of different difficulty, with learners of different asocial intelligence. We find that for simple tasks all learning mechanisms are roughly equivalent. However, for hard tasks, performance was maximised when populations consisted of highly intelligent individuals who nonetheless rarely innovated and instead recombined existing information. Our results thus show that cumulative cultural adaptation relies on the combination of individual intelligence and 'blind' population-level processes, although the former may be rarely used. The counterintuitive requirement that individuals be highly intelligent, but rarely use this intelligence, may help resolve the debate over the role of individual intelligence in cultural adaptation.

18.
J Clin Monit Comput ; 34(3): 397-399, 2020 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31254240
19.
J Clin Monit Comput ; 34(1): 17-20, 2020 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31079292

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To place in context the potential value of isolated plasma strong ion difference (SID) calculations and strong ion gap (SIG) calculations versus suggested cut-down versions such as SIDa adj and the BICgap respectively. METHODS: Stewart's physical chemical approach is seen as a mathematical model of isolated plasma not displacing traditional Copenhagen and Boston approaches. Scanning tools for unmeasured ions based on the Principle of Electrical Neutrality such as the SIG and suggested cut-down versions such as the albumin adjusted anion gap and the BICgap are evaluated for accuracy and clinical usefulness. RESULTS: Plasma SID and abbreviations such as SIDa adj are not independent variables in vivo since they vary with PCO due to Gibbs Donnan ion traffic. They can also exhibit positive and negative bias, and SID values must be partnered with non-volatile weak acid concentrations when evaluating metabolic acid-base status. The BICgap calculation is a cut down version of the SIG fixed for pH 7.4. It includes phosphate but is otherwise similar in form to the albumin corrected anion gap, with similar sensitivity and specificity characteristics. CONCLUSIONS: Clinicians are unlikely to find SID calculations or cut-down versions such as the SIDa adj clinically useful. The albumin corrected anion gap is in current use and easily determined by mental arithmetic from point of care anion gap printouts plus recent plasma albumin measurements. Any slight advantage of the BICgap would be offset by the complexity of its calculation.


Subject(s)
Acid-Base Equilibrium , Acid-Base Imbalance/blood , Acid-Base Imbalance/diagnosis , Albumins/analysis , Anions , Bicarbonates , Critical Care/methods , Humans , Hydrogen-Ion Concentration , Ions , Models, Theoretical , Phosphates/chemistry , Reproducibility of Results
20.
Crit Care Resusc ; 21(4): 274-83, 2019 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31778634

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Clinically apparent cerebral oedema during diabetic ketoacidosis (DKA) is rare and more common in children and young adults. Subclinical oedema with mild brain dysfunction is more frequent, with unknown long term effects. Rapid tonicity changes may be a factor although not well studied. Guidelines recommend capping hypertonicity resolution at ≤ 3 mOsmol/kg/h. OBJECTIVES: To audit current DKA management in the emergency department (ED) and in the intensive care unit (ICU) for tonicity benchmark compliance, and to determine interactions between plasma tonicity, plasma glucose concentrations and blood haemoglobin concentrations. METHODS: Twenty-five adult DKA admissions from ED to ICU were studied retrospectively. Blood gas and electrolyte data were sequenced for 24 hours from first ED blood sample. RESULTS: Sampling was frequent (median, 11 times per day; range, 6-26). Tonicity reduction was largely accomplished by the first ICU blood sample and exceeded 3 mOsmol/ kg/h in 72% of admissions. Correlation with haemoglobin reduction (haemodilution) rates exceeded correlation with glucose rates (R2 = 0.52 v 0.38). In benchmark noncompliant admissions, haemodilution was more rapid (6.1 g/L/h v 2.1 g/L/h; P = 0.001). Although also true of glucose reduction (4.5 mmol/L/h v 2.2 mmol/L/h; P = 0.007), there was no interaction between haemodilution and glucose reduction (R2 = 0.09). CONCLUSIONS: Major tonicity reductions often exceeding guidelines were common by ICU admission. Correcting DKA-induced hypertonicity at ≤ 3 mOsmol/kg/h requires controlled hyperglycaemia correction and, based on our data, avoidance of high fluid replacement rates; for example, sufficient to reduce haemoglobin concentrations by > 3 g/L/h, unless there is evidence of intravascular hypovolaemia.


Subject(s)
Critical Care/methods , Critical Illness/therapy , Diabetic Ketoacidosis/therapy , Hemodilution , Blood Glucose/metabolism , Child , Diabetic Ketoacidosis/blood , Diabetic Ketoacidosis/metabolism , Emergency Service, Hospital/organization & administration , Hospitalization , Humans , Intensive Care Units/organization & administration , Retrospective Studies , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...