Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 18 de 18
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neurosci Conscious ; 2019(1): niz007, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31191982

ABSTRACT

Using the method of experience sampling, we studied the fluctuations in thought generation and cognitive control strength during the wakeful hours of the day, centered around episodes of mind wandering. Thought generation, measured in terms of the number of thoughts that concurrently occupy the mind at sampling time, goes through regular 4-6 h cycles, suggesting the mind operates with an alternation of focused and multitasking modes. Cognitive control strength rises and falls in relative coordination with thought generation, implying that both are occasionally misaligned. This happens, in particular, when cognitive control suddenly drops after having been keeping up with a cycle of thought generation. When this drop occurs while the thought generation cycle is still up, mind wandering appears. As cognitive control quickly resumes before returning to intermediate values, the thought generation cycle begins to fall again, and the mind wandering episode comes to an end. Implications regarding the role of long-term regulation in mind-wandering processes are discussed.

2.
PLoS One ; 12(4): e0173776, 2017.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28380064

ABSTRACT

Electrophysiological signals of cortical activity show a range of possible frequency and amplitude modulations, both within and across regions, collectively known as cross-frequency coupling. To investigate whether these modulations could be considered as manifestations of the same underlying mechanism, we developed a neural mass model. The model provides five out of the theoretically proposed six different coupling types. Within model components, slow and fast activity engage in phase-frequency coupling in conditions of low ambient noise level and with high noise level engage in phase-amplitude coupling. Between model components, these couplings can be coordinated via slow activity, giving rise to more complex modulations. The model, thus, provides a coherent account of cross-frequency coupling, both within and between components, with which regional and cross-regional frequency and amplitude modulations could be addressed.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Humans , Models, Biological
3.
Biol Cybern ; 110(2-3): 171-92, 2016 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27241189

ABSTRACT

Brain activity shows phase-amplitude coupling between its slow and fast oscillatory components. We study phase-amplitude coupling as recorded at individual sites, using a modified version of the well-known Wendling neural mass model. To the population of fast inhibitory interneurons of this model, we added external modulatory input and dynamic self-feedback. These two modifications together are sufficient to let the inhibitory population serve as a limit-cycle oscillator, with frequency characteristics comparable to the beta and gamma bands. The frequency and power of these oscillations can be tuned through the time constant of the dynamic and modulatory input. Alpha band activity is generated, as is usual in such models, as a result of interactions of pyramidal neurons and a population of slow inhibitory interneurons. The slow inhibitory population activity directly influences the fast oscillations via the synaptic gain between slow and fast inhibitory populations. As a result, the amplitude envelope of the fast oscillation is coupled to the phase of the slow activity; this result is consistent with the notion that phase-amplitude coupling is effectuated by interactions between inhibitory interneurons.


Subject(s)
Interneurons/physiology , Neural Inhibition , Cybernetics , Models, Neurological , Pyramidal Cells/physiology
4.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 26(5): 1168-79, 2014 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24345169

ABSTRACT

Global workspace access is considered as a critical factor for the ability to report a visual target. A plausible candidate mechanism for global workspace access is coupling of slow and fast brain activity. We studied coupling in EEG data using cross-frequency phase-amplitude modulation measurement between delta/theta phases and beta/gamma amplitudes from two experimental sessions, held on different days, of a typical attentional blink (AB) task, implying conscious access to targets. As the AB effect improved with practice between sessions, theta-gamma and theta-beta coupling increased generically. Most importantly, practice effects observed in delta-gamma and delta-beta couplings were specific to performance on the AB task. In particular, delta-gamma coupling showed the largest increase in cases of correct target detection in the most challenging AB conditions. All these practice effects were observed in the right temporal region. Given that the delta band is the main frequency of the P3 ERP, which is a marker of global workspace activity for conscious access, and because the gamma band is involved in visual object processing, the current results substantiate the role of phase-amplitude modulation in conscious access to visual target representations.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Consciousness/physiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Photic Stimulation/methods , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
5.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 7: 26, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23818877

ABSTRACT

In scrutinizing a scene, the eyes alternate between fixations and saccades. During a fixation, two component processes can be distinguished: visual encoding and selection of the next fixation target. We aimed to distinguish the neural correlates of these processes in the electrical brain activity prior to a saccade onset. Participants viewed color photographs of natural scenes, in preparation for a change detection task. Then, for each participant and each scene we computed an image heat map, with temperature representing the duration and density of fixations. The temperature difference between the start and end points of saccades was taken as a measure of the expected task-relevance of the information concentrated in specific regions of a scene. Visual encoding was evaluated according to whether subsequent change was correctly detected. Saccades with larger temperature difference were more likely to be followed by correct detection than ones with smaller temperature differences. The amplitude of presaccadic activity over anterior brain areas was larger for correct detection than for detection failure. This difference was observed for short "scrutinizing" but not for long "explorative" saccades, suggesting that presaccadic activity reflects top-down saccade guidance. Thus, successful encoding requires local scanning of scene regions which are expected to be task-relevant. Next, we evaluated fixation target selection. Saccades "moving up" in temperature were preceded by presaccadic activity of higher amplitude than those "moving down". This finding suggests that presaccadic activity reflects attention deployed to the following fixation location. Our findings illustrate how presaccadic activity can elucidate concurrent brain processes related to the immediate goal of planning the next saccade and the larger-scale goal of constructing a robust representation of the visual scene.

6.
Front Syst Neurosci ; 7: 18, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23754990

ABSTRACT

In active vision, eye-movements depend on perceivers' internal state. We investigated peri-fixation brain activity for internal state-specific tagging. Human participants performed a task, in which a visual object was presented for identification in lateral visual field, to which they moved their eyes as soon as possible from a central fixation point. Next, a phrase appeared in the same location; the phrase could either be an easy or hard question about the object, answered by pressing one of two alternative response buttons, or it could be an instruction to simply press one of these two buttons. Depending on whether these messages were blocked or randomly mixed, one of two different internal states was induced: either the task was known in advance or it wasn't. Eye movements and electroencephalogram (EEG) were recorded simultaneously during task performance. Using eye-event-time-locked averaging and independent component analysis, saccade- and fixation-related components were identified. Coss-frequency phase-synchrony was observed between the alpha/beta1 ranges of fixation-related and beta2/gamma1 ranges of saccade-related activity 50 ms prior to fixation onset in the mixed-phrase condition only. We interpreted this result as evidence for internal state-specific tagging.

7.
Vision Res ; 85: 20-5, 2013 Jun 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23041560

ABSTRACT

Human observers show remarkable efficiency in statistical estimation; they are able, for instance, to estimate the mean size of visual objects, even if their number exceeds the capacity limits of focused attention. This ability has been understood as the result of a distinct mode of attention, i.e. distributed attention. Compared to the focused attention mode, working memory representations under distributed attention are proposed to be more compressed, leading to reduced working memory loads. An alternate proposal is that distributed attention uses less structured, feature-level representations. These would fill up working memory (WM) more, even when target set size is low. Using event-related potentials, we compared WM loading in a typical distributed attention task (mean size estimation) to that in a corresponding focused attention task (object recognition), using a measure called contralateral delay activity (CDA). Participants performed both tasks on 2, 4, or 8 different-sized target disks. In the recognition task, CDA amplitude increased with set size; notably, however, in the mean estimation task the CDA amplitude was high regardless of set size. In particular for set-size 2, the amplitude was higher in the mean estimation task than in the recognition task. The result showed that the task involves full WM loading even with a low target set size. This suggests that in the distributed attention mode, representations are not compressed, but rather less structured than under focused attention conditions.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Size Perception/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Judgment/physiology , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Young Adult
8.
Psychol Rev ; 119(4): 745-69, 2012 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22823385

ABSTRACT

Two separate lines of study have clarified the role of selectivity in conscious access to visual information. Both involve presenting multiple targets and distracters: one simultaneously in a spatially distributed fashion, the other sequentially at a single location. To understand their findings in a unified framework, we propose a neurodynamic model for Visual Selection and Awareness (ViSA). ViSA supports the view that neural representations for conscious access and visuo-spatial working memory are globally distributed and are based on recurrent interactions between perceptual and access control processors. Its flexible global workspace mechanisms enable a unitary account of a broad range of effects: It accounts for the limited storage capacity of visuo-spatial working memory, attentional cueing, and efficient selection with multi-object displays, as well as for the attentional blink and associated sparing and masking effects. In particular, the speed of consolidation for storage in visuo-spatial working memory in ViSA is not fixed but depends adaptively on the input and recurrent signaling. Slowing down of consolidation due to weak bottom-up and recurrent input as a result of brief presentation and masking leads to the attentional blink. Thus, ViSA goes beyond earlier 2-stage and neuronal global workspace accounts of conscious processing limitations.


Subject(s)
Awareness/physiology , Computer Simulation , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Models, Biological , Nerve Net/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Attention/physiology , Attentional Blink/physiology , Brain/physiology , Consciousness/physiology , Cues , Humans , Neural Networks, Computer , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Time Factors
9.
Neuroimage ; 56(3): 1598-607, 2011 Jun 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21406236

ABSTRACT

We considered the hypothesis that spontaneous dissociation between the direction of attention and eye movement causes encoding failure in change detection. We tested this hypothesis by analyzing eye fixation-related potentials (EFRP) at the encoding stage of a change blindness task; when participants freely inspect a scene containing an unmarked target region, in which a change will occur in a subsequent presentation. We measured EFRP amplitude prior to the execution of a saccade, depending on its starting or landing position relative to the target region. For those landings inside the target region, we found a difference in EFRP between correct detection and failure. Overall, correspondence between EFRP amplitude and the size of the saccade predicted successful detection of change; lack of correspondence was followed by change blindness. By contrast, saccade sizes and fixation durations around the target region were unrelated to subsequent change detection. Since correspondence between EFRP and eye movement indicates that overt attention was given to the target region, we concluded that overt attention is needed for successful encoding and that dissociation between eye movement and attention leads to change blindness.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Attention/physiology , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Eye Movements/physiology , Eyelids/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Saccades/physiology , Young Adult
10.
Mem Cognit ; 39(4): 700-7, 2011 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21264585

ABSTRACT

One critical question for the front end of models of visual-word recognition and reading is whether the stage of letter position coding is purely orthographic or whether phonology is (to some degree) involved. To explore this issue, we conducted a silent reading experiment in Japanese Kana--a script in which orthography and phonology can be easily separated--using a technique that is highly sensitive to phonological effects (i.e., Rayner's (1975) boundary technique). Results showed shorter fixation times on the target word when the parafoveal preview was a transposed-mora nonword (a.ri.me.ka [アリメカ]-a.me.ri.ka [アメリカ]) than when the preview was a replacement-mora nonword (a.ka.ho.ka [アカホカ] -a.me.ri.ka [アメリカ]). More critically, fixation times on the target word were remarkably similar when the parafoveal preview was a transposed-consonant nonword (a.re.mi.ka [アレミカ]-a.ri.me.ka [アリメカ]) and when the parafoveal preview was an orthographic control nonword (a.ke.hi.ka [アケヒカ]-a.me.ri.ka [アメリカ]). Thus, these findings offer strong support for the view that letter/mora position coding during silent reading is orthographic in nature.


Subject(s)
Language , Phonetics , Reading , Semantics , Writing , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Perceptual Masking , Psycholinguistics , Reaction Time , Recognition, Psychology , Saccades , Visual Fields
11.
Perception ; 39(12): 1659-71, 2010.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21425703

ABSTRACT

We investigated cortical activity in response to abstract and representational paintings in artists and non-artists. Participants engaged in visual inspection of works of art and recalled them immediately afterwards through mental imagery. Meanwhile, we recorded their EEG, and calculated the power of their alpha band and theta band activity afterwards. In accordance with previous studies, theta band and alpha band power differed between artists and non-artists; these differences were found to depend, however, on the abstract or representational character of the paintings. Differences between abstract and representational art, and between inspection and imagery, occurred in alpha band power for non-artists only and in theta band power for artists. These results were taken to suggest that effects in artists reflect sustained focused attention and perceptual flexibility; in non-artists motivation and engagement with the task. The results were essentially whole-head, despite the local character of the measurement.


Subject(s)
Imagination/physiology , Paintings , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Statistical , Young Adult
12.
Prog Brain Res ; 176: 123-34, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19733753

ABSTRACT

Even with unimpaired vision, observers sometimes fail to see things right before their open eyes. A typical example is the attentional blink effect, a period in which observers are unable to detect a target item in a sequence of stimuli, for as long as the previous one occupies their mind. Having considered a range of mechanisms proposed to explain attentional blink effect, we arrive at our preferred explanation, which ascribes the effect to a contextually motivated imbalance in the allocation of attentional resources between earlier and later target information. We interpret in this perspective our data on how the attentional blink effect changes as a result of practice.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Attentional Blink/physiology , Practice, Psychological , Humans , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Time Factors
13.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 17(12): 1969-79, 2005 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16356332

ABSTRACT

The attentional blink (AB) phenomenon occurs when perceivers must report two targets embedded in a sequence of distracters; if the first target precedes the second by 200-600 msec, the second one is often missed. We investigated AB by measuring dynamic cross-lag phase synchronization for 565 electrode pairs in 40-Hz-range EEG. Phase synchrony, on average, was higher in experimental conditions, where two targets are reported, than in control conditions, where only the second target is reported. The effect occurred in electrode pairs covering the whole head. Timing of the synchrony was crucial: Brief episodes of enhanced synchrony occurred 100-500 msec before expected target onset in AB conditions where the second target was correctly reported. These results show that intrinsic brain dynamics produce anticipatory synchronization in transient assemblies of cortical areas. Enhanced levels of anticipatory synchronization occur in response to the demands of the task in conditions where the system's limited capacity is under strain.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Blinking/physiology , Electroencephalography , Adolescent , Adult , Algorithms , Cortical Synchronization , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Electrodes , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
14.
Perception ; 33(10): 1185-200, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15693664

ABSTRACT

In two experiments amodal completion of partly occluded shapes was investigated by recording eye movements in a directed visual-search task. Participants searched arrays of shapes in a prescribed order for target figures that could partly be occluded. Longer gaze durations were found on occlusion patterns than on truncated control patterns for targets but not for non-targets. This effect of occlusion was restricted to a subset of the stimuli. A second experiment was carried out to establish whether this restriction resulted from structural properties of the stimuli or their familiarity. Occlusion patterns in this experiment were ambiguous with respect to structure, allowing both local and global completions. One of the completions was always less familiar than the other. The results showed longer gazes only for the less familiar completions, irrespective of whether they were local or global.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Closure/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Psychophysics , Time Factors
15.
Percept Psychophys ; 66(7): 1227-45, 2004 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15751478

ABSTRACT

Participants saw a standard scene of three objects on a desktop and then judged whether a comparison scene was either the same, except for the viewpoint of the scene, or different, when one or more of the objects either exchanged places or were rotated around their center. As in Nakatani, Pollatsek, and Johnson (2002), judgment times were longer when the rotation angles of the comparison scene increased, and the size of the rotation effect varied for different axes and was larger for same judgments than for different judgments. A second experiment, which included trials without the desktop, indicated that removing the desktop frame of reference mainly affected the y-axis rotation conditions (the axis going vertically through the desktop plane). In addition, eye movement analyses indicated that the process was far more than a simple analogue rotation of the standard scene. The total response latency was divided into three components: the initial eye movement latency, the first-pass time, and the second-pass time. The only indication of a rotation effect in the time to execute the first two components was for z-axis (plane of sight) rotations. Thus, for x- and y-axis rotations, rotation effects occurred only in the probability of there being a second pass and the time to execute it. The data are inconsistent either with an initial rotation of the memory representation of the standard scene to the orientation of the comparison scene or with a holistic alignment of the comparison scene prior to comparing it with the memory representation of the standard scene. Indeed, the eye movement analysis suggests that little of the increased response time for rotated comparison scenes is due to something like a time-consuming analogue process but is, instead, due to more comparisons on individual objects being made (possibly more double checking).


Subject(s)
Depth Perception , Eye Movements , Imagination , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Concept Formation , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Mathematical Computing , Probability , Problem Solving , Psychophysics , Reaction Time
16.
Perception ; 32(6): 681-98, 2003.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12892429

ABSTRACT

According to a widely cited finding by Ellis and Stark (1978 Perception 7 575-581), the duration of eye fixations is longer at the instant of perceptual reversal of an ambiguous figure than before or after the reversal. However, long fixations are more likely to include samples of an independent random event than are short fixations. This sampling bias would produce the pattern of results also when no correlation exists between fixation duration and perceptual reversals. When an appropriate correction is applied to the measurement of fixation durations, the effect disappears. In fact, there are fewer actual button-presses during the long intervals than would be expected by chance. Moving-window analyses performed on eye-fixation data reveal that no unique eye event is associated with switching behaviour. However, several indicators, such as blink frequency, saccade frequency, and the direction of the saccade, are each differentially sensitive to perceptual and response-related aspects of the switching process. The time course of these indicators depicts switching behaviour as a process of cascaded stages.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Blinking/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Poisson Distribution , Psychophysics , Saccades/physiology
17.
J Dermatol ; 29(9): 593-8, 2002 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12392068

ABSTRACT

A case of angiosarcoma arising from the nose of a 69-year-old man is presented in this report. The patient was treated with a combination of recombinant interleukin-2 (rIL-2, Celeuk), electron beam irradiation, and surgery. He died 27 months after diagnosis, but there was no apparent remote metastasis.


Subject(s)
Hemangiosarcoma/pathology , Hemangiosarcoma/therapy , Skin Neoplasms/pathology , Skin Neoplasms/therapy , Aged , Biopsy, Needle , Combined Modality Therapy , Fatal Outcome , Hemangiosarcoma/diagnosis , Humans , Immunohistochemistry , Interleukin-2/administration & dosage , Japan , Male , Nose , Radiotherapy, Adjuvant , Skin Neoplasms/diagnosis , Surgical Procedures, Operative/methods
18.
Q J Exp Psychol A ; 55(1): 115-39, 2002 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11873843

ABSTRACT

Three experiments investigated scene recognition across viewpoint changes, involving same/different judgements on scenes consisting of three objects on a desktop. On same trials, the comparison scene appeared either from the same viewpoint as the standard scene or from a different viewpoint with the desktop rotated about one or more axes. Different trials were created either by interchanging the locations of two or three of the objects (location change condition), or by rotating either one or all three of the objects around their vertical axes (orientation change condition). Response times and errors increased as a function of the angular distance between the standard and comparison views, but this effect was bigger for rotations around the vertical axis than for those about the line of sight or horizontal axis. Furthermore, the time to detect location changes was less than that to detect orientation changes, and this difference increased with increasing angular disparity between the standard and comparison scenes. Rotation times estimated in a double-axis rotation were no longer than other rotations in depth, indicating that alignment was not necessarily simpler around a "natural" axis of rotation. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that scenes, like many objects, may be represented in a viewpoint dependent manner and recognized by aligning standard and comparison views, but that the alignment of scenes is not a holistic process.


Subject(s)
Recognition, Psychology , Feedback , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Random Allocation
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...