Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 7: 23-9, 2014 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24270044

ABSTRACT

The study explores infants' ability to generate on-line predictions about others' action goals through the recruitment of motor resonance mechanisms. To this aim, electromyography was recorded from mouth-opening suprahyoid muscles (SM) of 9-month-old infants while watching a video of an adult agent reaching-to-grasp an object and bringing it either to mouth or head. The results demonstrated, for the first time, that at the age of 9 months there is a dynamic mirror modulation of SM activity by action observation, with the infant's muscles responsible for the action final goal being recruited from the action outset. The comparison with the responses of 6-month-olds tested on the same task showed that in younger and older infants there is a different chronometry of the SM activation with respect to the different phases of the observed action (i.e., bringing vs. grasping, respectively). Results suggest that motor resonance mechanisms triggered within the infants' motor system by action observation undergo gradual development during the first year of life. They also indicate that motor resonance may reflect anticipation of the agent's intention based on the goal of the action.


Subject(s)
Child Development , Electromyography , Intention , Motion Perception , Motor Activity , Adult , Female , Humans , Infant , Male
2.
Dev Sci ; 16(6): 793-800, 2013 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24118707

ABSTRACT

In primates and adult humans direct understanding of others' action is provided by mirror mechanisms matching action observation and action execution (e.g. Casile, Caggiano & Ferrari, 2011). Despite the growing body of evidence detailing the existence of these mechanisms in the adult human brain, their origins and early development are largely unknown. In this study, for the first time, electromyographic (EMG) measures were used to shed light on the emergence of mirror motor mechanisms in infancy. EMG activity was recorded while 6- and 3-month-old infants watched two videos displaying an agent reaching for, grasping and bringing an object either to the mouth or to the head. Results indicate that the motor system of 6-month-olds, but not 3-month-olds, was recruited and selectively modulated during observation of the goal-directed actions, favoring the idea that mirror mechanisms driving action understanding gradually emerge during early development.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Electromyography , Imitative Behavior/physiology , Motor Cortex/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Feedback, Sensory , Female , Humans , Infant , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology
3.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 30(8): 2367-81, 2009 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19034897

ABSTRACT

The control of visuo-spatial attention entails the joint contribution of goal-directed (endogenous) and stimulus-driven (exogenous) factors. However, little is known about the neural bases of the interplay between these two mechanisms. To address this issue, we presented endogenous (spatially informative) and exogenous (noninformative) visual cues sequentially within the same trial (double-cue paradigm) during fMRI, crossing factorially the validity of the two cues. We found that both endogenous and exogenous cues affected behavioral performance, speeding-up or slowing-down target discrimination when valid and invalid, respectively. Despite the double-cue paradigm maximizes the interplay between endogenous and exogenous factors, the two types of cue affected responses in an independent manner without any significant effect of congruence. The imaging data revealed increased activation in separate cortical areas following invalid endogenous and invalid exogenous cues. A fronto-parietal system was activated during invalid endogenous trials, whereas a region at the temporo-occipital junction was activated during invalid exogenous trials. Within both circuits, activity was unaffected by the validity of the other cue. These results indicate the existence of separate, noninteracting neural circuits for endogenous and exogenous reorienting of visuo-spatial attention.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time , Signal Detection, Psychological/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis , Visual Cortex/physiology , Young Adult
4.
Cell Signal ; 18(12): 2200-8, 2006 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16806823

ABSTRACT

Heterotrimeric GTP-binding (G) proteins transduce hormone-induced signals to their effector enzymes, which include several phospholipases. In particular, the G(o)/G(i) and G(q) protein families have been shown to couple signaling to phospholipase A(2) (PLA(2)), phospholipase C, and phospholipase D, while the G(12)/G(13) family has been linked to the activation of small GTPases of the Rho family, and hence, to phospholipase D activation. Here, we demonstrate that in CHO cells, the G(12)/G(13) family is also able to activate cPLA(2)alpha, through the activation of RhoA and, subsequently, ERK1/2. Hormone-induced arachidonic acid release increased as a consequence of Galpha(13) overexpression, and was inhibited through inhibition of Galpha(13) signaling. The Galpha(13)-mediated cPLA(2)alpha activation was inhibited by pharmacological blockade of ERK1/2 with either U0126 or PD98059, and by RhoA inactivation with C3 toxin or a dominant-negative RhoA (N19RhoA), and was stimulated by the serine-threonine phosphatase inhibitor calyculin A. Our data thus identify a pathway of cPLA(2)alpha regulation that is initiated by thrombin and purinergic receptor activation, and that signals through Galpha(13), RhoA and ERK1/2, with the involvement of a calyculin-sensitive phosphatase.


Subject(s)
GTP-Binding Protein alpha Subunits, G12-G13/metabolism , Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinases/metabolism , Phospholipases A/metabolism , Animals , Arachidonic Acid/metabolism , Butadienes/pharmacology , CHO Cells , Cricetinae , Cricetulus , Cytosol/enzymology , Enzyme Activation , Enzyme Inhibitors/pharmacology , GTP-Binding Protein alpha Subunits, G12-G13/genetics , Group IV Phospholipases A2 , Imidazoles/pharmacology , Marine Toxins , Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinase 1/antagonists & inhibitors , Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinase 1/metabolism , Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinase 3/antagonists & inhibitors , Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinase 3/metabolism , Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinases/antagonists & inhibitors , Mutation/genetics , Nitriles/pharmacology , Oxazoles/pharmacology , Phospholipases A/antagonists & inhibitors , Phosphoric Monoester Hydrolases/antagonists & inhibitors , Phosphoric Monoester Hydrolases/metabolism , Phosphorylation , Pyridines/pharmacology , Pyrrolidines/pharmacology , Transfection , rhoA GTP-Binding Protein/metabolism
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...