Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 15 de 15
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Soc Neurosci ; 12(4): 409-418, 2017 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27151065

ABSTRACT

The personal/impersonal distinction of moral decision-making postulates intuitive emotional responses from medial frontal activity and rational evaluation from lateral frontal activity. This model can be analyzed in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), a disorder characterized by impaired emotional intuitions, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) involvement, and relative sparing of lateral frontal regions. Moral dilemmas were presented to 10 bvFTD, 11 Alzheimer's disease (AD), and 9 healthy control (HC) participants while recording skin conductance responses, a measure of emotional arousal. We evaluated their personal versus impersonal conflict, subjective discomfort, and adherence to social norms. Replicating prior work, bvFTD participants were more willing to harm in the personal, but not the impersonal, dilemma compared to AD and HC groups. BvFTD participants had lower arousal and less of an increase in conflict on the personal versus the impersonal dilemma, in contrast to increased arousal and conflict for the AD and HC groups. Furthermore, bvFTD participants verbalized less discomfort, a correlate of low adherence to social norms. These findings support impaired emotional reactions to moral dilemmas in bvFTD and vmPFC lesions and the personal/impersonal model. It suggests a reversion to utilitarian-like considerations when emotional intuition is impaired in the brain.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Conflict, Psychological , Frontotemporal Dementia/physiopathology , Frontotemporal Dementia/psychology , Morals , Analysis of Variance , Arousal/physiology , Autonomic Nervous System/physiopathology , Decision Making/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychophysics , Regression Analysis , Social Behavior
2.
Conserv Biol ; 30(6): 1200-1211, 2016 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27062105

ABSTRACT

Despite increasing support for conservation globally, controversy over specific conservation policies persists among diverse stakeholders. Investigating the links between morals in relation to conservation can help increase understanding about why humans support or oppose policy, especially related to human-wildlife conflict or human conflict over wildlife. Yet the moral dimension of human-wildlife conflict has mostly gone unconsidered and unmeasured; thus, policy and programmatic efforts to reduce controversy may be missing a key part of the equation. We conducted a web-based survey (n = 1239 respondents) in Michigan (U.S.A.) to investigate cognitive and emotional influences on the value-behavior relationship. Respondents were identified by their interest and involvement in Michigan wolf management. The survey consisted of questions about values, emotions, cognitions, and behaviors relative to wolves in Michigan. We used path analysis to explore whether emotions and cognitions mediated the relationship between value and behavior. Most respondents attributed intrinsic value to wolves (n = 734) and all life (n = 773) and engaged in behaviors that benefited wolf populations and ecosystems regardless of stakeholder group (e.g., environmentalist, farmer). Attributing intrinsic value to wolves was positively related to favorable emotions toward wolves and cognitive assessments that hunting and trapping of wolves is unacceptable. Despite similarities in attribution of intrinsic value, groups differed in emotions and cognitions about wolf hunting. These differences provide a useful way to predict stakeholder behavior. Our findings may inform interventions aimed at increasing support for wolf management policies and positive interactions among stakeholders and wildlife. Leveraging agreement over intrinsic value may foster cooperation among stakeholders and garner support for controversial conservation policy.


Subject(s)
Conservation of Natural Resources , Morals , Public Opinion , Adult , Aged , Animals , Animals, Wild , Female , Humans , Male , Michigan , Middle Aged , Wolves
3.
Neuroimage ; 121: 171-83, 2015 Nov 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26166625

ABSTRACT

Associations linking a fearful experience to a member of a social group other than one's own (out-group) are more resistant to change than corresponding associations to a member of one's own (in-group) (Olsson et al., 2005; Kubota et al., 2012), providing a possible link to discriminative behavior. Using a fear conditioning paradigm, we investigated the neural activity underlying aversive learning biases towards in-group (White) and out-group (Black) members, and their predictive value for discriminatory interactive behavior towards novel virtual members of the racial out-group (n=20). Our results indicate that activity in brain regions previously linked to conditioned fear and perception of individuals belonging to the racial out-groups, or otherwise stigmatized groups, jointly contribute to the expression of race-based biases in learning and behavior. In particular, we found that the amygdala and anterior insula (AI) played key roles in differentiating between in-group and out-group faces both when the faces were paired with an aversive event (acquisition) and when no more shocks were administered (extinction). In addition, functional connectivity between the amygdala and the fusiform gyrus increased during perception of conditioned out-group faces. Moreover, we showed that brain activity in the fear-learning-bias network was related to participants' discriminatory interactions with novel out-group members on a later day. Our findings are the first to identify the neural mechanism of fear learning biases towards out-group members, and its relationship to interactive behavior. Our findings provide important clues towards understanding the mechanisms underlying biases between social groups.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/physiology , Brain Mapping/methods , Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Facial Recognition/physiology , Fear/physiology , Interpersonal Relations , Racism , Social Perception , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
5.
Behav Brain Sci ; 38: e159, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26785604

ABSTRACT

Evolutionary psychologists are personally liberal, just as social psychologists are. Yet their research has rarely been perceived as liberally biased--if anything, it has been erroneously perceived as motivated by conservative political agendas. Taking a closer look at evolutionary psychologists might offer the broader social psychology community guidance in neutralizing some of the biases Duarte et al. discuss.


Subject(s)
Motivation , Politics , Biological Evolution , Humans , Perception , Psychology , Psychology, Social
6.
PLoS One ; 9(4): e95167, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24736596

ABSTRACT

The functions of cultural beliefs are often opaque to those who hold them. Accordingly, to benefit from cultural evolution's ability to solve complex adaptive problems, learners must be credulous. However, credulity entails costs, including susceptibility to exploitation, and effort wasted due to false beliefs. One determinant of the optimal level of credulity is the ratio between the costs of two types of errors: erroneous incredulity (failing to believe information that is true) and erroneous credulity (believing information that is false). This ratio can be expected to be asymmetric when information concerns hazards, as the costs of erroneous incredulity will, on average, exceed the costs of erroneous credulity; no equivalent asymmetry characterizes information concerning benefits. Natural selection can therefore be expected to have crafted learners' minds so as to be more credulous toward information concerning hazards. This negatively-biased credulity extends general negativity bias, the adaptive tendency for negative events to be more salient than positive events. Together, these biases constitute attractors that should shape cultural evolution via the aggregated effects of learners' differential retention and transmission of information. In two studies in the U.S., we demonstrate the existence of negatively-biased credulity, and show that it is most pronounced in those who believe the world to be dangerous, individuals who may constitute important nodes in cultural transmission networks. We then document the predicted imbalance in cultural content using a sample of urban legends collected from the Internet and a sample of supernatural beliefs obtained from ethnographies of a representative collection of the world's cultures, showing that beliefs about hazards predominate in both.


Subject(s)
Cultural Evolution , Culture , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Ethnology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Young Adult
7.
Behav Brain Sci ; 35(2): 89-90, 2012 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22289268

ABSTRACT

Group assortative biases are stronger in regions where pathogen stress has been historically prevalent. Pushing the logic of this approach, extensions should include investigations of how cultural norms related to prosociality and relational striving may also covary with regional pathogen stress. Likewise, the pan-specific observation that diseased animals show decreased motor activity to facilitate recovery suggests that norms relevant to sickness behaviors may also vary as a function of regional parasite stress.


Subject(s)
Communicable Diseases/psychology , Family Relations , Parasitic Diseases/psychology , Religion and Psychology , Social Behavior , Stress, Psychological , Humans
8.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 367(1589): 670-9, 2012 Mar 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22271783

ABSTRACT

The social science literature contains numerous examples of human tribalism and parochialism-the tendency to categorize individuals on the basis of their group membership, and treat ingroup members benevolently and outgroup members malevolently. We hypothesize that this tribal inclination is an adaptive response to the threat of coalitional aggression and intergroup conflict perpetrated by 'warrior males' in both ancestral and modern human environments. Here, we describe how male coalitional aggression could have affected the social psychologies of men and women differently and present preliminary evidence from experimental social psychological studies testing various predictions from the 'male warrior' hypothesis. Finally, we discuss the theoretical implications of our research for studying intergroup relations both in humans and non-humans and discuss some practical implications.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Biological Evolution , Conflict, Psychological , Psychology, Social , Competitive Behavior , Discrimination, Psychological , Fear/psychology , Female , Galvanic Skin Response , Group Processes , Humans , Male , Sex Characteristics , Sexual Behavior/psychology , Social Perception
9.
Psychol Sci ; 22(7): 860-5, 2011 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21636834

ABSTRACT

Recent research has shown that White women's bias against Black men increases with elevated fertility across the menstrual cycle. We demonstrate that the association between fertility and intergroup bias is not limited to groups defined by race, but extends to group categories that are minimally defined, and may depend on the extent to which women associate out-group men with physical formidability. In Study 1, Black and White women with strong associations between the racial out-group and physical formidability displayed greater bias against out-group men as conception risk increased. Study 2 replicated these results in a minimal-group paradigm. These findings are consistent with the notion that women may be endowed with a psychological system that generates intergroup bias via mechanisms that rely on categorization heuristics and perceptions of the physical formidability of out-group men, particularly when the costs of sexual coercion are high.


Subject(s)
Menstrual Cycle/psychology , Prejudice , Adolescent , Adult , Black People/psychology , Female , Group Processes , Humans , Male , Social Identification , Social Perception , Stereotyping , White People/psychology , Young Adult
10.
Psychol Sci ; 21(9): 1311-7, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20660891

ABSTRACT

What is the role of ecology in automatic cognitive processes and social behavior? Our motivated-preparation account posits that priming a social category readies the individual for adaptive behavioral responses to that category-responses that take into account the physical environment. We present the first evidence showing that the cognitive responses (Study 1) and the behavioral responses (Studies 2a and 2b) automatically elicited by a social-category prime differ depending on a person's physical surroundings. Specifically, after priming with pictures of Black men (a threatening out-group), participants responded with either aggressive behavior (fight) or distancing behavior (flight), depending on what action was allowed by the situation. For example, when participants were seated in an enclosed booth (no distancing behavior possible) during priming, they showed increased accessibility of fight-related action semantics; however, when seated in an open field (distancing behavior possible), they showed increased accessibility of flight-related action semantics. These findings suggest that an understanding of automaticity must consider its situated nature.


Subject(s)
Escape Reaction , Semantics , Social Behavior , Adolescent , Adult , Defense Mechanisms , Environment , Female , Humans , Male , Motivation , Racial Groups , Social Perception , Young Adult
11.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 98(6): 933-45, 2010 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20515248

ABSTRACT

Adopting an evolutionary approach to the psychology of race bias, we posit that intergroup conflict perpetrated by male aggressors throughout human evolutionary history has shaped the psychology of modern forms of intergroup bias and that this psychology reflects the unique adaptive problems that differ between men and women in coping with male aggressors from groups other than one's own. Here we report results across 4 studies consistent with this perspective, showing that race bias is moderated by gender differences in traits relevant to threat responses that differ in their adaptive utility between the sexes-namely, aggression and dominance motives for men and fear of sexual coercion for women. These results are consistent with the notion that the psychology of intergroup bias is generated by different psychological systems for men and women, and the results underscore the importance of considering the gender of the outgroup target as well as the gender of the agent in psychological studies on prejudice and discrimination.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Conflict, Psychological , Gender Identity , Prejudice , Social Desirability , Social Identification , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Character , Coercion , Fear , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motivation , Psychological Theory , Sexual Behavior , Social Dominance , Violence/psychology , Young Adult
12.
Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol ; 16(2): 248-55, 2010 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20438163

ABSTRACT

This study examines direct, interactive, and indirect effects of racial identity and depression in a sample of 379 African American women. Results indicated that higher racial private and public regard were associated with lower depression. The relationship between private regard and depression was moderated by racial centrality, such that higher private regard was more strongly related to lower depression when women's race was a central part of their self-concept. Finally, results indicated that self-esteem fully mediated the relationship between private regard and depression and partially mediated the relationship between public regard and depression. The authors discuss the results in relation to reflected appraisal, the insulation hypothesis, and identity theory.


Subject(s)
Black or African American/psychology , Black or African American/statistics & numerical data , Depressive Disorder, Major/ethnology , Racial Groups , Social Identification , Adolescent , Adult , Depressive Disorder, Major/diagnosis , Depressive Disorder, Major/psychology , Female , Humans , Middle Aged , Self Concept , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
13.
Evol Psychol ; 8(4): 599-616, 2010 Oct 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22947823

ABSTRACT

Previous research suggests that several individual and cultural level attitudes, cognitions, and societal structures may have evolved to mitigate the pathogen threats posed by intergroup interactions. It has been suggested that these anti-pathogen defenses are at the root of conservative political ideology. Here, we test a hypothesis that political conservatism functions as a pathogen-avoidance strategy. Across three studies, we consistently find no relationship between sensitivity to pathogen disgust and multiple measures of political conservatism. These results are contrasted with theoretical perspectives suggesting a relationship between conservatism and pathogen avoidance, and with previous findings of a relationship between conservatism and disgust sensitivity.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Biological Evolution , Communicable Diseases/psychology , Emotions , Politics , Psychological Theory , Facial Expression , Fear , Female , Humans , Immune System , Individuality , Male , Morals , Pregnancy , Psychology, Social , Psychometrics/statistics & numerical data , Regression Analysis , Sexual Behavior/psychology , United States , Young Adult
14.
Psychol Sci ; 20(6): 661-5, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19422626

ABSTRACT

Although a considerable body of research explores alterations in women's mating-relevant preferences across the menstrual cycle, investigators have yet to examine the potential for the menstrual cycle to influence intergroup attitudes. We examined the effects of changes in conception risk across the menstrual cycle on intergroup bias and found that increased conception risk was positively associated with several measures of race bias. This association was particularly strong when perceived vulnerability to sexual coercion was high. Our findings highlight the potential for hypotheses informed by an evolutionary perspective to generate new knowledge about current social problems-an avenue that may lead to new predictions in the study of intergroup relations.


Subject(s)
Black People/psychology , Fertilization , Menstrual Cycle/ethnology , Menstrual Cycle/psychology , Prejudice , Social Identification , White People/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Avoidance Learning , Biological Evolution , Coercion , Female , Humans , Individuality , Risk , Sex Offenses/ethnology , Sex Offenses/psychology , Stereotyping , Young Adult
15.
Psychol Sci ; 20(2): 155-8, 2009 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19175529

ABSTRACT

Conditioning studies on humans and other primates show that fear responses acquired toward danger-relevant stimuli, such as snakes, resist extinction, whereas responses toward danger-irrelevant stimuli, such as birds, are more readily extinguished. Similar evolved biases may extend to human groups, as recent research demonstrates that a conditioned fear response to faces of persons of a social out-group resists extinction, whereas fear toward a social in-group is more readily extinguished. Here, we provide an important extension to previous work by demonstrating that this fear-extinction bias occurs solely when the exemplars are male. These results underscore the importance of considering how gender of the target stimulus affects psychological and physiological responses to out-group threat.


Subject(s)
Face , Fear , Adolescent , Adult , Black People , Extinction, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sex Factors , White People , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...