Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Child Dev ; 85(1): 326-37, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23638945

ABSTRACT

This study provides evidence that children give priority to ownership when judging who should use an object. Children (N = 269) and adults (N = 154) considered disputes over objects. In disputes between a character using an object and the owner of the object, children, as young as 3 years and as old as 7 years, sided with the owner, and did so more than adults. However, children aged 4 and older disregarded owners' rights in dilemmas where these were pitted against the need to prevent harm. These findings suggest that ownership is central in children's judgments about object use and constrain developmental accounts of how children acquire an appreciation of ownership.


Subject(s)
Child Development/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Ownership , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Conflict, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
2.
Child Dev ; 84(5): 1519-25, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23480031

ABSTRACT

It is impossible to perceive who owns an object; this must be inferred. One way that children make such inferences is through a first possession bias--when two agents each use an object, children judge the object belongs to the one who used it first. Two experiments show that this bias does not result from children directly inferring ownership from first possession; the experiments instead support an alternative account according to which the first possession bias reflects children's historical reasoning. In Experiment 1, eighty-five 3- to 5-year-olds only based inferences on first possession when it was informative about the past. In Experiment 2, thirty-two 5-year-olds based ownership judgments on testimony about past contact, while disregarding testimony about future contact.


Subject(s)
Concept Formation/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Ownership , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Perception/physiology , Recognition, Psychology
3.
Dev Psychol ; 48(1): 149-58, 2012 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21928884

ABSTRACT

People's behavior in relation to objects depends on whether they are owned. But how do people judge whether objects are owned? We propose that people expect human-made objects (artifacts) to be more likely to be owned than naturally occurring objects (natural kinds), and we examine the development of these expectations in young children. Experiment 1 found that when shown pictures of familiar kinds of objects, 3-year-olds expected artifacts to be owned and inanimate natural kinds to be non-owned. In Experiments 2A and 2B, 3-6-year-olds likewise had different expectations about the ownership of unfamiliar artifacts and natural kinds. Children at all ages viewed unfamiliar natural kinds as non-owned, but children younger than 6 years of age only endorsed artifacts as owned at chance rates. In Experiment 3, children saw the same pictures but were also told whether objects were human-made. With this information provided, even 3-year-olds viewed unfamiliar artifacts as owned. Finally, in Experiment 4, 4- and 5-year-olds chose unfamiliar artifacts over natural kinds when judging which object in a pair belongs to a person, but not when judging which the person prefers. These experiments provide first evidence about how children judge whether objects are owned. In contrast to claims that children think about natural kinds as being similar to artifacts, the current findings reveal that children have differing expectations about whether they are owned.


Subject(s)
Artifacts , Child Behavior , Child Development , Judgment/physiology , Age Factors , Analysis of Variance , Child , Child, Preschool , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Recognition, Psychology
4.
New Dir Child Adolesc Dev ; 2011(132): 79-89, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21671343

ABSTRACT

Appropriate behavior in relation to an object often requires judging whether it is owned and, if so, by whom. The authors propose accounts of how people make these judgments. Our central claim is that both judgments often involve making inferences about object history. In judging whether objects are owned, people may assume that artifacts (e.g., chairs) are owned and that natural objects (e.g., pinecones) are not. However, people may override these assumptions by inferring the history of intentional acts made in relation to objects. In judging who owns an object, people may often consider which person likely possessed the object in the past--such reasoning may be responsible for people's bias to assume that the first person known to possess an object is its owner.


Subject(s)
Decision Making , Judgment , Ownership , Child , Child Behavior , Child, Preschool , Cognition , Humans , Mental Recall , Object Attachment , Problem Solving , Psychology, Child
5.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 36(3): 671-85, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20438265

ABSTRACT

In 8 recognition experiments, we investigated the production effect-the fact that producing a word aloud during study, relative to simply reading a word silently, improves explicit memory. Experiments 1, 2, and 3 showed the effect to be restricted to within-subject, mixed-list designs in which some individual words are spoken aloud at study. Because the effect was not evident when the same repeated manual or vocal overt response was made to some words (Experiment 4), producing a subset of studied words appears to provide additional unique and discriminative information for those words-they become distinctive. This interpretation is supported by observing a production effect in Experiment 5, in which some words were mouthed (i.e., articulated without speaking); in Experiment 6, in which the materials were pronounceable nonwords; and even in Experiment 7, in which the already robust generation effect was incremented by production. Experiment 8 incorporated a semantic judgment and showed that the production effect was not due to "lazy reading" of the words studied silently. The distinctiveness that accrues to the records of produced items at the time of study is useful at the time of test for discriminating these produced items from other items. The production effect represents a simple but quite powerful mechanism for improving memory for selected information.


Subject(s)
Mental Recall/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Practice, Psychological , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Verbal Behavior/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Attention/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reading , Vocabulary
6.
Cognition ; 115(2): 314-9, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20185121

ABSTRACT

When young children observe pretend-play, do they interpret it simply as a type of behavior, or do they infer the underlying mental state that gives the behavior meaning? This is a long-standing question with deep implications for how "theory on mind" develops. The two leading accounts of shared pretense give opposing answers. The behavioral theory proposes that children represent pretense as a form of behavior (behaving in a way that would be appropriate if P); the metarepresentational theory argues that children instead represent pretense via the early concept PRETEND. A test between these accounts is provided by children's understanding of pretend sounds and speech. We report the first experiments directly investigating this understanding. In three experiments, 2- and 3-year-olds' listened to requests that were either spoken normally, or with the pretense that a teddy bear was uttering them. To correctly fulfill the requests, children had to represent the normal utterance as the experimenter's, and the pretend utterances as the bear's. Children succeeded at both ages, suggesting that they can represent pretend speech (the requests) as coming from counterfactual sources (the bear rather than the experimenter). We argue that this is readily explained by the metarepresentational theory, but harder to explain if children are behaviorists about pretense.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Speech Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Child, Preschool , Comprehension/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Female , Humans , Male
7.
Dev Psychol ; 45(3): 873-6, 2009 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19413438

ABSTRACT

Owners control permission-they forbid and permit others to use their property. So it is reasonable to assume that someone controlling permission over an object is its owner. The authors tested whether preschoolers infer ownership in this way. In the first experiment, 4- and 5-year-olds, but not 3-year-olds, chose as owner of an object a character who granted or denied another character permission to use it. In Experiment 2, older 3-year-olds chose as owner of an object a character who prevented another character from using it when prevention was accomplished by controlling permission but not otherwise. Younger 3-year-olds chose between the characters at chance. These findings indicate that preschoolers infer ownership from control of permission.


Subject(s)
Concept Formation , Culture , Ownership , Play and Playthings , Social Perception , Age Factors , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Social Behavior
8.
Cognition ; 107(3): 829-49, 2008 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18243169

ABSTRACT

A basic problem of daily life is determining who owns what. One way that people may solve this problem is by relying on a 'first possession' heuristic, according to which the first person who possesses an object is its owner, even if others subsequently possess the object. We investigated preschoolers' use of this heuristic in five experiments. In Experiments 1 and 2, 3- and 4-year-olds inferred that an object was owned by the character who possessed it first, even though another character subsequently possessed it. Two-year-olds also showed this bias, but only when the object was placed between the characters when children were asked about ownership. Experiment 3 ruled out the possibility that children's bias to select the first possessor results from a tendency to select the character first associated with the object. Experiment 4 showed that 3- and 4-year-olds have difficulty disregarding the first possession heuristic, even when provided with evidence that the character who first possessed an object is not its owner. But Experiment 5 found that children can disregard the heuristic in at least some situations. These five experiments suggest that the first possession heuristic guides children's ownership inferences. The findings provide the first evidence that preschoolers can infer who owns what, when not explicitly told, and when not reasoning about objects with which they are personally acquainted.


Subject(s)
Awareness , Cognition , Ownership , Social Perception , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...